Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Articles, Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

Geometric Cues Influence Head Direction Cells Only Weakly in Nondisoriented Rats

Rebecca Knight, Robin Hayman, Lin Lin Ginzberg and Kathryn Jeffery
Journal of Neuroscience 2 November 2011, 31 (44) 15681-15692; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2257-11.2011
Rebecca Knight
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Robin Hayman
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Lin Lin Ginzberg
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Kathryn Jeffery
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

The influential hypothesis that environmental geometry is critical for spatial orientation has been extensively tested behaviorally, and yet findings have been conflicting. Head direction (HD) cells, the neural correlate of the sense of direction, offer a window into the processes underlying directional orientation and may help clarify the issue. In the present study, HD cells were recorded as rats foraged in enclosures of varying geometry, with or without simultaneous manipulation of landmarks and self-motion cues (path integration). All geometric enclosures had single-order rotational symmetry and thus completely polarized the environment. They also had unique features, such as corners, which could, in principle, act as landmarks. Despite these strongly polarizing geometric cues, HD cells in nondisoriented rats never rotated with these shapes. In contrast, when a cue card (white or gray) was added to one wall, HD cells readily rotated with the enclosure. When path integration was disrupted by disorienting the rat, HD cells rotated with the enclosure even without the landmark. Collectively, these findings indicate that geometry exerts little or no influence on heading computations in nondisoriented rats, but it can do so in disoriented rats. We suggest that geometric processing is only a weak influence, providing a backup system for heading calculations and recruited only under conditions of disorientation.

Introduction

One of the most influential hypotheses concerning spatial orientation in animals is the geometric module hypothesis of Cheng (1986), which proposes that environmental geometry overrides features in orienting animals. This hypothesis has inspired much behavioral testing in many different species, but results have been mixed (for review, see Cheng and Newcombe, 2005), and the contribution of geometric processing to orientation remains incompletely understood (Cheng, 2008).

A possible complicating factor in behavioral studies is that determining an animal's heading calculation relies on measurement of a behavioral response such as goal localization, which is the end product of numerous processes over and above orientation per se, such as memory for the goal, use of environmental features as beacons rather than orienting cues, and navigational decision making. One way around this difficulty is to supplement behavioral observation with observation of head direction (HD) cells (Taube, 1995), which are neurons that encode the animals' directional heading in the horizontal plane and probably underlie the animal's sense of direction (Taube et al., 1996). Because the HD signal stands as proxy for the animal's directional sense, it provides a useful means of assessing the animal's calculated heading in the absence of behavioral confounds. We therefore used this signal to explore the use of geometric cues in heading determination.

We used three distinctly polarized geometric environments—a trapezoid, an isosceles triangle and a teardrop (Table 1)—which varied in their geometric complexity and ambiguity and which were each situated within the center of a circular curtained arena. We show here that, surprisingly, despite the strong polarization of the environments, the HD cells in nondisoriented animals did not use the environments for orientation but instead remained aligned with the outside room. To rule out the possibility that room cues were unusually strong in our environment, we created fourth and fifth conditions by attaching either a gray or white cue card to the shortest wall of the triangle. As is typical (Taube et al., 1990b), HD cells rotated readily with the white, highly salient cue card and partially with the gray, less salient cue card. Finally, because behavioral experiments have typically used disoriented animals (Cheng, 1986; Wall et al., 2004; Pecchia and Vallortigara, 2010), and because orientation has also been shown to modulate the learning of geometric cues (Batty et al., 2009), we added a sixth condition where rats were disoriented between exposures to the triangle. Now HD cells oriented their firing using environment geometry.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

A summary of the six conditions, along with the type of geometric cues available in each condition (not drawn to scale), the manipulation to path integration, and whether or not visual landmark information was available

Our results thus suggest that the influence of geometry on HD cells, which has been reported before (Clark et al., 2010), is evident only in highly disoriented rats. Under normal (nondisoriented) circumstances, it seems that environment geometry has little or no influence on heading computations in navigation.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Sixteen adult male Lister Hooded rats (weighing between 273 and 465 g at the time of surgery) were housed individually [11:11 h light:dark, with 1 h (×2) simulated dawn/dusk] on a food-restricted diet sufficient to maintain 90% of free-feeding weight, with ad libitum access to water. All procedures were licensed by the UK Home Office subject to the restrictions and provisions contained in the Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986.

Apparatus

The experiment was performed in a circular arena 250 cm diameter, surrounded by thick black curtains. The arena was lit from directly above (∼250 lux of light—approximately the same light level as an indoor office). Adjacent to the light was a video camera to record the location and orientation of the rat, and a small radio generating white noise to overshadow external uncontrolled auditory cues. Recordings in this arena took place inside one of three geometric enclosures, which were constructed from hardboard, painted black, and had no floor or ceiling (Fig. 1A). The enclosures comprised a trapezoid, an isosceles triangle, and a teardrop, chosen for the reasons outlined below.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

A, The dimensions of the environments used for each condition, along with photos of the A3 cue cards attached to the shortest wall of the isosceles triangle. All environments were constructed using hardboard and painted matte black. The height of all the environments measured 60 cm. The teardrop shape environment was constructed from a flexible piece of hardboard inserted inside the isosceles triangle. B, An example of a protocol used in one session in the triangle condition. The outline of a triangular environment indicates how the environments were rotated between trials inside the curtained environment. Each session consisted of two 180° and two 90° [clockwise (CW) and counterclockwise (CCW)] rotations. To achieve this, 90° and 180° rotations had to be interleaved, which meant that four sequences of rotations were possible. These four sequences were pseudorandomized.

Trapezoid.

The trapezoid was regular (mirror-symmetric). The parallel walls were 60 (short wall) and 120 (long wall) cm long; the angled walls were both 70 cm long. All walls were 60 cm high. This enclosure had two pairs of congruent angles of 65° and 115°, respectively, so geometric information emanating from the corners would be ambiguous. Thus, any ability of the directional system to use geometry to orient unambiguously would require either an ability to discriminate wall length (since there was a unique shortest wall and a unique longest wall) or an ability to integrate information from more than one corner, or from a corner and a wall, simultaneously.

Isosceles triangle.

The triangle enclosure was designed as a response to the finding that HD cells did not rotate with the trapezoid. The triangle had two equal long walls of 122 cm and a single short wall of 60 cm, making a single unique angle of 30° and two congruent angles of 75° flanking the base of the triangle. The height of the walls was 60 cm. We considered that the increased aspect ratio of the triangle, together with the presence of one very obvious, narrow corner, would enhance the likelihood that HD cells would use this feature of the geometry to orient, if orientation by geometric cues occurs at all.

Teardrop.

The teardrop was introduced following the surprising finding that HD cells did not rotate with the isosceles triangle either. We hypothesized that perhaps the cells were unable to use wall length as a cue, and that the three angles of the triangle might not have been sufficiently discriminable to aid orientation. Thus, the teardrop possessed only a single corner with an angle similar to that in the triangle (30°). If HD cells can use corners as a geometric cue, then the teardrop should provide unambiguous polarizing information to them.

Triangle plus gray or white cue card.

