Abstract
Loss of somatosensory drive results in functional reorganization of the primary somatosensory cortex (SI). While the phenomenon of functional cortical reorganization is well established, it remains unknown whether in humans, functional reorganization results from changes in brain anatomy, or simply reflects an unmasking of already existing dormant synapses. In 20 subjects with complete thoracic spinal cord injuries (SCIs) and 23 controls, we used functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging to determine whether SI reorganization was associated with changes in SI anatomy. SCI resulted in a significant SI reorganization, with the little finger representation moving medially toward the lower body representation (i.e., area of sensory loss). Furthermore, although SCI was associated with gray matter volume loss in the lower body representation, this loss was minimized as reorganization increased. That is, the greater the medial shift in little finger representation, the greater the gray matter preservation in the lower body representation. In addition, in the region of greatest SI reorganization (little finger), fractional anisotropy was correlated with SI reorganization. That is, as SI reorganization increased, the extent of aligned structures decreased. Finally, although thalamocortical fibers remained unchanged, the ease and direction of water movement within the little finger representation was altered, being directed more toward the midline in SCI subjects. These data show that SI reorganization following SCI is associated with changes in SI anatomy and provide compelling evidence that SI reorganization in humans results from the growth of new lateral connections, and not simply from the unmasking of already existing lateral connections.
Introduction
It is well established that a loss of afferent sensory drive is followed by functional reorganization of the primary somatosensory cortex (SI) (Merzenich et al., 1983, 1984; Allard et al., 1991; Pons et al., 1991; Flor et al., 1995; Wrigley et al., 2009). That is, the area of SI deprived of its normal inputs is activated by sensory stimulation of the surrounding intact body regions. Despite the well established phenomenon of cortical reorganization, it remains unknown whether in humans, this functional reorganization results from physical changes in brain anatomy, such as dendritic sprouting, or simply reflects an unmasking of already existing dormant synapses. Although it has recently been reported in humans that SI reorganization can occur within hours of limb anesthesia, supporting the existence of dormant synapses (Björkman et al., 2009), it remains unknown whether such an “unmasking” is responsible for the large SI reorganization that occurs following limb amputation or spinal cord injury (SCI).
Over 10 years ago, Florence et al. (1998) investigated thalamocortical projection patterns in macaque monkeys with long-standing arm injuries including amputation. They found that although microelectrode recordings revealed massive reorganization of the affected limb SI representation, subsequent tract tracing revealed normal thalamocortical connections, but significantly expanded lateral connections. They suggested that the growth of intracortical, not thalamocortical connections, was responsible for the observed SI reorganization. Furthermore, nearly two decades ago, Pons et al. (1991) also reported a substantial degree of SI reorganization in monkeys with unilateral dorsal rhizotomies of all cervical spinal segments, with the representation of the face expanding by up to 14 mm. The authors suggested that the physical size of this reorganization was too great to be accounted for by solely the unmasking of latent connections and overlap of thalamocortical projections and must involve the growth of lateral connections (Garraghty and Sur, 1990; Rausell and Jones, 1995).
Despite evidence in experimental animals that cortical reorganization can involve physical changes in brain anatomy, it remains unknown whether in humans, SI reorganization involves changes in cortical anatomy. It is important to understand the precise nature of the underlying mechanisms of cortical organization in humans as anatomical changes are likely to be more resistant to treatments aimed at reversal. The aim of this investigation is to use voxel-based morphometry of T1-weighted and diffusion tensor images (DTIs) to determine whether the SI reorganization that occurs following SCI is associated with changes in cortical anatomy. Furthermore, using fiber-tracking techniques, we aim to determine whether changes in cortical anatomy reflect alterations in the properties of thalamocortical axons or changes within the cortical gray matter itself.
Materials and Methods
Subjects
Twenty subjects with established complete thoracic SCI (18 males, 2 females; aged 22–63 years) and 23 healthy control subjects without SCI (20 males; 3 females; aged 23–60 years) were recruited for the study (Table 1). For the SCI subjects, the neurological level of injury was determined using the American Spinal Injury Association (ASIA) Impairment Scale (AIS) (Marino et al., 2003). Each subject was examined by a clinician (P.J.W.) in the research group. The standardized ASIA examination protocol was used to determine the most caudal level of the spinal cord with normal sensory and motor function on both sides of the body. An injury was termed complete when there was an absence of sensory and motor function, which included the lowest sacral segments.
