Abstract
Deep anesthesia is commonly used as a model of slow-wave sleep (SWS). Ketamine–xylazine anesthesia reproduces the main features of sleep slow oscillation: slow, large-amplitude waves in field potential, which are generated by the alternation of hyperpolarized and depolarized states of cortical neurons. However, direct quantitative comparison of field potential and membrane potential fluctuations during natural sleep and anesthesia is lacking, so it remains unclear how well the properties of sleep slow oscillation are reproduced by the ketamine–xylazine anesthesia model. Here, we used field potential and intracellular recordings in different cortical areas in the cat to directly compare properties of slow oscillation during natural sleep and ketamine–xylazine anesthesia. During SWS cortical activity showed higher power in the slow/delta (0.1–4 Hz) and spindle (8–14 Hz) frequency range, whereas under anesthesia the power in the gamma band (30–100 Hz) was higher. During anesthesia, slow waves were more rhythmic and more synchronous across the cortex. Intracellular recordings revealed that silent states were longer and the amplitude of membrane potential around transition between active and silent states was bigger under anesthesia. Slow waves were mostly uniform across cortical areas under anesthesia, but in SWS, they were most pronounced in associative and visual areas but smaller and less regular in somatosensory and motor cortices. We conclude that, although the main features of the slow oscillation in sleep and anesthesia appear similar, multiple cellular and network features are differently expressed during natural SWS compared with ketamine–xylazine anesthesia.
Introduction
Slow-wave sleep (SWS) is characterized by large-amplitude slow waves in the electroencephalogram (Blake and Gerard, 1937). Their repetitive and more or less regular appearance during slow-wave sleep and under anesthesia is commonly referred to as slow oscillation, which consists of an alternation of active (Up) and silent (Down) states in the cortico-thalamic network (Steriade et al., 1993a,b,c; Cowan and Wilson, 1994). During active states, neocortical neurons are depolarized, reveal vigorous synaptic activities, and may fire action potentials, whereas during silent states, they are hyperpolarized, display few synaptic events, and do not fire spikes (Contreras and Steriade, 1995; Steriade et al., 2001; Timofeev et al., 2001; Chauvette et al., 2010). Intracellular recordings from non-anesthetized cats (Steriade et al., 2001; Timofeev et al., 2001; Rudolph et al., 2007; Chauvette et al., 2010) and rats (Mahon et al., 2006; Okun et al., 2010) demonstrated that, when local field potential (LFP) displays slow waves, cortical and striatal neurons alternate between active and silent states. In contrast, when LFP displays activated patterns, normally associated with wakefulness and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, the membrane potential of cortical neurons does not express silent states and no large-amplitude LFP waves occur (Matsumura, 1979; Steriade et al., 2001; Timofeev et al., 2001; Rudolph et al., 2007; Okun et al., 2010; Constantinople and Bruno, 2011). Similar activities were found during active but not quiet wakefulness in mice (Crochet and Petersen, 2006; Poulet and Petersen, 2008; Gentet et al., 2010). After prolonged periods of wakefulness, local slow waves can occur in otherwise awake animals (Vyazovskiy et al., 2011).
Experiments on cats and rats anesthetized with ketamine–xylazine or urethane revealed a slow cortical activity similar to that observed during natural SWS (Steriade et al., 1993a,b,c; Contreras and Steriade, 1995; Haider et al., 2006; Volgushev et al., 2006; Haider et al., 2007; Hasenstaub et al., 2007; Luczak et al., 2007; Chauvette et al., 2010; Sharma et al., 2010). Anesthesia-induced slow oscillation appears more regular than during SWS (Steriade et al., 1993b; Wolansky et al., 2006; Clement et al., 2008), and its frequency was higher with ketamine–xylazine than urethane anesthesia (Steriade et al., 1993b; Sharma et al., 2010), indicating that properties of the slow oscillation are not fixed but depend on experimental conditions. Recordings from different cortical areas show a preferential origin of slow waves at specific locations (Massimini et al., 2004; Volgushev et al., 2006; Kurth et al., 2010; Nir et al., 2011), suggesting regional specificity.
Ketamine–xylazine anesthesia is extensively used as a model of sleep slow oscillation; however, no direct comparison of the properties of slow oscillation during anesthesia and SWS had been done so far. To address this issue, we performed LFP and intracellular recordings across different cortical areas in both naturally sleeping and ketamine–xylazine-anesthetized cats. We found that, despite apparent similarities, major differences exist between SWS and anesthesia.
