Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Brief Communications

Optimal Encoding of Interval Timing in Expert Percussionists

Guido Marco Cicchini, Roberto Arrighi, Luca Cecchetti, Marco Giusti and David C. Burr
Journal of Neuroscience 18 January 2012, 32 (3) 1056-1060; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3411-11.2012
Guido Marco Cicchini
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Roberto Arrighi
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Luca Cecchetti
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Marco Giusti
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
David C. Burr
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Figures

  • Figure 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 1.

    A, Illustration of Jazayeri and Shadlen's (2010) ideal observer model. The likelihood function for the current stimulus [P(M|S); Eq. 1] is modeled by a Gaussian of width σL, and windowed by the prior P(S) (red), which represents the previous sensory history, to yield the posteriori estimate P(S|M) in Equation 1 (orange). The reproduced interval is given by the BLS estimate from the posteriori distribution (star). The model introduces some biasing errors (as the BLS estimate no longer corresponds to the stimulus), but it reduces the variance of production times. B, Illustration of Gaussian prior models. In this version, the prior is a Gaussian probability density function derived from past trials. In the fixed prior model, the width of the prior σP is fixed across observers and conditions. In the optimized prior version, σP is calculated to minimize total error (see text; Fig. 4).

  • Figure 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 2.

    A, Reproduction distribution of visual stimuli for nonmusical controls for the duration 850 ms during sessions where the intervals were drawn from short (squares, 494–847 ms), intermediate (circles, 671–1024 ms), or long (triangles, 847–1200 ms) intervals. B, C, As in A, for expert drummers (B) or bowstring musicians (C). D, Average reproduction durations of visual stimuli for subjects without musical training as a function of stimulus duration for three stimulus ranges (squares, 494–847 ms; circles, 671–1024 ms; triangles, 847–1200 ms). Straight lines show best-fitting linear regressions over each range. The central tendency index is given as the difference between the slope of these fits and the equality line (dashed). E, F, As in D, for expert drummers (E) and bowstring musicians (F). G–I, As for D–F, except for auditory rather than visual stimuli. Here all subjects performed veridically (on average).

  • Figure 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 3.

    A, Regression index plotted against Weber fraction for interval bisection. The regression index is the deviation from veridicality of the linear fit to reproduction data like those of Figure 2 (0 for veridicality, 1 for total regression to the mean). Small symbols are individual subjects; large symbols are groups (blue, drummers; green, string musicians; red, controls). Filled circles show results for the visual task, open triangle for audition. The curves show the no-prior model (dashed black), Jazayeri and Shadlen's (2010) BLS model (gray), our Bayesian model (Fig. 1B) with prior of fixed width of 120 ms (cyan), and the Bayesian model where the width of the prior is chosen to optimize performance following the procedure of Figure 4 (orange). The continuous curve shows the model where the width of the prior is curtailed to remain >90 ms (estimated to best fit the data), the dashed where it is free to become infinitely narrow. B, CV (normalized average root variance of the reproductions) plotted against bias (difference between average production time and physical sample interval) for visual reproductions for the three subject groups (colors as in A). The total error (root mean squared error) is given by the distance from the origin, similar for the three groups. The continuous curves show simulations of the models in A, assuming the existence of a further nonsensory noise source, assumed to be constant for all subjects, and optimized to provide best overall fit of data (in practice a Weber fraction of 0.1). Each curve was created by varying sensory Weber fraction from 0.01 to 0.3: the colored stars superimposed indicate the average Weber fractions for that subject group.

  • Figure 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 4.

    A, B, Error landscapes showing relative RMS error for Weber fractions plotted against regression index (A) or width of the prior, σp (B). The points show the average data for our subjects (circles, vision; triangles, audition; blue, drummers; green, string musicians; red, controls). All points lie in the minimal-error valley, indicating that their behavioral choices are near optimal and adaptable. Note that A is a generic solution for any mechanism that causes a regression toward the mean, while B refers specifically to the Gaussian-prior model of variable width.

Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 32 (3)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 32, Issue 3
18 Jan 2012
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Optimal Encoding of Interval Timing in Expert Percussionists
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Optimal Encoding of Interval Timing in Expert Percussionists
Guido Marco Cicchini, Roberto Arrighi, Luca Cecchetti, Marco Giusti, David C. Burr
Journal of Neuroscience 18 January 2012, 32 (3) 1056-1060; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3411-11.2012

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Optimal Encoding of Interval Timing in Expert Percussionists
Guido Marco Cicchini, Roberto Arrighi, Luca Cecchetti, Marco Giusti, David C. Burr
Journal of Neuroscience 18 January 2012, 32 (3) 1056-1060; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3411-11.2012
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Heteromodal Cortical Areas Encode Sensory-Motor Features of Word Meaning
  • Pharmacologically Counteracting a Phenotypic Difference in Cerebellar GABAA Receptor Response to Alcohol Prevents Excessive Alcohol Consumption in a High Alcohol-Consuming Rodent Genotype
  • Neuromuscular NMDA Receptors Modulate Developmental Synapse Elimination
Show more Brief Communications
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.