The failure of HD cells to rotate their firing with the above enclosures might have been due to their small size—perhaps HD cells cannot use information of such close proximity. Alternatively, perhaps enclosure size is irrelevant but the cells prefer visual information, which was impoverished in the geometric enclosures due to the low contrast provided by corners etc. In these manipulations, therefore, a cue card of high contrast (white) or low contrast (gray) was introduced to the isosceles triangle. As we will show, cells rotated readily with the white cue card and somewhat with the gray card.

For each session, one of these enclosures was placed in the center of the arena, directly onto two black plastic sheets that could be wiped and rotated between trials to scramble olfactory cues. The orientation of the arena on the first trial of each day was pseudorandomized so that the orientation was never the same for consecutive days. To maximize the chance of the rats relying on the geometric properties of the shape for orientation, no polarizing extramaze cues were placed anywhere in the arena. For cue card trials, either a white or a gray A3 card was taped to the inside of the isosceles triangle's shortest wall (Fig. 1A).

Surgery and electrodes

All rats were implanted at the start of the experiment with moveable microelectrodes. Four tetrodes were constructed from four interwound, 25-μm-diameter platinum–iridium wire (California Fine Wire). The tetrodes were held in a microdrive assembly (Axona) that allowed them to be lowered or raised, with one full turn of the screw equal to an increment of 200 μm dorsoventrally. During surgery, the animals were anesthetized with isoflurane and oxygen (3 l/min). Animals were placed on a stereotaxic frame, with lambda and bregma in the horizontal plane. Microdrives were fixed to the skull with six 1.6 mm jewelers' screws (Precision Technology Supplies) and dental cement. One of the screws was soldered to a ground wire to enable the animal to be electronically grounded. The electrodes were lowered into the brain in areas known to contain HD cells: the postsubiculum (PoS), the anterodorsal thalamic nucleus (ADN), the retrosplenial cortex (RSP), or the lateral mammillary nuclei (LMN). Four HD cells were also recorded from two animals that were implanted for a separate experiment using coordinates for the CA1 region of the hippocampus, which has been shown to contain HD cells (Leutgeb et al., 2000). Once the electrodes were implanted, a metallic sleeve was pulled down over the remaining exposed wires. Postsurgery, the animals were monitored periodically until they awoke. All animals were given at least 1 week to recover following the surgery.

Screening procedures

Recording commenced 1 week after surgery. Recording was done using multichannel recording equipment (DacqUSB, Axona). The rats were connected to the recording device via lightweight wires and a socket attached to the microdrive plug. The potentials recorded on each of the 16 electrodes of the four tetrodes were passed through AC-coupled unity gain operational amplifiers mounted on the rat's head and fed to the recording system. The signal was amplified (∼20,000×), bandpass filtered (300 Hz–7 kHz), collected, and stored on a computer. Each of the four wires of one tetrode was recorded differentially with respect to the wires of other tetrodes. Screening for HD cells took place in a room separate from the actual experimental room to minimize the learning of extraneous cues in the recording environment by the rats. During screening, rats were encouraged to completely sample the floor surface by foraging for Coco Pops (Kellogg's). A directional head-stage with two LED arrays, one large and one small, aligned opposite each other at a separation of 10 cm, was used along with a video camera attached to the ceiling to track the rat's location and head direction. If no HD cell activity was present, the electrodes were lowered by ∼50 μm.

Recording procedures

When an HD cell was found, the rat was taken into the experimental room in an opaque holding box and placed on a raised platform box outside of the curtained enclosure to acclimatize to the room for 5 min. The rat was connected to the recording device while outside of the curtained enclosure, then returned to the holding box, which was carried into the enclosure through one of three joins in the curtains and placed in a pseudorandom location on the floor within the curtained area. After 1 min, the rat was taken out of the holding box, carried ∼1 m, and placed in the center of the geometric enclosure, using a different point of entry for each trial. The lightweight wires were attached by clips to two pieces of elastic string suspended from the ceiling and the recording session, which comprised five individual trials, commenced. Food was thrown into the environment to encourage rats to sample the whole space. During this time, the empty holding box was moved to a new location in the curtained enclosure to prevent it from becoming a stable directional cue. Each trial lasted for 240 s, after which the wires were unclipped from the string and the rat was removed from the environment. The rat was placed back in the holding box, which now occupied a new location in the curtained enclosure. The rat remained in the holding box while the environments were rotated.

While the rat was in the holding box, the two floor cards were flipped, shuffled, and cleaned with water. The geometric enclosure was also flipped upside down and rotated by either 90° (clockwise or counterclockwise) or 180°. Rotations were pseudorandom, so that each recording session contained one 90° clockwise rotation, one 90° counterclockwise rotation, and two 180° rotations. Thus, the shape was aligned with each of the compass axes once (north, south, east, west), except for the last trial, when the shape's orientation was the same as that in the first trial (Fig. 1B).

Before the rat was placed back in the newly rotated environment, it either did or did not undergo a disorientation treatment, which disrupts path integration. For five of the six conditions, the rat was not disoriented, because previous findings have shown that this can prevent a rat from forming a stable relationship between its internal sense of direction and the environment (Knierim et al., 1995). However, in the remaining condition, the holding box was manually spun by the experimenter (approximately one rotation per second) while they walked around the curtained enclosure for ∼1 min.

Then, for all six condition types, the rat was taken out of the holding box and placed back in the center of the shape for a further 240 s trial. The protocol was repeated until the rat had been recorded in the five trials, at which point the session was complete and the rat was taken out of the curtained enclosure in the holding box, disconnected from the recording cable, and returned to the home cage. The presentation order for each condition was pseudorandomized, ensuring that each of the six conditions was the first condition for at least one rat and the last condition for at least one rat (Table 2).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

The presentation order of conditions for each rat

Data analysis

Cluster-cutting software (Tint; Axona) was used to analyze the data off line. The peak-to-peak amplitude for one electrode was plotted against each of the other electrodes in the tetrode, creating clusters of spikes that allowed cells to be isolated by hand. All cells with a peak firing rate of <1.0 Hz across the session were excluded from further analysis. For the remaining data, polar plots (firing rate as a funciton of head direction) were created for each cell's directional activity using a smoothing kernel of 5°. To calculate each cell's mean firing direction, peak firing rate, and directional firing range for each trial, the animal's head direction was divided into 60 6° bins and smoothed in Matlab (Mathworks) using a smoothing kernel of 5°. To calculate the directional firing range, a triangle was fitted to a firing rate versus HD plot. Using Taube et al.'s (1990a) criteria, left and right maximum and left and right minimum data points were selected to form a triangular shape. A linear set of values was then extrapolated for the right and left legs. The base of the triangle (where the two legs intersected the x-axis) was then taken as the directional firing range. To assess the degree to which the data deviated from the circular mean, mean vector lengths were calculated on unsmoothed data. Mean vector lengths (r) measured the circular spread of data with values ranging from 0 to 1. Mean vector length r values that are closer to 1 indicate less variance in the dataset. HD cells that had an r value of <0.3 within a given session were excluded (van der Meer et al., 2010). Figure 2A shows an example of one HD cell from the PoS that had an r value of 0.66 pre-180° rotation. Figure 2, B and C, shows an example of one HD cell from the ADN with an r value of 0.71 and from an RSP cell with an r value of 0.88, respectively.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

A–C, Examples of single-cell firing profiles from the PoS (A), ADN (B), and RSP (C). A, PoS cell during a 180° rotation in a trapezoid trial. The plot shows the tuning curve of an HD cell prerotation (solid line) and postrotation (dotted line). The HD cell for the prerotation session had an r value of 0.66. For each rotation, the mean shift was calculated by taking the mean of the prerotation tuning curve and subtracting it from the mean of the postrotation tuning curve. For this example, the mean shift was 22.16°. B, ADN cell during a 90° rotation in a teardrop trial, where the cell had a prerotation r value of 0.71. C, RSP cell during a 90° rotation in a teardrop trial, where the cell had a prerotation r value of 0.88. Statistics showed that HD cells in the ADN had significantly higher peak firing rates compared with PoS HD cells. There were no significant differences in directional firing ranges and r values of HD cells in the PoS, ADN, and RSP.