Materials and Methods
Experiments were performed in accordance with the guideline of the Canadian Council on Animal Care and approved by the Laval University Committee on Ethics and Animal Research. The cats were purchased from an established animal breeding supplier. Good health conditions of all animals were certified by the supplier and determined on arrival to animal house by physical examination, which was performed by animal facilities technicians and a veterinarian in accordance with requirements of the Canadian Council on Animal Care. The animals were used for experiments in 5–20 d from arrival to the local animal house.
General description and database
We recorded field potentials, extracellular unit, and intracellular activities of cortical neurons in various cortical areas of cats during natural sleep or in cats anesthetized with ketamine–xylazine. We used recordings from database acquired over the past 10 years for neurons recorded from suprasylvian gyrus (associative cortex, 36 cats in acute experiments and four cats in chronic experiments), postcruciate gyrus (somatosensory cortex, five cats in acute experiments and one cat in chronic experiments), precruciate gyrus (motor cortex, one cat in acute experiments and one cat in chronic experiments), and posterior marginal gyrus (primary visual cortex, two cats in chronic experiments). To complement our database with multisite LFP recordings and to collect additional intracellular data, we performed additional experiments in motor (one acute and one chronic), somatosensory (four chronic), and visual (three acute and one chronic) cortices. Because in the same experiment we were able to record from several cortical areas, altogether we used 43 cats for acute experiments and eight cats for chronic experiments.
Preparation
Experiments on anesthetized animals.
For acute experiments, the cats of both sexes were anesthetized with ketamine and xylazine (10–15 and 2–3 mg/kg, i.m., respectively). All pressure points and tissues to be incised were infiltrated with lidocaine (0.5%). The animals were paralyzed with gallamine triethiodide (20 mg/kg) and artificially ventilated, maintaining the end-tidal CO2 concentration at 3.5–3.8%. A permanent sleep-like state, as ascertained by continuous recording of the LFP, was maintained throughout the experiments by administering additional doses of ketamine and xylazine (5 and 1 mg/kg, respectively) or just ketamine (5 mg/kg). The additional anesthesia was administrated when LFP slow waves were absent for 20–30 s. The body temperature was monitored by a rectal probe and maintained at 37°C via a feedback-controlled heating blanket. The heart rate was continuously monitored (90–110 beats/min). The stability of intracellular recordings was reinforced by cisternal drainage, bilateral pneumothorax, and filling the hole made for recordings with a solution of 4% agar.
Experiments on non-anesthetized animals.
Chronic experiments were conducted using an approach similar to that described previously (Steriade et al., 2001; Timofeev et al., 2001). For implantation of recording chamber and electrodes, four cats (data from the old database) were anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (30 mg/kg), and four cats were anesthetized with isoflurane (0.75–2%), in accordance with the current recommendation of the Committee for Animal Care of Laval University. All other procedures were very similar. Briefly, before surgery, the animal was given a dose of pre-anesthetic, which was composed of ketamine (15 mg/kg), buprenorphine (0.01 mg/kg), and acepromazine (0.3 mg/kg). After site shaving and cat intubation for gaseous anesthesia, the site of incision was washed with at least three alternating passages of proviodine solution and alcohol. Lidocaine (0.5%) was injected at the site of incision and applied at all pressure points. During surgery, electrodes for LFP recordings, electromyogram (EMG) from neck muscle, and electro-oculogram (EOG) were implanted and fixed with acrylic dental cement. Custom-made recording chambers were fixed for future intracellular recordings. Eight to 10 screws were fixed to the cranium. Four bolts were covered in the dental cement that also covered bone-fixed screws, permanently implanted electrodes, and fixed recording chamber to allow future head-restrained recordings without any pressure point. Throughout the surgery, the body temperature was maintained at 37°C using a water-circulating thermo-regulated blanket. Hearth beat and oxygen saturation were continuously monitored using a pulse oximeter (Rad-8; MatVet), and the level of anesthesia was adjusted to maintain a heart beat at 110–120 per minute. A lactate Ringer's solution (10 ml · kg−1 · h−1, i.v.) was given during the surgery. After the surgery, cats were given buprenorphine (0.01 mg/kg) or anafen (2 mg/kg) twice a day for 3 d and baytril (5 mg/kg) once a day for 7 d. Approximately 1 week was allowed to animals to recover from the surgery before the first recording session. Usually, 2–3 d of training were sufficient for cats to remain in head-restrained position for 2–4 h and display one to three full sleep–wake cycles, including periods of quiet wakefulness, SWS, and REM sleep. The recordings were performed up to 40 d after the surgery.
