Abstract
Using resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging data from two independent samples of healthy adults, we parsed the amygdala's intrinsic connectivity into three partially distinct large-scale networks that strongly resemble the known anatomical organization of amygdala connectivity in rodents and monkeys. Moreover, in a third independent sample, we discovered that people who fostered and maintained larger and more complex social networks not only had larger amygdala volumes, but also amygdalae with stronger intrinsic connectivity within two of these networks: one putatively subserving perceptual abilities and one subserving affiliative behaviors. Our findings were anatomically specific to amygdalar circuitry in that individual differences in social network size and complexity could not be explained by the strength of intrinsic connectivity between nodes within two networks that do not typically involve the amygdala (i.e., the mentalizing and mirror networks), and were behaviorally specific in that amygdala connectivity did not correlate with other self-report measures of sociality.
Introduction
Like most primates, humans are a very social species. For humans, other people can be a source of stress or the greatest source of joy. Having a rich social network with many relationships has quantifiable health benefits (Cohen et al., 1997; Hawkley and Cacioppo, 2010). Yet people vary greatly from one another in the size of their social networks (Dunbar and Spoors, 1995; Hill and Dunbar, 2003). In 2011, our laboratory demonstrated that individuals with larger amygdala volumes have larger and more complex social networks (Bickart et al., 2011). This initial study extended comparative neuroanatomy findings that, across species, primates living in larger social groupings also have larger amygdalae (Barton and Aggleton, 2000; Barton, 2006). Building on this work, two recent papers have shown that the amygdala's gray matter density is correlated with both online and real-world social network size (Kanai et al., 2012), and seems to increase in monkeys housed in larger social groups (Sallet et al., 2011). Although the amygdala is a key structure within the “social brain” (Brothers, 1990; Lieberman, 2007; Adolphs, 2009), individuals with larger social networks also have more gray matter in other brain regions implicated in adaptive social behaviors such as the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (Bickart et al., 2011), ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) (Lewis et al., 2011), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) (Sallet et al., 2011; Powell et al., 2012), superior temporal sulcus (STS), temporal pole, and frontal pole (Sallet et al., 2011).
In this paper, we tested the hypothesis that more socially connected people have brains characterized by stronger intrinsic connectivity between the amygdala and other brain regions subserving social cognition, using resting-state functional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging (fcMRI). Intrinsic connectivity provides a basis for understanding the large-scale anatomic organization of brain networks (Fox and Raichle, 2007), and individual differences in intrinsic connectivity strength within certain networks predicts individual differences in motor function (Fox et al., 2007), memory (Wang et al., 2010), executive function (Seeley et al., 2007), and affect (Seeley et al., 2007; van Marle et al., 2010; Touroutoglou et al., 2012).
Because the amygdala is composed of multiple nuclei with differing connectivity profiles, we first sought to delineate the anatomical networks anchored in the amygdala that might each subserve distinct functions needed to build and maintain larger social networks. Based on our synthesis of published anatomical and functional data in humans and nonhuman animals (see Materials and Methods), we hypothesized that the amygdala would parse into three subregions that each anchor a large-scale network of brain regions implicated in distinct processes of social cognition. The anatomic studies provided a priori hypotheses about the network constitution, and the functional studies supported these hypotheses while providing a priori descriptions of their psychological importance in social cognition. In two independent samples contained in Experiment 1 (n = 89 and n = 83), we tested and replicated our hypothetical neuroanatomical framework using a two-step data-driven fcMRI analytic approach. The results of this first experiment generated three sets of brain regions of interest (ROIs) representing the three hypothesized networks. In the second experiment, using a third independent sample (n = 29), we used the ROIs generated from the first experiment in an a priori fashion to test the hypothesis that the strength of amygdala-based network connectivity would predict social network size and complexity over and above amygdala volume, thereby significantly extending our findings from Bickart et al. (2011).
Materials and Methods
Experiment 1: identifying amygdala-based intrinsic connectivity networks
Participants
Discovery sample.