Because HD cells from a given animal always behave coherently (Taube et al., 1990b) and cannot be treated as independent data, from this point onwards in the analysis any subsequent calculations were performed using the circular mean values of cells that had been recorded simultaneously, i.e., as an ensemble (ranging from 1 to 7 cells). All analysis was done using the CircStat Matlab toolbox (Berens, 2009). To analyze the rotation of HD cells from one trial to the next, the circular mean direction of cells in the first trial was subtracted from the circular mean direction of cells in the second trial (Fig. 2) to provide a measure of how far the ensemble had rotated. This was repeated for each subsequent trial of the session so that each session produced four rotation estimates. These mean rotations were then subtracted from the predicted angle of shift, based on how much the environment had rotated, to produce absolute deviations from expected rotation. The mean vector length of absolute deviations for each condition was calculated. The Rayleigh test (Berens, 2009) was then used to determine whether these absolute mean deviations clustered around a particular direction. Circular one- and two-sample t tests (Berens, 2009) were used to compare absolute mean deviation values against zero (the predicted deviation given perfect enclosure-following) and to compare absolute mean deviation values between conditions, respectively. A mean absolute deviation, which was not significantly different from zero, indicates that the HD cells followed the rotation of the shaped enclosure.

Histological analysis

At the end of testing, the rats were deeply anesthetized with isoflurane before they were injected with sodium pentobarbital. They were then transcardially perfused using saline followed by paraformaldehyde (4%). The brains were removed and stored in paraformaldehyde (4%) for at least 1 week before sectioning. The brains were sliced at 40 μm on a freezing microtome. The sections were then mounted and stained with cresyl violet (Fig. 3).

Results

The environments used in the recordings are shown in Figure 1A, and a schematic protocol of one recording session (a sequence of five trials) is shown in Figure 1B. A total of 56 individual HD cells were recorded from 16 rats during 47 sessions. Simultaneously recorded cells were grouped together as single ensembles, ranging from one to seven cells, of which there were 22 in total. Below, we first present descriptive data from the individual neurons, and then present the data from the ensembles, which we take as proxy for the rat's internal direction sense.

Fifty-six cells met the inclusion criteria, with a peak firing rate of >1.0 Hz and a mean vector length (r) of >0.3 (van der Meer et al., 2010). Of these 56 cells, histological analysis (Fig. 3) showed that 24 were recorded in the PoS, 16 in the ADN, 12 in the RSP, and one in the LMN (Table 2). The three remaining cells were recorded from rats implanted using CA1 coordinates [where HD cells are sometimes found (Leutgeb et al., 2000)], although this could not be confirmed with histology. Overall, these 56 HD cells had an average peak firing rate of 21.6 Hz and an average directional firing range of 109°. Circular statistical analysis determined the mean vector length, or r value, which is a measure of the spread of the data ranging from 1 (perfectly concentrated) to 0 (randomly distributed around all directions). The mean r value for the complete HD cell dataset was 0.5.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

A–D, Histology from postsubiculum (Post; A), ADN (B), retrosplenial cortex (dysgranular, RSD; granular a, RSGa; granular b, RSGb; granular c, RSGc; C), and lateral mammillary nucleus (LM; D). An example slice is shown for each brain region, along with the corresponding red dot on the template (Paxinos and Watson, 2004). Each black dot represents the recording site for the remaining rats implanted in that region. The histology for the two rats implanted in the CA1 region of the hippocampus could not be recovered. They were, however, implanted using the following coordinates: anterior/posterior: −3.5; medial/lateral: 2.0; dorsal/ventral: −1.5. Place cells were also found in this region. R371, R333, R1704, and R1693 are the identifiers of the rats whose data is shown here.

Comparisons were made between these brain areas to determine whether there were significant differences in peak firing rate, directional firing range, and r values of the cells. As only one cell was recorded from the LMN and the three cells implanted with CA1 coordinates could not be confirmed, these areas were excluded from comparisons. Of the three parameters, peak firing rate (F(2,49) = 6.04, p < 0.05) showed overall significant differences. Post hoc tests (with Bonferroni corrections) at an alpha level of 0.05 found that cells in the ADN had significantly higher peak firing rates compared with the PoS (upper CI = 39.87, lower CI = 5.76). Figure 2 shows prerotation and postrotation tuning curves of one cell recorded in the ADN, one cell in the PoS, and one cell in the RSP.

During each session, rats were exposed to one of the environments over five trials. Between trials, the environments were rotated by 90° clockwise, 90° counterclockwise, or 180°, during which time we took care not to disorient the rats (except for condition 6) (Knierim et al., 1995). There were no overall significant differences in peak firing rate for pre- and post-90° rotations (21.6 and 22.02 Hz, respectively; t(1,204) = −0.468, NS) or pre- and post-180° rotations (21.77 and 22.12 Hz, respectively; t(1,204) = −0.563, NS). There were also no overall significant differences in directional firing range for pre- and post-90° rotations (108.6° and 108.5°, respectively; t(1,204) = 0.059, NS) or pre- and post-180° rotations (109.1° and 108.7°, respectively; t(1,204) = 0.144, NS).

Henceforth, the analyses detailed below consider the data at ensemble level rather than single-cell level. No differences in ensemble behavior between clockwise and counterclockwise rotations were noted. Thus, in the analyses to follow, rotations of 90° clockwise and 90° counterclockwise were equated by a normalization process in which the counterclockwise data were reflected in the 0–180° axis to map them onto the clockwise data.

Condition 1: Trapezoid

An example of a session in the trapezoid is shown in Figure 4A. Note that the firing direction of the cell did not follow the enclosure except on the first trial; thereafter, it resumed and maintained a stable orientation with respect to the room. A stable orientation could be due to a stable path integration cue or uncontrolled extramaze cues. Indeed, we cannot entirely rule out the possibility that rats were using extramaze cues during some trials, or more likely a combination of both extramaze cues and path integration. Efforts were made to minimize the influence of any extramaze cues in all conditions. For example, white noise was played and a black curtain surrounded the environment. However, there may have been odor or auditory cues evident to the rat but not the experimenter.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

A–F, An example of the results produced during one trapezoid session (A), one triangle session (B), one teardrop session (C), one white cue card session (D), one gray cue card session (E), and one disorientation session (F). The polar plots indicate the firing direction of one cell across the five trials. The corresponding paths that the rat took for each trial are shown along with a superimposed red line to illustrate how the rotation of the HD cell corresponded to the rotation of the environment. CW, Clockwise; CCW, counterclockwise.