Recordings
All recordings were done in a Faraday chamber. Using AM 3000 amplifiers (A-M Systems) with custom modifications, LFPs were recorded in marginal gyrus [visual cortex (areas 17 and 18)], suprasylvian gyrus [associative cortex (areas 5, 7, and 21)], ectosylvian gyrus [auditory cortex (areas 22 and 50)], postcruciate gyrus [somatosensory cortex (area 3)], precruciate gyrus [motor cortex (areas 4 and 6)], and frontal gyrus [medial prefrontal cortex (area 32)] using tungsten electrodes (2 MΩ; bandpass filter, 0.1 Hz to 10 kHz). We aimed to implant electrodes at 1 mm below the cortical surface. Intracellular recordings were performed using glass micropipettes filled with 2.5 m potassium acetate and having a resistance of 30–70 MΩ. A high-impedance amplifier with active bridge circuitry (Neurodata IR-283 amplifiers; Cygnus Technology; low-pass filter, 10 kHz) was used to record the membrane potential and to inject current into the neurons. Intracellular recordings were performed from areas 3, 4, 7, and 17 according to an atlas (Reinoso-Suarez, 1961). In acute experiments, a chloridized silver wire, placed between the skull and the temporal muscle, was used as a reference electrode. In chronic experiments, a silver wire was fixed either in the frontal bone over the sinus cavity or over the cerebellum. All electrical signals were sampled at 20 kHz and digitally stored on Vision (Nicolet). At the end of experiments, the cats were anesthetized with ketamine–xylazine to record LFP activities with the same electrodes as during natural states of vigilance. After collecting 1 h or more of recordings, the cats were killed with a lethal dose of pentobarbital (50 mg/kg, i.v.).
Data analysis
Electrographic recordings were analyzed offline using custom-written routines in IgorPro (WaveMetrics) and MATLAB (MathWorks) environment. To measure the amplitude of slow oscillation, we used intracellular recordings. The amplitude of field potential recordings depends on the activity of cellular elements in the region but also on the electrode location relative to electrical dipole(s). Because the cortical thickness is different in different areas, the amplitude of LFP at 1 mm from the surface of the pia can give erroneous estimation of slow oscillation amplitude. It is generally assumed that, during slow-wave activity, the membrane potential of neurons has a bimodal distribution (Metherate and Ashe, 1993), and this was the case in the majority of our recordings (Fig. 1a1–b1,a3–b3). However, when transitions between active and silent states were slow and their duration was comparable with the time a neuron spent in those states, the membrane potential distribution was not bimodal, although silent states could be clearly identified (Fig. 1a2–b2,a4–b4). To resolve this problem, we analyzed periods around transitions from silent to active states. We first selected time at half-amplitude of transitions from silent to active state from 1 min segment of a recording. We then selected segments of ±100 ms from that point (Fig. 1c) and compiled a histogram using all these segments (Fig. 1d, gray histograms). On all occasions, the distribution of the membrane potential was bimodal; however, because of the natural fluctuation of the membrane potential, the two maxima were broad. Therefore, from selected segments, we computed averages (Fig. 1c, black traces) and calculated the histogram of membrane potential distribution from these averaged traces (Fig. 1d, black histogram). The results gave clear bimodal distribution of membrane potential (Fig. 1). This method allowed unbiased analysis of all recorded neurons.