Spinal cord injury subject characteristics
To assess whether SCI resulted in increased fine upper limb movements, each SCI subject was asked to compare the average use of their hands for detailed task before and following their SCI. Each SCI subject was asked the following questions: (1) “Compared to before your SCI, I use my hands now: very rarely, rarely, in the same manner, more frequently, or much more frequently,” and (2) “Compared to before your SCI, has the use of your hands for detailed tasks changed (e.g., playing guitar or piano): I do not use my hands for more for detailed tasks, I sometimes use my hands for more detailed tasks, I frequently use my hands for detailed tasks.” Informed written consent was obtained for all procedures and the study was approved by the Northern Sydney and Central Coast Area Health Service Institutional Human Research Ethics Committees.
MRI scans
Subjects lay supine on the bed of a 3T MRI scanner (Philips, Intera) with their head immobilized in a tight-fitting head coil. Four continuous series of 130 gradient echo, echo-planar images, using blood oxygen level-dependent contrast and covering the entire brain were collected. Each functional MRI (fMRI) series was collected over a period of 390 s (57 axial slices, repetition time = 4 s, echo time = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°, field of view = 250 mm, raw voxel size = 1.95 × 1.62 × 3.3 mm). A 10 volume baseline period was followed by a 10 volume period during which either the right corner of the mouth, the pad of the right thumb, the pad of the right little finger, or the pad of the right big toe was continuously brushed with a fine plastic brush at ∼2 strokes/s. This was repeated another 5 times for a total of 6 baseline and 6 brushing periods.
In addition, using a single-shot multisection spin-echo echo-planar pulse sequence (repetition time = 8788 ms; flip angle = 90°, matrix size = 112 × 112, field of view = 224 × 224 mm, slice thickness = 2.5 mm, 55 axial slices), four high-resolution DTI image sets covering the entire brain were collected. For each slice, diffusion gradients were applied along 32 independent orientations with b = 1000 s/mm2 after the acquisition of b = 0 s/mm2 (b0) images. Four DTI series were collected separately for subsequent averaging.
MRI analysis
fMRI.
Using SPM5 (Friston et al., 1995), fMRI images were realigned and spatially normalized to the MNI template, global signal changes were removed using the detrending method described by Macey et al. (2004), and the images were spatially smoothed (6 mm FWHM). Signal intensity increases were assessed using a repeated box-car model convolved with a conical hemodynamic response function. The maximally activated voxel in contralateral (left) SI was determined in each individual subject during each of the four brushing paradigms and the Euclidean distance between the maximally activated voxel and an anatomical marker (dorsal point where the central sulcus meets the midline) was determined and compared between the SCI and control groups (two-tailed t test) (for details, see Wrigley et al., 2009). The degree of cortical reorganization of the little finger was displayed as absolute changes compared to healthy controls (difference of the euclidean distance between controls and SCI).
In no SCI subject did brushing of the big toe evoke a significant change in SI signal intensity. In 15 of the remaining 152 fMRI scans, brushing did not result in any significantly activated voxels within the contralateral SI (3 lip; 0 thumb; 9 little finger; 3 big toe).
For display purposes and for subsequent DTI tractography, clusters containing the highest activated 20 contiguous voxels within the left SI and the left thalamus during each brushing paradigm were derived and overlaid onto an individual's T1-weighted anatomical image. To take into consideration the bilateral nature of spinal cord injury, these clusters were reflected across the midline for display on the right SI. The percentage change in signal intensity (relative to the first 10 volumes) was then calculated from this cluster for each individual subject and these were then averaged to create a mean ± SEM signal intensity change time-trend.
VBM of T1-weighted images.