A total of 16 HD cells, comprising five ensembles, were recorded in the trapezoid from five rats over eight sessions. The data from the collection of sessions are shown in Figure 5A. The subsequent statistical analyses looked at how much the cells' firing directions remained locked onto the room cues versus rotated with the enclosure (the deviation-from-expected measure) and whether the resulting distribution was randomly scattered or clustered around a particular direction. We present the data from the 90° rotations first, followed by the 180° rotations.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

A–F, Circular plots showing the mean shift of HD cells in each condition for 90° rotations (left plots) or 180° rotations (right plots). Ninety degree rotations were normalized for direction by reflecting the data from clockwise rotations onto counterclockwise rotations. The degree to which the environments were shifted (either 90° or 180°) is indicated on each plot. Each dot corresponds to the mean rotation for an ensemble of cells (one to seven cells per ensemble) during one session. For all plots, the black dots represent cells from rats that only experienced that condition once. Colored dots represent cells from rats that experienced that condition more than once; different colors represent different rats. Red lines show the mean shift for each condition. The length of each red line represents the extent to which the data points are clustered. A longer line has an r value nearer to 1, which indicates a greater clustering of data points. Note that in the 90° rotations for the gray cue card condition (E), cells tended to follow the environment during one rotation but not both rotations in the session. As the data points represent one session, this bimodal distribution is averaged out (∼40°).

Overall, for all ensembles during a 90° trapezoid rotation, the mean shift in firing direction with respect to the room beyond the curtains was −1°, with a resultant vector length of 0.89 (Fig. 5A). A Rayleigh test showed that these data significantly clustered around the −1° shift (r = 7.16, p < 0.05) and were not significantly different from zero (upper CI = 25.78, lower CI = −28.07), indicating a strong propensity for the cells to retain their orientation with respect to the outside room. Correspondingly, we found a final deviation-from-expected value of 91° (±26.33 SD), which was significantly different from zero (upper CI = 118.0, lower CI = 64.2). Together, these results indicate that the rotations of the firing directions of the HD cells were significantly different from rotations of the trapezoid environment and were not different from the direction predicted by the room.

For all ensembles during a 180° trapezoid rotation, the mean shift in firing direction was −14°, with a resultant vector length of 0.9 (Fig. 5A). A Rayleigh test showed that these data significantly clustered around the −14° value (r = 7.21, p < 0.01) and were not significantly different from zero (upper CI = 8.59, lower CI = −36.1), indicating a strong propensity for the cells to remain oriented to the room. Correspondingly, we found a final deviation-from-expected value of −166° (±25.45 SD), which was significantly different from zero (upper CI = −143.8, lower CI = −188.5).

Thus, the trapezoid, despite its polarized geometry, failed to gain control of HD cells, which instead appeared to lock onto uncontrolled cues. The next manipulation aimed to test this by introducing an environment (an isosceles triangle) that was even more polarized, with a larger difference between the short wall and long walls, and a unique acute angle.

Condition 2: Triangle

A total of 15 HD cells (eight ensembles) were recorded in the triangle from seven rats over 12 sessions. An example of a session in the triangle is shown in Figure 4B. In this example, which was representative, the firing direction of the cell did not rotate with the enclosure.

Overall, for all ensembles during a 90° triangle rotation, the mean shift in firing direction was −13°, with a resultant vector length of 0.52 (Fig. 5B). A Rayleigh test showed that these data significantly clustered around the 13° shift (r = 6.78, p < 0.05) and were not significantly different from zero (upper CI = 57.87, lower CI = −32.66), indicating, as with the trapezoid, a strong propensity for the cells to remain oriented to the room. Correspondingly, we found a final deviation-from-expected value of 77° (±56.0 SD), which was significantly different from zero (upper CI = 122.61, lower CI = 31.51). Together, these results indicate that the rotations of the firing directions of the HD cells were significantly different from rotations of the triangle environment and were not different from the direction predicted by the room.

Similarly, for all ensembles during a 180° triangle rotation, the mean shift in firing direction was 2°, with a resultant vector length of 0.75 (Fig. 5B). A Rayleigh test showed that these data significantly clustered around the 2° value (r = 9.72, p < 0.01) and were not significantly different from zero (upper CI = 29.22, lower CI = −25.21), indicating a strong propensity for the cells to remain oriented to the room. Correspondingly, we found a final deviation-from-expected value of 178° (±40.69 SD), which was significantly different from zero (upper CI = 205.12, lower CI = 151.26).

One possible reason for the failure of cells to lock onto the geometry of the triangle is that from the perspective of the rat, the corners look similar and the wall lengths are hard to discriminate. This seems surprising, but nevertheless, we attempted to make the environment less rotationally ambiguous by reducing the number of corners to one (the teardrop enclosure).

Condition 3: Teardrop

A total of 12 HD cells (seven ensembles) were recorded in the teardrop from five rats over seven sessions. An example of a session in the teardrop is shown in Figure 4C. Note that, as in the trapezoid and the triangle, the firing direction of the cell did not rotate with the enclosure.

Overall, for all ensembles during a 90° teardrop rotation, the mean shift in firing direction was −23°, with a resultant vector length of 0.77 (Fig. 5C). A Rayleigh test showed that these data significantly clustered around the 23° shift (r = 5.41, p < 0.01) and were not significantly different from zero (upper CI = 60.73, lower CI = −14.9), indicating a strong propensity for the cells to remain oriented to the room. Correspondingly, we found a final deviation-from-expected value of 67° (±38.62 SD), which was significantly different from zero (upper CI = 104.85, lower CI = 29.79). Together, these results indicate that the rotations of the firing directions of the HD cells were significantly different from rotations of the teardrop environment and were not different from the direction predicted by the room.

Similarly, for all ensembles during a 180° teardrop rotation, the mean shift in firing direction was 12°, with a resultant vector length of 0.9 (Fig. 5C). A Rayleigh test showed that these data significantly clustered around the 12° value (r = 6.29, p < 0.01) and were not significantly different from zero (upper CI = 41.25, lower CI = −17.19), indicating a strong propensity for the cells to remain oriented to the room. Correspondingly, we found a final deviation-from-expected value of 168° (±25.75 SD), which was significantly different from zero (upper CI = 197.1, lower CI = 139.23).

The failure of HD cells to rotate their firing even in this unambiguously polarized teardrop condition suggests that perhaps HD cells are incapable of following geometric cues. However, it could be argued that, in a situation where cue conflict is very strong (geometry conflicting with uncontrolled auditory and olfactory cues, as well as the rat's internal sense of direction), HD cells may be unable to reliably use any local environmental cue. To test this possibility, a landmark (a white cue card) was attached to the shortest wall of the isosceles triangle. As we show below, the white cue card readily gained control of the cells.

Condition 4: Triangle plus white cue card

A total of 15 HD cells (six ensembles) were recorded from six rats during seven sessions when a white A3 cue card was attached to the shortest wall of the triangle. An example of a trial in the white cue card condition is shown in Figure 4D. Note that, unlike the previous examples, in this representative example, the firing direction of the cell reliably followed the triangle plus white card.

Overall, for all ensembles during a 90° triangle plus white card rotation, the mean shift in firing direction was 85°, with a resultant vector length of 0.99 (Fig. 5D). A Rayleigh test showed that these data significantly clustered around the 85° shift (r = 7.78, p < 0.01) and were significantly different from zero (upper CI = 92.82, lower CI = 77.35), indicating that the cells did not remain oriented to the room. Correspondingly, we found a final deviation-from-expected value of 5° (±9.62 SD), which was not significantly different from zero (upper CI = 12.61, lower CI = −2.86), indicating that the cells had followed the white cue card.