The frequency composition of electric signals was calculated using fast Fourier transformation of multiple 20 s segments of LFP traces. From power spectrum of each data segment, we calculated the integral power in the frequency ranges between 0.1 and 4 Hz for slow and delta activities, 8 and 14 Hz for spindles, and 30 and 100 Hz for gamma-band fluctuations. We also calculated autocorrelation of field potential and membrane potential recordings and coherence between pairs of simultaneously recorded LFPs. Coherence was calculated using the MATLAB (R2011b) function mscohere, which uses Welch's averaged modified periodogram method to calculate the magnitude squared coherence estimate. Periodic Hamming windows with 50% overlap were used to obtain eight equal-length sections of each signal. Coherence function was calculated with a step of 0.1526 Hz. Then, coherence in three frequency ranges [slow and delta (0.2–4 Hz), spindle (8–14 Hz), and gamma (30–100 Hz)] was calculated by averaging the values within the respective range. Unless specified, all numerical values are given as mean ± SD. Specific statistical tests are indicated throughout.
Results
We used two different strategies to compare properties of LFP, extracellular unit activity, and intracellular membrane potential traces during natural SWS and ketamine–xylazine anesthesia. Field potentials were recorded with chronically implanted electrodes during natural sleep–wake cycle and thereafter under ketamine–xylazine anesthesia. This allowed us to compare LFP signals pairwise, with each pair consisting of recordings made from the same electrode in the same animal during natural sleep and anesthesia. Intracellular recordings from neurons in different cortical areas were made sequentially during natural sleep or anesthesia; therefore, intracellular data were analyzed as groups.
On one occasion (Fig. 2), we were able to record LFP and extracellular unit activities of the same neuron in the somatosensory cortex (postcruciate gyrus, area 3) during waking state, SWS, and ketamine–xylazine anesthesia (Fig. 2b, insets). During waking state characterized by high-frequency low-amplitude activity in the LFP, the neuronal firing was continuous and did not reveal periodic patterns (Fig. 2). Only central peak, but no additional peaks, were present in the autocorrelogram of the firing of the neurons (Fig. 2b, left). During transition to SWS, evidenced by characteristic large-amplitude slow waves in the LFP and EEG, the neuronal firing became grouped and occurred during depth-negative phases of the LFP (Fig. 2a). Several minutes after injection of ketamine–xylazine anesthesia, the rhythmicity and the frequency of slow waves increased, and the firing rate increased and became highly rhythmic (Fig. 2). Similar to SWS, the neuronal firing occurred during depth-negative phases of the LFP (Fig. 2a). The rhythmicity of neuronal firing increased, as evidenced by multiple alternating peaks and troughs in the autocorrelogram (Fig. 2b, right).
To examine whether these changes in activity could be generalized to other cortical areas, we have studied cortical electrographic activities in cat neocortex during natural SWS and during ketamine–xylazine anesthesia in precruciate gyrus, postcruciate gyrus, frontal gyrus, marginal gyrus, suprasylvian gyrus, and ectosylvian gyrus (Fig. 3a). Figure 3b shows results of a typical experiment in which field potentials were recorded simultaneously from multiple cortical locations using chronically implanted electrodes, first during an episode of natural SWS, and then during ketamine–xylazine anesthesia. Slow oscillation is clearly present in each individual trace in this example: all recordings in Figure 3b express slow, large-amplitude waves and fulfill a formal criterion of a low (<4 Hz) over high (>4 Hz) frequencies power ratio >3 (Mukovski et al., 2007). An averaged ratio for all 11 locations was 6.11 ± 5.8 during anesthesia and 8.18 ± 6.9 (mean ± SEM) during SWS. However, the whole picture of the slow oscillation recorded simultaneously from multiple locations was clearly different during SWS compared with anesthesia. During SWS, slow waves were always more prominent in suprasylvian gyrus (Fig. 3b, locations 8–10). In other cortical areas, slow waves were of smaller amplitude and even not evident in some cycles (Fig. 3b, left). In all cortical regions, the intervals between slow waves were highly irregular. After the injection of ketamine–xylazine anesthesia, the pattern of slow oscillation changed (Fig. 3b, right). The slow waves became more regular and rhythm like. They also were more generalized across different cortical regions, so that every wave was evident in all simultaneously recorded channels.
The differences between patterns of slow waves in field potential observed during SWS and anesthesia were corroborated by results of intracellular recordings. Figure 3c–f shows examples of membrane potential traces of neurons recorded in different areas of the neocortex during SWS or anesthesia. All neurons recorded during SWS or under ketamine–xylazine anesthesia exhibited alternating active (depolarized) and silent (hyperpolarized) states. However, during anesthesia, the slow oscillation appears to be more rhythmic. Furthermore, during anesthesia, the slow oscillation was of overall higher amplitude, and the silent states were of longer duration than in SWS (Fig. 3c–f).