The discovery sample consisted of 89 young adults (45 females, age M = 22.4, SD = 3.34, range = 18–33 years) (Yeo et al., 2011; Touroutoglou et al., 2012). Participants were included if they were right-handed, native English speakers with normal or corrected-to-normal vision and reported no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders with a confirmed absence of DSM-IV Axis I diagnoses using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV and were also free of psychoactive medications and had a verbal IQ ≥ 97, performance IQ ≥ 98, and full-scale IQ ≥ 98 as measured by the American National Adult Reading Test. Each participant gave written informed consent in accordance with institutional Human Subjects Research Committee guidelines.
Replication sample.
The replication sample consisted of 83 young adults (53 females, age M = 23.6, SD = 3.13, range = 18–35 years). All participants in this sample fulfilled the same inclusion criteria and consent procedures as the discovery sample.
Amygdala-based networks important for social connectedness
Based on our synthesis of tract-tracing work in rodents and monkeys (Haber and Knutson, 2010; Price and Drevets, 2010; Barbas et al., 2011) and human functional neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies in social cognition (Moll et al., 2005; Lieberman, 2007; Adolphs, 2009; Rilling and Sanfey, 2011), we developed a hypothetical neuroanatomical framework in which separate subregions of the amygdala each anchor a large-scale network of brain regions subserving distinct processes of social cognition (Fig. 1). Specifically, we hypothesized a network supporting social perception would be anchored in the ventrolateral sector of the amygdala, which contains nuclei that share anatomical connections with sensory association areas of the temporal cortex and OFC (Aggleton et al., 1980; Barbas and De Olmos, 1990; Carmichael and Price, 1995; Ghashghaei and Barbas, 2002; Höistad and Barbas, 2008). These regions are implicated in decoding and interpreting social signals from others in the context of past experience and current goals (Morris et al., 1996; Allison et al., 2000; Hart et al., 2000; Phelps et al., 2000; George et al., 2001; Cunningham et al., 2004; Gobbini and Haxby, 2006; Richeson et al., 2008). We hypothesized a network supporting social affiliation would be anchored in the medial sector of the amygdala, which contains nuclei that share anatomical connections with mesolimbic, reward-related areas of the vmPFC, medial temporal lobe, and ventromedial striatum and hypothalamus (McDonald, 1987, 1991a,b; Kunishio and Haber, 1994; Carmichael and Price, 1996; An et al., 1998; Ongür et al., 1998, 2003; Ferry et al., 2000; Ongür and Price, 2000; Fudge et al., 2002; Kondo et al., 2003, 2005; Haber et al., 2006; Hsu and Price, 2007; Price, 2007; Saleem et al., 2008; Haber and Calzavara, 2009; Haber and Knutson, 2010; Price and Drevets, 2010). These regions are implicated in motivating prosocial or affiliative behaviors, such as cooperating with a trustworthy partner or comforting a loved one (Bartels and Zeki, 2004; Rilling et al., 2004; Delgado et al., 2005; Moll et al., 2006, 2007; Harbaugh et al., 2007; Tabibnia et al., 2008; Li et al., 2009; Zahn et al., 2009; Izuma et al., 2010). Finally, we hypothesized a network supporting social aversion would be anchored in the dorsal sector of the amygdala, which contains nuclei that share anatomical connections with interoceptive, pain-sensitive areas of the caudal ACC (cACC), insula, and ventrolateral striatum, hypothalamus, and brainstem (Mufson et al., 1981; McDonald, 1987, 1991a,b; Fudge et al., 2002; Stefanacci and Amaral, 2002). These regions are implicated in motivating avoidant behaviors, such as rejecting cooperation with an unfair partner or avoiding a seemingly untrustworthy stranger (Phillips et al., 1997; Winston et al., 2002; Eisenberger et al., 2003; Sanfey et al., 2003; Moll et al., 2006, 2007; Cheng et al., 2007; Buckholtz et al., 2008; Rilling et al., 2008; Zahn et al., 2009; Kross et al., 2011).