Similarly, for all ensembles during a 180° triangle plus white card rotation, the mean shift in firing direction was 173°, with a resultant vector length of 0.81 (Fig. 5D). A Rayleigh test showed that these data significantly clustered around the 173° value (r = 6.5, p < 0.01) and were significantly different from zero (upper CI = 204.55, lower CI = 141.52), indicating that the cells did not lock onto the room cues. Correspondingly, we found a final deviation-from-expected value of 7° (±35.0 SD), which was not significantly different from zero (upper CI = 38.96, lower CI = −24.64). Together, these results show that the rotations of the firing directions of the HD cells were not significantly different from rotations of the triangle plus white card environment, indicating that the firing directions remained generally aligned to the cue card.

We wondered whether visual saliency might explain why the cue card but not the shape controlled HD cells. Alternatively, perhaps the difference is categorical: cells will always follow a cue card but never follow geometry, regardless of salience. We tested this hypothesis in the next manipulation. To reduce the visual saliency of the cue card and thus make it visually more comparable to the geometry-only conditions, a dark gray cue card was attached to the shortest wall of the isosceles triangle.

Condition 5: Triangle plus gray cue card

A total of 15 HD cells in seven ensembles were recorded from five rats during seven sessions when a gray A3 cue card was attached to the shortest wall of the triangle. An example of a trial in the gray cue card condition is shown in Figure 4E. In this example, the HD cell clearly rotates on two trials but fails to rotate on the other two trials. This combination of HD rotations and fixations were typical of sessions in this condition. As the data points in Figure 5 represent total shifts in each four-rotation session, these intertrial variations have been averaged.

Overall, for all ensembles during a 90° triangle plus gray card rotation, the mean shift in firing direction was 41°, with a resultant vector length of 0.73 (Fig. 5E). A Rayleigh test showed that these data significantly clustered around the 41° shift (r = 5.08, p < 0.01) and were not significantly different from zero (upper CI = 82.51, lower CI = −0.57). Note that the confidence intervals show the values to be just significant. However, we did find a final deviation-from-expected value of 49° (±42.46 SD), which was significantly different from zero (upper CI = 91.1, lower CI = 7.45). Together, these results indicate that the rotations of the firing directions of the HD cells were significantly different from rotations of the triangle plus gray card environment.

For all ensembles during a 180° triangle plus gray card rotation, the mean shift in firing direction was −143°, with a resultant vector length of 0.11 (Fig. 5E). A Rayleigh test showed that these data did not significantly cluster around the −143° value (r = 0.8, NS), indicating that the cells in this condition showed large variations in the amount of firing direction rotation.

The cells during the gray cue card condition therefore failed to rotate with the gray card. However, we did observe that cells tended to rotate with the environment during one rotation, but never for all five rotations within a session. One possible explanation for this is that, unlike a visually salient white cue card, which could arguably be seen by the rats from anywhere in the environment, information about the gray cue card might only be processed if the rat was close to the card, by detection of tactile, olfactory, or weak visual cues. If the rat did not visit the card early on in a trial, this information might be processed after the HD circuit had established a stable orientation, resulting in a failure of the gray card to gain control over the cells. We therefore examined the path of the rats in this gray cue card condition to estimate when they first approached the cue card. The average time that a rat took to first approach the gray cue card when the deviation-from-expected value for the trial was <20° was 14.6 s. The average approach time when the deviation-from-expected for the trial was >20° was 15.0 s. An independent samples t test showed that these approach times were not significantly different (t(33) = 0.055, NS). These findings suggest that this proximity hypothesis is unlikely. However, no behavioral measures were taken during the trial, so definitive conclusions cannot be drawn.

The failure of cells to rotate with the gray cue card suggests that one possible reason for a lack of HD rotation in the geometry-only conditions is poor visual saliency. It could therefore be argued that HD cells can theoretically use geometric information but were unable to in this situation because the geometric cues had low salience and did not override other information sources. The final condition was designed to test this possibility by disrupting the path integration signal, a manipulation that has been shown to increase the use of geometric cues in behavioral studies (Batty et al., 2009). With fewer sources of competing directional information, geometric cues might now be able to influence HD orientation.

Condition 6: Disorientation

In this condition, rats were disoriented by the experimenter manually spinning the holding box (approximately one rotation per second) while walking around the curtained enclosure for ∼1 min before recording.

A total of 10 HD cells in four ensembles were recorded from three rats during six disorientation sessions in the triangle. An example of a disorientation trial is shown in Figure 4F. Note that, like the white cue card example, in this representative example, the firing direction of the cell reliably followed the triangle.

Overall, for all ensembles during a 90° disorientation plus triangle rotation, the mean shift in firing direction was 73°, with a resultant vector length of 0.87 (Fig. 5F). A Rayleigh test showed that these data significantly clustered around the 73° shift (r = 5.24, p < 0.01) and were significantly different from zero (upper CI = 107.14, lower CI = 39.53), indicating that the cells did not remain oriented to the room. Correspondingly, the final deviation-from-expected value of 17° (±28.87 SD) was not significantly different from zero (upper CI = 50.99, lower CI = −16.62). Together, these results suggest that the rotations of the firing directions of the HD cells were not significantly different from rotations of the triangle.

Similarly, for all ensembles during a 180° disorientation plus triangle rotation, the mean shift in firing direction was 175°, with a resultant vector length of 0.57 (Fig. 5F). A Rayleigh test showed that these data significantly clustered around the 175° value (r = 5.24, p < 0.05) and were significantly different from zero (upper CI = 107.14, lower CI = 39.53), indicating that the cells did not lock onto the room cues. Correspondingly, we found a final deviation-from-expected value of 5° (±53.12 SD), which was not significantly different from zero (upper CI = 50.99, lower CI = −16.62), indicating that the firing directions remained generally aligned to the geometry of the triangle.

This indicates that when the rat was disorientated, the HD cells used the geometric features of the triangle. This consequently suggests that in the basic triangle condition (where no disorientation occurred), geometric cues were potentially available to the HD cells, but were unable to override other uncontrolled directional cues such as path integration.

Comparison between conditions

It was predicted that because the conditions varied in the degree to which rats needed to process geometry to orient, the influence of geometry in each condition would also vary. More precisely, it was thought that the trapezoid would cause the HD cells to rotate the least, followed by the triangle, the teardrop, the gray cue card, and finally the white cue card. Overall significant differences were found between all the conditions for 180° (F(5,48) = 14.69, p < 0.001) and 90° (F(5,48) = 4.34, p < 0.01) rotations. Circular t tests were then used to do pairwise comparisons (with Bonferroni corrections) between all the conditions. All comparisons are shown in Table 3 for 180° and 90° rotations. Due to the small r value in the 180° gray cue card condition, the parametric test could not be used. However, sample sizes were too small to use the nonparametric test.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3.

Table of statistics for each condition

Table 3 show that there were no significant differences between the absolute mean deviations in the trapezoid, triangle, and teardrop conditions, which were the conditions in which firing directions failed to follow the enclosures. Similarly, there were no significant differences between the white cue card condition and the disorientation condition, which were the two conditions in which firing did follow the enclosures. Significant differences were found, however, with all the pairwise comparisons between the geometry-only conditions (trapezoid, triangle, and teardrop) and the white cue card and with the majority of comparisons between the geometry-only conditions and the disorientation condition, with the absolute mean deviations in the white cue card and disorientation conditions being significantly smaller (Fig. 6).