To validate and quantify these differences, we have compared, for SWS and anesthesia, and for different cortical regions, the following parameters: spectral composition, rhythmicity and coherence of the slow oscillation, its amplitude, and the duration of silent states.
Less power in the slow and spindle range but more in the high-frequency range during ketamine–xylazine anesthesia than in natural sleep
Although LFPs recorded in a same animal in both SWS and ketamine–xylazine anesthesia expressed clear slow activities, spectral analysis revealed substantial differences in their frequency composition. Figure 4 shows an example of LFP recorded in one experiment in somatosensory cortex (area 3) of the right hemisphere during an SWS episode and then under ketamine–xylazine anesthesia. A higher rhythmicity of slow oscillation and an increase in fast activities (see filtered traces in Fig. 4a) can be clearly seen in the recording obtained under anesthesia. Power spectra of 20 s epochs around the illustrated segments of recordings revealed marked differences in three characteristic frequency ranges (Fig. 4b). Power in the slow and delta frequency range (0.1–4 Hz) and at spindle frequencies (8–14 Hz) in suprasylvian gyrus was significantly higher during SWS (Fig. 4c). A similar pattern was found in different regions, but the difference did not reach significance level. When data from all investigated areas were pooled together, the power was significantly decreased for slow/delta (p = 0.003, two-tailed one-sample t test) and for spindle frequency (p < 0.0001) ranges, but the gamma range activities were dramatically increased under anesthesia (Fig. 4c, p < 0.0001).
Stronger rhythmicity of neuronal activity during anesthesia than in natural sleep
LFP, extracellular unit, and membrane potential of cortical neurons showed a stronger rhythmicity of slow oscillation under anesthesia (Figs. 2, 3). To quantify the rhythmicity, we computed autocorrelations of 20 s segments of LFP and intracellular recordings (40 neurons) during SWS and under anesthesia (Fig. 5). During SWS, the slow waves appeared with only weak, if at all, rhythmicity. This is evidenced by the low amplitude or the absence of secondary peaks with a period of 1–2 s in the autocorrelograms (Fig. 5, left column). Under anesthesia, autocorrelograms of both LFP and intracellular recordings from all four investigated areas expressed several clear secondary peaks. These recurrent peaks occurring at regular intervals are indicative of a strong rhythmicity of the slow oscillation (Fig. 5a,b, right columns). The amplitude of the secondary peak, usually occurring between 0.5 and 1.5 s, was significantly higher during anesthesia than in SWS for all studied areas (Fig. 5c), indicating a strong increase in the rhythmicity of the slow oscillation under ketamine–xylazine anesthesia.
Higher coherence of slow oscillation between different regions during ketamine–xylazine anesthesia than during SWS
Simultaneous LFP recordings made from multiple cortical regions during SWS and subsequent anesthesia (Fig. 3b) suggest that slow waves occur more synchronously in different cortical regions during anesthesia than in SWS. To assess this relation quantitatively, we have calculated pairwise coherence of the LFPs recorded at different locations during SWS and compared it with the coherence of LFPs recorded during anesthesia from the same pairs of chronically implanted electrodes (Fig. 6). The coherence was calculated for three frequency ranges (Fig. 6): slow and delta (0.2–4 Hz), spindle (8–14 Hz), and gamma (30–100 Hz). The coherence between different cortical regions during anesthesia was high for all investigated ranges of frequencies (Fig. 6c, green–yellow–brown plots). It was systematically higher during the anesthesia than during natural SWS for signal components in the slow and delta range (0.2–4 Hz, 0.594 ± 0.102 vs 0.507 ± 0.149, p < 0.001, n = 45 pairs of recordings from the same electrodes during anesthesia and SWS, Wilcoxon's paired test) and in the gamma frequency range (30–100 Hz, 0.43 ± 0.031 vs 0.399 ± 0.024, p < 0.003). In this example, for the spindle range, the difference did not reach significance level (0.435 ± 0.094 vs 0.425 ± 0.031, p = 0.147). In the scatter plots in which coherence during anesthesia is plotted against coherence during the SWS for each pair of recorded electrodes, most points are located above the diagonal for slow/delta and gamma frequencies (Fig. 6a,b). Higher coherence during anesthesia is particularly evident in coherence difference (anesthesia minus SWS) color (red–white–blue) plots by a strong domination of blue colors, indicating a higher coherence during anesthesia (Fig. 6c). To validate these results, we performed the same analysis on n = 165 pairs (four cats) of SWS and anesthesia recordings. In each pair of electrodes, the coherence was calculated in epochs of 15 s (10–12 epochs for each recording) and then averaged for that pair of electrodes. For the whole sample, the coherence in all three frequency ranges was significantly higher during the anesthesia than during SWS: 0.524 ± 0.109 versus 0.487 ± 0.127, p < 0.001, Wilcoxon's paired test, for slow and delta range (0.2–4 Hz); 0.442 ± 0.1 versus 0.429 ± 0.088, p = 0.008 for the spindle range (8–14 Hz); and 0.4 ± 0.051 versus 0.386 ± 0.029, p < 0.001 for the gamma range (30–100 Hz). Thus, slow waves as well as activity in the spindle and gamma frequency ranges were more synchronized during anesthesia compared with natural SWS.