Using SPM, the three T1-weighted images from each subject were coregistered and averaged. The averaged image was bias corrected using the SPM5 unified segmentation (Ashburner and Friston, 2005). The bias-corrected images were segmented and spatially normalized using a second pass of the unified segmentation algorithm. The results of the segmentation and spatial normalization were whole-brain “maps” of gray matter probabilities, spatially normalized into the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template space, and “modulated” by the volume changes due to the normalization (Ashburner and Friston, 2000). The normalized, modulated gray matter images were smoothed (full width at half maximum = 6 mm). A voxel-by-voxel analysis was performed to search for gray matter volumes within SI that were significantly correlated with the degree of cortical reorganization in SCI subjects. The degree of reorganization was measured by the Euclidean distances observed during brushing of the little finger as changes in this area were most pronounced (random effects, uncorrected p < 0.001, age and gender as nuisance variables). Significant gray matter volumes were then overlaid onto an individual's T1-weighted anatomical image for visualization.
DTI—indices.
The DTI images were processed using SPM5 and Matlab-based custom software. The diffusion-weighted images were motion corrected, coregistered to one another, and averaged to increase signal-to-noise ratio. Using the 32 directions and b0 images, the diffusion tensor was then calculated using the method proposed by Basser and Pierpaoli (1996). Once the elements of diffusion tensor were calculated, fractional anisotropy (FA) maps were derived and the images spatially normalized and smoothed (6 mm FWHM). A voxel-by-voxel analysis was performed to search for FA values within SI that were significantly correlated with the degree of cortical reorganization in SCI subjects, i.e., using the Euclidean distances observed during brushing of the little finger (random effects, uncorrected p < 0.001, age and gender as nuisance variables). Significant FA clusters were then overlaid onto an individual's T1-weighted anatomical image. For each significant cluster, the relationship between FA and time since injury was also assessed.
DTI—fiber tracking.
Tractography was performed using mrDiffusion software (Dougherty et al., 2005), based on the diffusion tensor calculated as described above. Fibers were tracked between the thalamic region activated during brushing of the lip, little finger, thumb, and toe and the SI region in which MD was correlated with SI reorganization. For each subject, these clusters were “un-normalized” into their native space and the output fibers tracked from this seeding cluster using a minimum FA value of 0.15 and a maximum turning angle of 30°. These fibers were then clipped so that any fibers caudal to the thalamus were removed and the resulting fibers were then normalized into MNI space, binarized, and added to the output fibers from other subjects to provide an indication of the frequency of each fiber bundle in all 40 subjects (controls n = 23; SCI n = 17). The mean FA values of the fibers were calculated in each subject and averaged for the control and SCI groups, and differences between the groups were determined (two-sample t tests, two tailed, p < 0.05).
DTI—principal eigenvector analysis.
For selected areas in SI showing functional reorganization, the directional nature of structure was indicated with a vector on a polar plot, corresponding to the direction of the principal eigenvector of the diffusion tensor (see above, DTI—indices). The vector was scaled by the fractional anisotropy, so that regions with little directional organization of structure would show small vectors. This vector indicates the predominant direction of water diffusion, and therefore is an indirect measure of axonal direction. We calculated the mean direction across voxels in regions that showed the greatest structural differences (see above, DTI—indices) to better understand the nature of those structural changes. We also indicated variation by displaying vectors of the mean ± the mean absolute deviation (in lieu of SD, which is complex for vectors).
Results
Cortical reorganization
All subjects had complete (AIS A; profound loss of sensation and motor control below the injury level with absence in the sacral segments) thoracic spinal cord injuries. Fourteen of the twenty SCI subjects stated that they did not use their upper limbs to perform fine manipulation more frequently following their SCI than before their injury. Three SCI patients stated they used their upper limbs more for detailed tasks, and the remaining three reported they used their upper limbs for a lot more following their SCI.