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Circular plots showing the mean HD cell shifts (lines) and confidence intervals (shaded areas) during 180° rotations for the triangle condition (red), the white cue card condition (blue), and the disorientation condition (green). The two graphs show that adding a proximal landmark (left) or disorientating the rat (right) produce significantly greater shifts in HD cells compared with a condition where geometric information was pitted against a stable path integration cue.

Discussion

This study explored the use of geometric cues by HD cells to determine whether geometry is an important source of orienting information (Cheng, 1986). We found that when rats were not disoriented, HD cells did not use even a highly polarized environmental geometry to orient their firing directions. Instead, firing directions generally remained aligned to the room. When rats were strongly disoriented, HD cells rotated with the bare enclosures. This demonstrates that HD cells can process geometry when other cues, such as landmarks and path integration, are unavailable, but this effect was relatively weak and failed to override path integration. This finding is important in light of previous suggestions that geometric cues are preferentially involved in reorientation—the so-called “geometric module hypothesis” (Cheng, 1986, 2008). Our results suggest this may be the case only when path integration is severely disrupted following disorientation.

There are two aspects of note in our findings: first, that HD cells did not appear sensitive to geometry in oriented rats; and second, that they were sensitive in disoriented rats. These two situations are discussed in turn below.

Influence of geometric cues in oriented animals

Relatively few behavioral studies have examined the influence of geometry in nondisoriented animals. Lourenco and Huttenlocher (2006) found that children used geometric cues to find a goal when disoriented by self-rotation, but did not use the same cues when it was the environment that had been rotated instead. Batty et al. (2009) found that disorientation of rats during training caused a relative preference for the use of geometric cues (as opposed to internal, path integration cues) during testing, although disorientation before the test itself did not increase the use of geometry any further. It appears from these studies that when orientation (path integration) cues were present, they influenced choices: findings that are consistent with our observation that when orientation or visual cues were present, HD cells apparently preferred them over geometry.

Studies of neuronal responses to changes in geometry have, by contrast, produced results that seem to contradict ours. Kubie and Ranck (1983) and Muller and Kubie (1987) found that hippocampal place cells remapped when nondisoriented animals were placed in environments of differing geometry. However, the environments also differed in nongeometric aspects (e.g., the material of the walls), information to which place cells are known to be sensitive (Anderson and Jeffery, 2003). It is not known whether HD cells changed orientation following these manipulations. These studies therefore do not speak directly to the issue of HD cell geometric processing.

Direct observation of HD cells has also, however, suggested a possible influence of geometry. Golob et al. (2001) found that HD cells changed orientation in response to the shift of the animals (without disorientation) from a square enclosure to a rectangular one. In these experiments, rats were trained to locate a corner of the square arena, which was polarized with a cue card, for water reward. When the arena was changed to a rectangle, cells rotated their firing directions on 12 of 13 occasions. These results suggest the processing of geometric cues by HD cells; however, most of the shifts in firing direction were multiples of 90°, suggesting that what changed was the cells' choice of corner to orient by. Given that the rats had been trained to specifically attend to corners to locate the reward, it is possible the HD cells were using corners as landmarks, rather than processing geometry as a holistic property of the environment. Thus, it is not clear whether this alteration represents true geometric processing by the HD cells.

An apparent effect of environment geometry on HD cells was also found by Dudchenko and Zinyuk (2005), who recorded HD cells in two T-mazes that were located in adjacent rooms and oriented differently. They found that HD cells rotated between the two rooms so as to maintain the same orientation with respect to the maze. However, while the cells were clearly orienting using the apparatus, it may not have been geometry per se that the cells were using. The rats were always started from the same place in the maze, and studies of place cells have shown that start position can be an orienting cue for place cells (Sharp et al., 1990), presumably via the head direction cells. Furthermore, the maze would provide a visually unique panorama from most locations, which means the HD cells would have potential visual orienting cues even if they did not process geometry directly.

Thus, although prior studies of HD cells have shown responsiveness to manipulations of environmental geometry, it has not been shown that the relevant factor was geometry itself and not some feature or other cue also present. The fact that our environments were relatively low in unique features, despite their distinctly polarized geometry, may explain why HD cells in our oriented rats preferred to use path integration or the cue card when these were available.

Influence of geometric cues in disoriented animals

Our finding that geometric cues by themselves have only a weak influence on HD cell orientation seems surprising given the substantial behavioral literature suggesting a primacy for geometric cues over landmarks (Cheng, 1986; for review, see Cheng and Newcombe, 2005). However, most behavioral studies have been undertaken in disoriented rats (Cheng, 1986; Maurer and Derivaz, 2000; Wall et al., 2004; Cheng and Gallistel, 2005; Skov-Rackette and Shettleworth, 2005; Gibson et al., 2007; Maes et al., 2009), in which path integration has been disrupted. The present results suggest that geometric cues are able to influence HD cells if other sources of orienting information are absent.

When other orienting cues are absent, our findings accord with previous research on HD cells. Taube et al. (1990b) found that when a square enclosure was changed to a rectangle, HD cells in disoriented rats rotated their firing directions, while Golob and Taube (1997) found that HD cells in disoriented rats with hippocampal lesions altered their firing when an enclosure was changed from a cylinder to a square, triangle, or pentagon. More recently, Clark et al. (2010) found that geometric cues could override the effect of a single landmark. These rotations may have been due to geometric processing, but may, as noted above, also result from processing of specific features arising from the geometry. Further experiments will be needed to test whether true geometric processing occurs during reorientation.

Interestingly, Clark et al. (2010) found that geometric cues tended not to override the effects of multiple landmarks, suggesting that the hierarchy of cue preference is salience-dependent rather than absolute. We found the use of a landmark to be salience-dependent when we manipulated its visual contrast. Thus, the switch between the use of different cues may result from salience-based competition rather than any absolute hierarchy of cue use. Previous studies have also found a switch in directional cue use by HD cells under some circumstances. For example, Knierim et al. (1995) found that a cue card was more likely to gain control over HD cells in rats that had learned about the cue while they were nondisoriented than in rats that had been disoriented during training. All animals were tested under the same disorientation conditions, suggesting that the cue card had lost salience because of the disorientation-training procedure. This finding is reminiscent of the Batty et al. (2009) behavioral study, in which disorientation during training caused a reduction in the use of geometric cues during testing. These experiments show that the influence of directional cues on HD cells can be reweighted with experience and is not absolute.

Our results additionally indicate that the orientation state of the animal at the time of testing influences cue use too. The finding that disorientation instantaneously modulates the relative influence of geometric cues is interesting because disorientation does not degrade the HD signal. This suggests that whatever decision process is responsible for assigning precedence to an input, it knows about the state of the path integrator. Thus, it seems able to distinguish between mild disorientation, when there is merely a slight disagreement between the path integrator and the environmental cues (as when the shape was rotated), and complete disorientation. HD cells seemed to use path integration in the former case and geometry in the latter. Since HD cells in thoroughly disoriented rats still fired strongly and with tight (albeit misdirected) tuning curves, the implication is that the signal about orientation state (mild vs severe), and thus the decision to use or ignore geometry, occurs outside the HD cells themselves. Potentially, there is an input that tells the HD cells that severe disorientation has occurred and therefore to weight external cues, including geometry, more heavily than normal. Such a scheme could provide an interesting model system for the study of Bayesian integration processes (Cheng et al., 2007).