Silent states are more prominent during anesthesia than in SWS
Intracellular recordings suggested that, for a given cortical area, the slow oscillation was usually of smaller amplitude and silent states of a shorter duration during SWS compared with anesthesia (examples in Fig. 3c–f). To assess these differences quantitatively, we measured the duration of silent states at half-amplitude of transition to and from silent states. Comparison of silent states in all recordings performed under anesthesia to all recordings made during SWS revealed that the duration of silent states was significantly shorter during SWS in each investigated area and for all areas pooled together (Fig. 7a1). The proportion of time spent in silent state, calculated as the ratio of time a cell spent in silent state to the total time analyzed, was also higher under anesthesia than in SWS. This difference was significant both when recordings from all cortical regions were pooled together and when data were segregated by cortical area (Fig. 7b1).
The amplitude of slow oscillation recorded intracellularly was calculated as described in Materials and Methods (Fig. 1). For the whole sample, when recordings made from different cortical areas were pooled together, the amplitude of slow oscillation was significantly larger under ketamine–xylazine anesthesia than in natural SWS (Fig. 7c1). When the comparison was made separately for recordings obtained in each area, the amplitude of silent states during anesthesia was significantly higher than in SWS in area 3 but not in other areas (Fig. 7c1).
Next, we compared the duration and amplitude of silent states in membrane potential of neurons recorded during slow oscillation between different areas in the same experimental condition of either SWS or anesthesia. This comparison revealed area-specific differences in silent-state duration in both SWS and anesthesia (Fig. 7a2,b2). A striking difference between SWS and anesthesia was observed for the amplitude of slow oscillation, which showed area-specific differences during SWS that were not present under anesthesia (Fig. 7c2).
Discussion
In this study, we demonstrated that, despite a generally similar appearance of slow oscillation during deep SWS and anesthesia, their quantitative comparison revealed multiple features that are distinct in these two brain states. (1) During anesthesia, the rhythmicity of slow oscillation in the LFP and intracellular recordings was higher than in SWS. (2) During anesthesia, the LFP power in slow and delta (0.1–4 Hz) and in spindle (8–14 Hz) frequency ranges was decreased, but the power of gamma activity was doubled. (3) During anesthesia, the coherence of LFP in different areas and for all investigated frequency ranges was higher than during SWS. (4) Intracellular recordings revealed area-specific differences of slow oscillation amplitude during SWS with larger amplitudes in visual and associative areas and smaller amplitudes in somatosensory area. Anesthesia increased the amplitude of slow oscillation in most areas and abolished differences between areas. (5) The duration of silent states was also area-specific, both during SWS and under anesthesia; however, in every area, anesthesia increased the duration of silent states to 148–240% compared with its value during SWS.