The sensory loss was associated with a significant SI reorganization. That is, the SI representations of the thumb and little finger were displaced medially in SCI subjects compared to control subjects. Mean (±SEM) euclidean distance for the lip was as follows: controls 70.8 ± 0.8 mm, SCI 69.5 ± 1.5 mm; for the thumb: controls 65.8 ± 1.3 mm, SCI 59.3 ± 2.6 mm (p < 0.05); and for the little finger: controls 62.3 ± 2.0 mm, SCI 48.8 ± 2.2 mm (p < 0.0005) (Fig. 1). Therefore, compared to controls, in SCI subjects, the thumb activation was displaced ∼7 mm and the little finger activation 13 mm toward the midline. Although the degree of SI reorganization was not significantly correlated with the injury level (r = 0.13; p > 0.05), it was significantly correlated with time since injury (r = −0.51; p < 0.05) and age (r = −0.57; p < 0.05). That is, the greater the age and time since injury, the greater the medial displacement of the little finger.
SI functional reorganization following complete thoracic SCI. Mean maximally activated SI voxels during innocuous brushing of the lip, thumb, and little finger are shown for healthy controls (green) and subjects with complete thoracic SCI (blue). Cortical organization of the lip, the thumb, and the little finger is displayed as difference of the euclidean distance between controls and SCI. Note that in SCI subjects, the SI representations of the little finger and thumb have moved medially toward the SI region that would normally receive sensory information from the lower body (the region from which sensory information is lost in SCI subjects).
Anatomical changes in SI region of sensory loss (lower body)
Complete thoracic SCI was associated with a significant reduction in gray matter volume in the region of SI that would normally represent the lower body [mean ± SEM (prob × vol): right SI: controls 0.45 ± 0.05; SCI 0.33 ± 0.02; left SI: controls 0.39 ± 0.07; SCI 0.26 ± 0.03] (Fig. 2). Although SCI was associated with a significant decrease in gray matter volume in the SI region of sensory loss, the magnitude of this gray matter decrease was minimized with increased medial displacement of the surrounding SI representations. That is, the greater the invasion of surrounding SI representations (greater medial displacement), the less gray matter loss (increased gray matter volume) within the leg area of SI (left SI: r = 0.71, p = 0.001; right SI: r = 0.62, p = 0.008). Gray matter loss was not correlated with the injury level (p > 0.05; left SI: r = 0.05; right SI: r = 0.06). Furthermore, MD values within the area of gray matter loss were not significantly different between controls and SCI subjects [mean ± SEM (mm2/s × 10−3): right SI: controls 1.13 ± 0.04; SCI 1.17 ± 0.04; left SI: controls 1.61 ± 0.05; SCI 1.73 ± 0.06] and were not correlated with the degree of SI reorganization.
Top, Regions of the SI in which the degree of functional reorganization is significantly correlated with gray matter volume (hot color scale) in SCI subjects. The value at the bottom of each coronal section indicates the slice location in Montreal Neurological Institute space. Below are plots of gray matter volume against the degree of SI reorganization (displayed as difference of the euclidean distance between controls and SCI) and mean (±SEM) gray matter volumes in SCI (blue) and control (green) subjects. Note that gray matter volume is reduced in SCI subjects in the medial aspect of SI. Furthermore, this gray matter volume reduction is minimized as the degree of medial displacement of the little finger representation increases. Bottom, Region of SI that is activated by innocuous brushing the big toe in control subjects (green). Note that this region overlaps the region of gray matter loss in SCI subjects. To the right is a plot of the mean percentage change in signal intensity during brushing of the big toe in control and SCI subjects. The vertical gray bars indicate the brushing periods. Note the lack of signal change in SCI subjects.
Comparison of the locations of SI activations during innocuous brushing of the big toe in control subjects and the region of gray matter change in SCI subjects confirmed that the gray matter loss was located in the region of SI previously representing the lower body. Furthermore, brushing the big toe in SCI subjects did not evoke a significant signal intensity increase within SI.
Anatomical changes in SI region undergoing reorganization (little finger)
In addition to change in gray matter volume, in SCI subjects, the degree of SI reorganization was significantly correlated with FA in the region of SI representing the hand (Fig. 3). That is, the greater the little finger activation was displaced medially compared to controls (greater SI reorganization), the lower the FA within the hand area of SI (left SI: r = 0.65, p = 0.005; right SI: r = 0.82, p = 0.00006). This region did not display a significant FA difference in SCI compared with control subjects (mean ± SEM: right SI: controls 0.227 ± 0.009; SCI 0.209 ± 0.008; left SI: controls 0.145 ± 0.009; SCI 0.137 ± 0.008). Furthermore, gray matter volumes in these regions were not significantly correlated with the degree of SI reorganization and were not significantly different between controls and SCI subjects [mean ± SEM (prob × vol): right SI: controls 0.48 ± 0.04; SCI 0.40 ± 0.03; left SI: controls 0.40 ± 0.07; SCI 0.37 ± 0.02].