Why, adaptively speaking, should geometry be such a weak cue for HD cell orientation? We can only speculate, but it is worth noting that the HD cell system is probably old, in evolutionary terms, whereas the ecological lifestyles of rats living in bounded spaces are more recent. Many animals that share a common ancestor with rodents, such as fish and birds, live in open spaces that lack a clear geometry. Furthermore, it is not clear that even in bounded spaces, it is geometry per se that is the relevant feature, rather than the visual panorama that derives from geometric configurations (Cheung et al., 2008; Stürzl et al., 2008). Further work is needed to determine what aspect of geometry influences HD cells in disoriented rats.

In summary, we have explored the use of geometric cues by HD cells and found that, in nondisoriented rats, their influence does not appear as strong as the influence of other simultaneously present cues such as a landmark or path integration. Conversely, in disoriented rats, in which path integration was disabled, geometry did have an effect, suggesting that it is processed, even if its effect is only weak. Since disorientation is presumably an unusual condition in the normal life of a rat, the implication is that local geometry is not a primary guide to the HD system in rodents, but may be used as a weak backup system for reorientation when other cues are unavailable.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by EU FP7 (“Spacebrain”) and Wellcome Trust grants to K.J., and a Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council studentship to R.K. and L.L.G. We thank Dina Lew for comments on the manuscript.

  • The authors declare no financial conflicts of interest.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Kathryn Jeffery, Institute of Behavioural Neuroscience, Department of Cognitive, Perceptual and Brain Sciences, Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London, London WC1H 0AP, UK. k.jeffery{at}ucl.ac.uk

This article is freely available online through the J Neurosci Open Choice option.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Anderson MI,
    2. Jeffery KJ
    (2003) Heterogeneous modulation of place cell firing by changes in context. J Neurosci 23:8827–8835.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    1. Batty ER,
    2. Hoban L,
    3. Spetch ML,
    4. Dickson CT
    (2009) Rats' use of geometric, featural and orientation cues to locate a hidden goal. Behav Processes 82:327–334.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Berens P
    (2009) CircStat: a MATLAB toolbox for circular statistics. J Stat Softw 31:1–21.
    OpenUrl
  4. ↵
    1. Cheng K
    (1986) A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation. Cognition 23:149–178.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Cheng K
    (2008) Whither geometry? Troubles of the geometric module. Trends Cogn Sci 12:355–361.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Cheng K,
    2. Gallistel CR
    (2005) Shape parameters explain data from spatial transformations: comment on Pearce et al (2004) and Tommasi and Polli (2004) J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 31:254–259.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Cheng K,
    2. Newcombe NS
    (2005) Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? Squaring theory and evidence. Psychon Bull Rev 12:1–23.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Cheng K,
    2. Shettleworth SJ,
    3. Huttenlocher J,
    4. Rieser JJ
    (2007) Bayesian integration of spatial information. Psychol Bull 133:625–637.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Cheung A,
    2. Stüzl W,
    3. Zeil J,
    4. Cheng K
    (2008) The information content of panoramic images. II. View-based navigation in nonrectangular experimental arenas. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 34:15–30.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Clark BJ,
    2. Harris MJ,
    3. Taube JS
    (2010) Control of anterodorsal thalamic head direction cells by environmental boundaries: comparison with conflicting distal landmarks. Hippocampus, Advance online publication. Retrieved Nov 25, 2010. doi:10.1002/hipo.20880.
    1. Dudchenko PA,
    2. Goodridge JP,
    3. Taube JS
    (1997) The effects of disorientation on visual landmark control of head direction cell orientation. Exp Brain Res 115:375–380.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Gibson BM,
    2. Wilks TJ,
    3. Kelly DM
    (2007) Rats (Rattus norvegicus) encode the shape of an array of discrete objects. J Comp Psychol 121:130–144.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Golob EJ,
    2. Taube JS
    (1997) Head direction cells and episodic spatial information in rats without a hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 94:7645–7650.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  13. ↵
    1. Golob EJ,
    2. Stackman RW,
    3. Wong AC,
    4. Taube JS
    (2001) On the behavioral significance of head direction cells: neural and behavioral dynamics during spatial memory tasks. Behav Neurosci 115:285–304.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Knierim JJ,
    2. Kudrimoti HS,
    3. McNaughton BL
    (1995) Place cells, head direction cells, and the learning of landmark stability. J Neurosci 15:1648–1659.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  15. ↵
    1. Seifert W
    1. Kubie J,
    2. Ranck J-B Jr.
    (1983) in The hippocampus, Sensory-behavioral correlates in individual hippocampus neurons in three situations: space and context, ed Seifert W (Academic, New York), pp 433–447.
  16. ↵
    1. Leutgeb S,
    2. Ragozzino KE,
    3. Mizumori SJ
    (2000) Convergence of head direction and place information in the CA1 region of hippocampus. Neuroscience 100:11–19.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Lourenco SF,
    2. Huttenlocher J
    (2006) How do young children determine location? Evidence from disorientation tasks. Cognition 100:511–529.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Maes JH,
    2. Fontanari L,
    3. Regolin L
    (2009) Spatial reorientation in rats (Rattus norvegicus): use of geometric and featural information as a function of arena size and feature location. Behav Brain Res 201:285–291.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Maurer R,
    2. Derivaz V
    (2000) Rats in a transparent Morris water maze use elemental and configural geometry of landmarks as well as distance to the pool wall. Spatial Cogn Comput 2:135–156.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  20. ↵
    1. Muller RU,
    2. Kubie JL
    (1987) The effects of changes in the environment on the spatial firing of hippocampal complex-spike cells. J Neurosci 7:1951–1968.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  21. ↵
    1. Paxinos G,
    2. Watson C
    (2004) The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates (Academic, New York).
  22. ↵
    1. Pecchia T,
    2. Vallortigara G
    (2010) Reorienting strategies in a rectangular array of landmarks by domestic chicks (Gallus gallus) J Comp Psychol 124:147–158.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Sharp PE,
    2. Kubie JL,
    3. Muller RU
    (1990) Firing properties of hippocampal neurons in a visually symmetrical environment: contributions of multiple sensory cues and mnemonic processes. J Neurosci 10:3093–3105.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  24. ↵
    1. Skov-Rackette SI,
    2. Shettleworth SJ
    (2005) What do rats learn about the geometry of object arrays? Tests with exploratory behavior. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 31:142–154.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Stürzl W,
    2. Cheung A,
    3. Cheng K,
    4. Zeil J
    (2008) The information content of panoramic images. I. The rotational errors and the similarity of views in rectangular experimental arenas. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 34:1–14.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Taube JS
    (1995) Place cells recorded in the parasubiculum of freely moving rats. Hippocampus 5:569–583.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Taube JS,
    2. Muller RU,
    3. Ranck JB Jr.
    (1990a) Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. I. Description and quantitative analysis. J Neurosci 10:420–435.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  28. ↵
    1. Taube JS,
    2. Muller RU,
    3. Ranck JB Jr.
    (1990b) Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. II. Effects of environmental manipulations. J Neurosci 10:436–447.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  29. ↵
    1. Taube JS,
    2. Goodridge JP,
    3. Golob EJ,
    4. Dudchenko PA,
    5. Stackman RW
    (1996) Processing the head direction cell signal: a review and commentary. Brain Res Bull 40:477–484.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. van der Meer MA,
    2. Richmond Z,
    3. Braga RM,
    4. Wood ER,
    5. Dudchenko PA
    (2010) Evidence for the use of an internal sense of direction in homing. Behav Neurosci 124:164–169.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Wall PL,
    2. Botly LC,
    3. Black CK,
    4. Shettleworth SJ
    (2004) The geometric module in the rat: independence of shape and feature learning in a food finding task. Learn Behav 32:289–298.
    OpenUrlPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 31 (44)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 31, Issue 44
2 Nov 2011
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Geometric Cues Influence Head Direction Cells Only Weakly in Nondisoriented Rats
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Geometric Cues Influence Head Direction Cells Only Weakly in Nondisoriented Rats
Rebecca Knight, Robin Hayman, Lin Lin Ginzberg, Kathryn Jeffery
Journal of Neuroscience 2 November 2011, 31 (44) 15681-15692; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2257-11.2011