Effects of anesthesia on cortical neurons
Slow oscillation is composed of two different phases: an active phase when cortical neurons are depolarized and may fire action potentials, and a silent phase when cortical neurons are hyperpolarized and silent (Steriade et al., 1993b). This general pattern of slow oscillation was seen in both SWS and ketamine–xylazine anesthesia. Silent state is a period of disfacilitation (absence of synaptic activity) during which the membrane potential of cortical neurons is primarily dominated by potassium leak conductance (Wilson et al., 1983; Metherate and Ashe, 1993; Contreras et al., 1996; Timofeev et al., 1996, 2001; Wilson and Kawaguchi, 1996). Active states are associated with high levels of excitatory and inhibitory conductances of both synaptic and intrinsic origin (Wilson and Kawaguchi, 1996). Excitation and inhibition can be approximately balanced (Haider et al., 2006) or dominated by inhibition (Rudolph et al., 2007) depending on experimental condition. Ketamine is a noncompetitive, use- and voltage-dependent antagonist of NMDA receptors, leading to a partial blockade of glutamatergic neurotransmission (MacDonald et al., 1991; Liu et al., 2006). Therefore, the use of ketamine should decrease excitatory conductance. Indeed, in all investigated cortical regions, we observed under ketamine–xylazine anesthesia an increase of both the duration of silent states and the proportion of time spent in silent states (Fig. 7a,b). This implies a decrease in the duration of active states. This observation is congruent to a recent finding demonstrating that a hypofunction of NMDA receptor attributable to genetic modulation or antagonist application increases the length of silent states (Fellin et al., 2009). A recent study demonstrated that a decrease in inhibition had similar effects: it shortened active states and prolonged silent states (Sanchez-Vives et al., 2010). This suggests that a fine tuning of excitation and inhibition in each cortical area is responsible for the generation of a particular frequency of slow oscillation and that manipulations leading to a decrease of the efficiency of either excitation or inhibition will shorten active states and lengthen silent states. Several additional effects of ketamine on cortical neurons had been reported. Ketamine application hyperpolarizes cortical neurons via a block of HCN1 channels, enhances synaptic responses (Chen et al., 2009), and enhances Ca2+ spikes and dendritic excitability in pyramidal cells in vitro, but not in vivo because synaptic activity reduces dendritic spiking in cortical neurons (Potez and Larkum, 2008). Ketamine also inhibits nicotinic cholinoreceptors (Rudolph and Antkowiak, 2004); however, because the firing of cholinergic neurons is strongly reduced during both SWS and anesthesia (Steriade and McCarley, 2005; Boucetta and Jones, 2009), it is unlikely that ketamine induces slow oscillations via this mechanism. Ketamine may also potentiate GABA responses (Rudolph and Antkowiak, 2004; Alkire et al., 2008), but, at anesthetic doses, it acts exclusively on α6β2δ and α6β3δ receptors (Hevers et al., 2008), which, in mammalian brain, were found only in the granular layer of cerebellum (Pirker et al., 2000). GABAergic action of ketamine unlikely contributes to the generation of active and silent states in the thalamocortical system.
We observed a significant increase in the amplitude of intracellularly recorded slow oscillation (Fig. 7c) that was likely attributable to a more pronounced hyperpolarization during silent states induced by ketamine–xylazine anesthesia. In all recorded areas, we observed a more pronounced hyperpolarization during silent states in anesthetized cats compared with SWS. However, in chronic experiments, the reference electrode remained implanted for weeks or months and thus could develop some direct current polarization. Therefore, we were not confident in comparing absolute values of the membrane potential, but we compared the difference between active and silent states (Fig. 7). Xylazine is an agonist for α2 adrenergic receptors, which are abundant in the neocortex (Hedler et al., 1981; Nicholas et al., 1993). The α2 adrenoreceptor agonist clonidine activates a potassium current in rat spinal cord neurons (Sonohata et al., 2004). Noradrenaline also increases potassium conductance in mouse entorhinal cortex (Pralong and Magistretti, 1995). In the locus ceruleus, noradrenaline and α2 adrenoreceptor agonists activate potassium current, whereas α2 antagonist blocks them (Arima et al., 1998). Activation of α2 adrenoreceptor increases cortical activity via closure of h-channels (Wang et al., 2007). Therefore, the action of xylazine could contribute to both the longer duration and the bigger difference in amplitude between active and silent states observed in our experiments in anesthetized animals.