Top, Regions of the SI in which the degree of functional reorganization is significantly correlated with fractional anisotropy (hot color scale) in SCI subjects. The value at the bottom of each coronal section indicates the slice location in Montreal Neurological Institute space. Below are plots of fractional anisotropy against the degree of SI reorganization (displayed as difference of the euclidean distance between controls and SCI), and average (±SEM) fractional anisotropy in SCI (blue) and control (green) subjects. Note that fractional anisotropy decreases in a discrete region of SI as medial displacement of the little finger representation increases. Bottom, Region of SI that is activated by innocuous brushing the little finger in control (green) and SCI (blue) subjects. Note that this region overlaps the region of fractional anisotropy change in SCI subjects. To the right is a plot of the mean percentage change in signal intensity during brushing of the little finger in control and SCI subjects. The vertical gray bars indicate the brushing periods. Note that brushing the little finger evokes signal intensity increases in both SCI and control subjects.
Comparison of the locations of SI activations during innocuous brushing of the little finger in control and SCI subjects and the region of FA change in SCI subjects confirmed that the FA change was located in the region of SI representing the upper body, the region of SI displaying reorganization.
Thalamocortical fiber tract properties
Although FA values in area of SI representing the little finger were altered in accordance with the degree of SI reorganization, the fiber tract connecting the ventroposterior (VP) thalamus to this SI region was unaffected. That is, the fiber tract connecting the VP thalamus with the SI region representing the little finger displayed no significant difference in FA values in SCI compared with controls (mean ± SEM FA: right VP-SI tract: controls 0.43 ± 0.01; SCI 0.41 ± 0.02; left VP-SI tract: controls 0.44 ± 0.01; SCI 0.44 ± 0.02) (Fig. 4). Further, there was no significant correlation between the FA values and the degree of SI reorganization.
Properties of thalamocortical fibers in control and SCI subjects. Fibers connecting the somatosensory ventroposterior (VP) thalamus (yellow) to the region of SI in which fractional anisotropy was significantly correlated with SI reorganization (pink), overlaid onto two coronal anatomical images. The thalamocortical fiber frequency was determined by combining all of the control and SCI subjects. The value at the bottom right of each coronal section indicates the slice location in Montreal Neurological Institute space. Hot color scale represents the number of subjects in which the voxel was present in the thalamocortical fiber bundle. Note that the fractional anisotropy of the thalamocortical bundles were not correlated with the degree of SI reorganization (medial displacement) in SCI subjects. Furthermore, there was no difference in the mean (± SEM) fractional anisotropy values of thalamocortical fibers in controls (green) compared with SCI (blue) subjects.
Water movement changes in the SI cortex
Although in SCI subjects there was no change in the properties of thalamocortical fibers, close inspection of the diffusion directions within the region of decreased MD, revealed significant changes in the direction and magnitude of the principle eigenvectors (Fig. 5). In controls, the principal eigenvector was directed anterior–posterior, whereas in SCI subjects it was directed medial–lateral, toward the region that would normally represent the leg. In contrast, in the coronal plane, both control and SCI subjects had principal eigenvectors that were directed approximately perpendicular to the cortical surface.
Principal eigenvectors in control and SCI subjects in the SI region in which fractional anisotropy decreased as SI reorganization increased. Mean principle eigenvectors in control (dark green) subjects are directed approximately perpendicular to the cortical surface, In contrast, the principle eigenvectors in SCI subjects (dark blue) are directed more parallel to the cortical surface, i.e., toward the area of sensory deprivation. The SDs of the principle eigenvectors for controls (light green) and SCI subjects (light blue) are also shown.