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Geometric Cues Influence Head Direction Cells Only Weakly in Nondisoriented Rats
Rebecca Knight, Robin Hayman, Lin Lin Ginzberg, Kathryn Jeffery
Journal of Neuroscience 2 November 2011, 31 (44) 15681-15692; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2257-11.2011
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

  • Why Geometry Is a Critical Determinant Only in the Disoriented Animal
    Charles R Gallistel
    Published on: 28 January 2015
  • Published on: (28 January 2015)
    Page navigation anchor for Why Geometry Is a Critical Determinant Only in the Disoriented Animal
    Why Geometry Is a Critical Determinant Only in the Disoriented Animal
    • Charles R Gallistel, professor

    The conclusion drawn from the very interesting results reported in the Knight et al paper fails to distinguish between, on the one hand, what heading fundamentally is and how, on the other hand, heading may be maintained by reference to the changing bearing of landmarks. Heading is the direction in which your nose is pointed. Direction, as used in navigation, is only definable by reference to the geometry of a charted (r...

    Show More

    The conclusion drawn from the very interesting results reported in the Knight et al paper fails to distinguish between, on the one hand, what heading fundamentally is and how, on the other hand, heading may be maintained by reference to the changing bearing of landmarks. Heading is the direction in which your nose is pointed. Direction, as used in navigation, is only definable by reference to the geometry of a charted (represented) environment. To say that a cell is tuned to head direction is to say that it fires whenever the head is aligned with an infinite set of parallel lines notionally imposed on a representation of the environment. We have known since the work of Carr and Watson (1908) that, for the non-disoriented rat (and probably most other animals), the relevant environment--the environment with respect to whose representation heading is defined--is not the experimental chamber but rather the macroscopic environment, the larger environment that contains the experimental environment, which is to say the room or perhaps even the building, depending on how extensive the animal's opportunity to map its environment has been. It is wonderful to see that the same is true for head direction cells; their frame of reference is the room, not the experimental chamber or maze. That is what Carr and Watson discovered. They were greatly astonished by this discovery, indeed, bewildered, because they knew nothing of inertial dead reckoning.

    When a neuron has the property that it fires whenever the rat points in the same direction from different locations within an enclosed experimental chamber, it is tuned to the rat's heading, not to the bearing of any local landmark, because the bearings of all the local landmarks change as the animal moves from location to location. The bearing of a landmark is either the angle between one's heading and the landmark or the direction of the landmark from one's location. As the animal moves around inside an experimental chamber, all of these angles change constantly and by large amounts. Therefore, landmark bearing is not a direct indication of heading. Indeed, when one is disoriented with respect to the geometry of the local environment, landmark bearing is of no use in establishing heading direction.

    What needs to be understood in order to grasp the import of these findings is that heading is commonly maintained by reference to the changing bearings of local landmarks. When attempting to make your way in a given heading direction when there is no currently perceptible point in that direction in the local environment, you pick a readily localizable point in another direction and maintain your heading by moving so that the bearing of that point changes in the appropriate way. If the point of reference is initially 30 degrees to the left of your heading, then as you move in the direction of your heading that angle should increase, slowly at first, then more rapidly as you bring the landmark onto the beam and then again more slowly as you move on past it. The homogenously painted walls of an enclosure do not offer favorable readily localizable azimuthal points for this purpose. The edges of strongly contrasting card on a wall do offer a suitable points of reference. Thus, the fact that neurons tuned to heading direction in the macroscopic environment shift their tuning when the card is repositioned implies that the heading-implications of their firing is remapped when a good local point of reference is available, so that their signal can continue to be used in the just described manner in order to maintain their sense of their heading.

    The important point is that the remapping presupposes that the system has independent knowledge of its approximate heading. The source of this knowledge is known; it is angular dead reckoning, which relies heavily on input from the semi-circular canals, the organs that signal rotation in the inertial frame of reference. That is why damage removing vestibular input permanently disrupts the well-tuned firing of head-direction cells and place cells (Stackman, et al., 2002). That is also why head direction cells maintain their tuning even when the card is removed or the lights are extinguished, which is one of the older findings in the literature on the effects of card movement.

    Sustained rotation in the dark removes this critical source of directional information. Once the sense of direction has been destroyed in this way, strategies for maintaining that sense are no longer of use. Before the sense of direction can be maintained, it must be re- established. It can only be re-established by reference to the geometry of the environment, because, as noted at the outset, direction can only be usefully defined by reference to environmental geometry, not by reference to landmarks. The only portion of the charted environment to whose geometry the disoriented animal has sensory access when it is placed in an experimental enclosure is the geometry of that enclosure. It no longer knows its approximate orientation with respect to the geometry of the space outside the enclosure. That is why enclosure geometry suddenly becomes determinative of the tuning of head direction cells. It all comes back to the fact that environmental directions are defined by reference to the represented geometry of the environment: A direction is an infinite set of parallel lines in the vector space in which the brain maps the experienced environment.

    Citations

    Carr H and Watson JB (1908) Orientation of the white rat." J. Comp Neurol and Psychol 18: 27-44

    Stackman RW, Clark AS and Taube, JS (2002) Hippocampal Spatial Representations Require Vestibular Input. Hippocampus Online 12(3): 291-303.

    Conflict of Interest:

    None declared

    Show Less
    Competing Interests: None declared.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Articles

  • Choice Behavior Guided by Learned, But Not Innate, Taste Aversion Recruits the Orbitofrontal Cortex
  • Maturation of Spontaneous Firing Properties after Hearing Onset in Rat Auditory Nerve Fibers: Spontaneous Rates, Refractoriness, and Interfiber Correlations
  • Insulin Treatment Prevents Neuroinflammation and Neuronal Injury with Restored Neurobehavioral Function in Models of HIV/AIDS Neurodegeneration
Show more Articles

Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

  • Influence of Reward on Corticospinal Excitability during Movement Preparation
  • Identification and Characterization of a Sleep-Active Cell Group in the Rostral Medullary Brainstem
  • Gravin Orchestrates Protein Kinase A and β2-Adrenergic Receptor Signaling Critical for Synaptic Plasticity and Memory
Show more Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.