Anesthesia, sleep, consciousness
Higher brain functions occurring during conscious states require coordinated spatiotemporal firing in large neuronal constellations with hierarchal organization (Konorski, 1967; Buzsáki, 2010) involving internally and externally generated signals (Edelman, 2003). A minimal time of 0.5–1 s is required to be conscious (Libet et al., 1967; Edelman, 2003). Continuous neuronal firing of cortical, thalamic, and striatal neurons occurs during persistent active (depolarized) network states in awake animals (Steriade et al., 2001; Timofeev et al., 2001; Mahon et al., 2006; Vyazovskiy et al., 2009) as well as during REM sleep (Hirsch et al., 1983; Steriade et al., 2001; Timofeev et al., 2001). This fulfills the requirement for uninterrupted coordinated activity; therefore, some forms of consciousness could occur during both waking state and REM sleep. On the contrary, during SWS, cortical, thalamocortical, and striatal neurons oscillate between active and silent states (Hirsch et al., 1983; Steriade et al., 2001; Timofeev et al., 2001; Mahon et al., 2006). Active states are depolarized states in cortical and striatal neurons, but in thalamocortical neurons, they are associated with multiple IPSPs originating from burst firing of reticular thalamic neurons (Contreras and Steriade, 1995; Timofeev and Steriade, 1996). During SWS, dominated by slow oscillation with a period of ∼1 s, the duration of silent states is ∼150–200 ms, and neurons spend 5–10% of time in silent states (Fig. 7). Therefore, in deep SWS, the continuous neuronal firing lasts <1 s, thus impairing the emergence of conscious experience. Slow-wave activity is highest and more global at the onset of sleep period and then progressively decreases toward the end of sleep (Achermann and Borbély, 2003; Vyazovskiy et al., 2009; Nir et al., 2011). It is well accepted that the expression of slow oscillation in cortical areas is correlated with activities in those areas during wake (Huber et al., 2004). Like in humans, most of sensory information in cat arrives via the visual system. Therefore, we expected, and it was indeed the case, to obtain larger amplitude of slow oscillation in area 17 (Fig. 7). The neurons in area 7 also showed larger amplitude of slow oscillation, because this is a poly-sensory area with visual dominance (Bignall, 1967). It is likely that, in rodent cortex, the biggest amplitude of slow waves would be recorded in somatosensory areas, because this is the leading sensory system in these animals. During slow waves, neurons in large cortical territories reveal almost simultaneous alternations of activity and silence (Volgushev et al., 2006). Because of the decreased slow-wave activity toward the end of the sleep period, the duration of active states, estimated from extracellular unit recordings, increases and often lasts longer than 1 s (Vyazovskiy et al., 2009). Therefore, some form of conscious processes are possible during late SWS, and the majority of dreams reported outside REM episodes was found during these late periods of sleep, when cortical neurons display longer uninterrupted firing stretches (Payne and Nadel, 2004).
Recent reviews discuss whether general anesthesia-induced amnesia, rather than loss of consciousness, plays a more important role in the overall effects of anesthesia (Rudolph and Antkowiak, 2004; Alkire et al., 2008; Tononi and Koch, 2008). All agreed that deep SWS might be considered as an unconscious state. When ketamine–xylazine anesthesia is used, the duration of silent states approximately doubles, and the cortical network spends almost 20% of time in silent states (Fig. 7), preventing an essential condition of conscious states, i.e., persistent neuronal firing. The synchrony of slow waves under ketamine–xylazine anesthesia is higher than during SWS (Figs. 3, 6). Therefore, the active states occur more synchronously across cortical areas, and virtually all cortical neurons become silent simultaneously, making logical processing of information and conscious states during ketamine–xylazine anesthesia impossible. Overall, we suggest that the enhanced presence of slow waves under anesthesia compared with SWS may be used as a safe criterion to estimate annihilation of conscious state.
Footnotes
This study was supported by Canadian Institutes of Health Research Grants MOP-37862 and MOP-67175, National Science and Engineering Research Council of Canada Grant 298475, and National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grants 1R01-NS060870 and 1R01-NS059740 (I.T.). M.V. received startup funds from University of Connecticut. S.Ch. is a Canadian Institutes of Health Research fellow. S.Cr. is funded by Agence Nationale de la Recherche, France. I.T. is a Fonds de la Recherche en Santé du Québec Research Scholar. We are grateful to Sergiu Ftomov for excellent technical support, Josée Seigneur for logistic support, and Monty Escabi and Stanislav Volgushev for advice on data processing.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Igor Timofeev, The Centre de Recherche Université Laval Robert-Giffard Local F-6500, 2601 de la Canardière, Québec, QC, Canada, G1J 2G3. igor.timofeev{at}phs.ulaval.ca