Discussion
Over twenty-five years ago, Merzenich et al. (1984) used electrophysiological techniques to show that following digit amputation in the owl monkey, the SI region normally innervated by the now amputated digit is invaded by neighboring SI representations. Consistent with this report and subsequent human studies (Flor et al., 1995; Bruehlmeier et al., 1998), we found that in complete thoracic SCI subjects, the functional representation of the little finger shifted toward the region that would normally receive inputs from the legs (Wrigley et al., 2009). If this functional reorganization resulted exclusively from the unmasking of dormant synapses, one would expect no change in SI anatomy. Instead, our data revealed that SI reorganization following long-standing SCI is associated with a significant change in SI anatomy. We therefore propose that in humans, cortical reorganization results at least in part from changes in cortical anatomy, such as axonal sprouting.
Gray matter loss in the area of sensory loss
Complete thoracic SCI is associated with SI gray matter loss, the degree of which is correlated with the degree of SI reorganization. Although SCI resulted in gray matter loss in the SI region representing the lower body, this loss was minimized with increased degrees of reorganization, i.e., the greater the invasion by neighboring SI representations, the less gray matter loss in the lower body SI representation. Although these changes appear contradictory, they are consistent with the idea that SI reorganization is associated with physical changes in cortical anatomy. For example, it is possible that a loss of ascending input neurons results in a gray matter volume decrease, which is followed by a physical invasion of neighboring representations, so the greater the SI reorganization, the smaller the gray matter volume loss. Combined, these two mechanisms could result in the findings presented here.
Despite variations in injury levels (T1 to T11), neither gray matter volume, nor the degree of SI reorganization varied with injury level. This may reflect the relatively little thoracic somatosensory input, as evidenced by poor two-point discrimination ability (Nolan, 1985). Further, it is unlikely that the change in SI reorganization following SCI resulted from changes in upper limb use, since the most SCI patients reported no change in fine upper limb use following their injuries. We also found that the degree of gray matter loss and SI reorganization did not diminish with age and varied considerably between individuals, despite similar injuries. These findings raise the possibility that the ability of an individual's brain to reorganize and remodel following traumatic injury varies from individual to individual and does not diminish with age.
Latent lateral synapses
At least some of the cortical reorganization that occurs following SCI may result from the unmasking of dormant synapses. Calford and Tweedale (1988) described rapid SI changes (within a single day) following single digit amputation in flying foxes, and in humans, cortical reorganization can occur within hours following brief limb or digit anesthesia (Rossini et al., 1994; Björkman et al., 2009). This relatively rapid reorganization likely results from disinhibition or facilitation of existing subthreshold inputs onto neighboring cortical representations. The unmasking of latent synapses may be responsible for at least small shifts in cortical organization (<2–3 mm), and this process may play a role in the cortical reorganization that occurs following complete SCI. However, given that complete SCI results in large-scale SI reorganization, with a shift in the little finger representation of >13 mm, and the clinical observation that neuropathic SCI pain commonly develops months or even years following injury (Siddall et al., 2003), it is likely that the major mechanism responsible for this large reorganization is the relatively slow growth of lateral connections.
Growth of SI lateral connections
Nearly two decades ago, Pons et al. (1991) reported a similar degree of SI reorganization in monkeys following unilateral cervical dorsal rhizotomies, with the face representation expanding by up to 14 mm. It was suggested that this extensive expansion was too great to be accounted for by the unmasking of latent connections and overlap of thalamocortical projections (Garraghty and Sur, 1990; Rausell and Jones, 1995). Furthermore, in noninjured animals, horizontal intracortical connections do not exist between areas of SI innervated by the mandibular nerve and those innervated by the second cervical nerve, and limited horizontal interconnections exist between hand and lower jaw/neck SI representations (Manger et al., 1997). Therefore, large cortical shifts that occur following massive sensory losses likely involve multiple mechanisms, which include the relatively rapid unmasking of already existing synapses, but more importantly, the slower growth of new dendrites.
Evidence in other sensory systems also supports the suggestion that axonal sprouting occurs following sensory denervation. Following retinal lesions, axonal sprouting of laterally projecting neurons accompanies topographic remodeling of the visual cortex (Darian-Smith and Gilbert, 1994, 1995). Within the somatosensory system, in macaque monkeys with long-standing, accidental trauma to a forelimb, “thalamocortical projections were at least relatively normal, whereas the changes at the cortical level were immense” (Florence et al., 1998). Furthermore, 5 months after discrete primary motor cortex lesions in primates, Dancause et al. (2005) reported that new connections were formed between the ventral premotor cortex and SI, revealing reorganization between different brain systems.
Our data also support the idea that SI cortical reorganization is associated with expansion of cortical lateral connections. Not only were the changes in SI fractional anisotropy localized to the area that represents the little finger, i.e., the area that displayed the greatest reorganization, but thalamocortical fibers were unaffected, although technical limitations may have limited our ability to detect fine changes in thalamocortical fibers (see below, Limitations). Furthermore, the direction of the principal water movement axis changed from being anterior–posterior in the axial plane to lateromedial, suggesting a greater degree of organized water movement toward the area representing the lower body. Indeed, if SI reorganization resulted entirely from the unmasking of latent synapses, one would expect no change in anatomy in the SI region displaying functional reorganization.
Subcortical reorganization
Subcortical structures may also play a role in SI reorganization. Garraghty and Kaas (1991) described thalamic reorganization following sectioning of peripheral afferents, and following dorsal column lesions or arm amputations in adult monkeys, face afferents in the brainstem trigeminal nucleus can grow into the adjacent cuneate nucleus (Jain et al., 2000). In humans with complete SCI, ventral thalamus cells that would normally respond to inputs from upper extremity respond to inputs from the neck and occiput (Lenz et al., 1987), and sensory thalamus stump representations expand in amputees (Davis et al., 1998). Further, Moore et al. (2000) reported in complete SCI patients that referred sensations were associated with activation of both the SI region representing the body region being stimulated and the area SI representing the location of referred sensation, with 2 cm of nonresponsive cortex in between. Whether changes in subcortical structures underlie the large cortical reorganization reported in this study remains an open question.
SI reorganization and pain
It is interesting to speculate how this observed SI reorganization contributes to behavior following SCI, particularly as the changes are occurring in a sensory rather than motor area of the cortex. Any observed behaviors are likely to be influenced and complicated by subsequent CNS processing, and thus not surprising is the evidence from previous work that the strongest link between reorganization and behavior is an abnormality of sensory processing and the presence of pain. We and others have shown that the degree of SI reorganization following amputation or SCI is positively correlated with the intensity of ongoing pain (Flor et al., 1995; Wrigley et al., 2009). Further, the finding that the lip representation did not alter suggests that the changes in SI organization following SCI are site specific and stronger in the region close to the site of deafferentation, rather than global in nature.
Limitations
Although possible, it is unlikely that SI reorganization following SCI resulted from alterations in upper limb use, since the vast majority of SCI patients reported no increase in fine upper limb use following their injury. Furthermore, our data reveal a shift in SI organization (thumb and little finger representations have shifted) rather than a simple expansion of the hand representation, which would have occurred due to somatosensory training.
While the fiber-tracking technique is not suited to detecting small bundles or nerves, due to resolution and noise limitations, the present study assessed primarily large tracts with this approach, which are more reliably detected by the technique; multiple series also improve signal-to-noise ratios (Turner et al., 2003). Crossing and sprouting of fiber groups are not well detected by fiber-tracking techniques, due to partial-volume and resolution factors, so we were unable to detect fine changes in thalamocortical connections. Such limitations are inherent in all current fiber-tracking approaches based on current DTI methodology.
Conclusions
We have shown that the functional reorganization in SI following spinal cord injury is accompanied by structural changes. Specifically, the functional representation of the little finger shifted toward the region that would normally receive inputs from the legs, with structural indications of axonal growth between these areas.
Footnotes
This work was supported by grants awarded by the New South Wales Office for Science and Medical Research. We thank Kirsten Moffat for help in all imaging procedures. Scans were performed at the Prince of Wales Imaging Centre.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Luke A. Henderson at the above address. lukeh{at}anatomy.usyd.edu.au