Hypothetical topographic model of amygdala subregions and their affiliated large-scale networks subserving social cognition. A schematic of (a) the amygdala subregions in coronal view that we hypothesize are anchors for (b) three large-scale networks subserving processes important for social cognition. Ins, insula; SS, somatosensory operculum; dTP, dorsal temporal pole; cACC, caudal anterior cingulate cortex; rACC, rostral anterior cingulate cortex; sgACC, subgenual anterior cingulate cortex; MTL, medial temporal lobe; FG, fusiform gyrus; vTP, ventral temporal pole; vlSt, ventrolateral striatum; vmSt, ventromedial striatum.
Structural MRI data acquisition
Imaging data for the discovery and replication samples were collected on a 3T Magnetom Tim Trio system at Massachusetts General Hospital (Siemens), using a 12-channel phased-array head coil. Structural MRI data for the discovery sample were acquired using a T1-weighted 3D MPRAGE sequence (TR/TE/flip angle = 2.20 s/1.54 ms/7°, resolution = 1.2 mm isotropic). Structural MRI data for the replication sample were acquired using two similar T1-weighted 3D MPRAGE sequences (n = 55: TR/TE/flip angle = 2.30 s/2.98 ms/9°, resolution = 1 mm isotropic; n = 28: TR/TE/flip angle = 2.53 s/3.48 ms/7°, resolution = 1 mm isotropic).
Resting-state fMRI data acquisition and preprocessing
fMRI data for the discovery sample were acquired during rest using a gradient-echo, echo-planar sequence sensitive to blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) contrast (discovery sample: TR = 3000 ms; TE = 30 ms; flip angle = 85°, 47 slices; acquisition voxel = 3 mm isotropic; replication sample: TR = 5000 ms; TE = 30 ms; flip angle = 90°, 55 slices, acquisition voxel = 2 mm isotropic). During all resting-state fMRI runs, participants in both samples were directed to keep their eyes open without fixating and to remain as still as possible. In both samples, resting-state fMRI runs were interleaved with task-based fMRI runs, which are unrelated to this study.
Next, resting-state data were preprocessed using a series of algorithms. After removing the first four functional volumes, the following steps were completed: correction for slice-dependent time shifts (SPM2, Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK), correction for head motion with rigid-body transformation in three translation and three rotations (FMRIB, Oxford, UK), spatial normalization to Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) atlas space, resampling to 2 mm isotropic voxels, spatial smoothing using a 6 mm full width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel, and temporal bandpass filtering to remove frequencies >0.08 Hz. We then removed sources of spurious variance and their temporal derivatives from the data through linear regression (six parameters derived from the rigid-body head motion correction, the signal averaged over the whole brain, the signal averaged over a region within the deep white matter, and the signal averaged over the ventricles) and the residual BOLD time course was retained for functional connectivity analysis.
Resting-state fMRI connectivity analysis
Delineating connectional subregions of the amygdala.
In validating fcMRI results against nonhuman animal tract-tracing studies, the topography of putative origins and terminations of large-scale networks is often defined using an iterative seed-target-seed approach (Vincent et al., 2006, 2008; Yeo et al., 2011). Using a similar logic, we first tested our hypothetical topographic model of the functional-anatomic organization of brain networks subserving social cognition (Fig. 1).
We identified three brain ROIs outside the amygdala that represent core nodes within each of our three hypothesized networks: lateral OFC (lOFC: MNI coordinates +/− 38, 34, −18), vmPFC (0, 32, −12), and cACC (0, 16, 32), anchoring the perception, affiliation, and aversion networks, respectively (see Fig. 3a). We chose these areas because they are each heavily interconnected with one of the amygdala's subregions and other regions within each respective network (Haber and Knutson, 2010; Price and Drevets, 2010; Barbas et al., 2011).
We selected the MNI coordinates of each seed ROI within each anatomically defined cortical region based on the approximate location of peak voxels derived from a resting-state functional connectivity map of a whole amygdala seed. To generate the resting-state functional connectivity map of the whole amygdala seed, we computed a Pearson's product moment correlation coefficient, r, between fluctuations in BOLD signal within the left and right whole amygdala, averaged between the hemispheres, and all other voxels in the brain for each participant in the discovery sample. We defined the whole amygdala using probabilistic maps from the Harvard–Oxford Subcortical Structural Atlas available for FSL, only including voxels that had 25% or greater probability of being labeled as the amygdala (left: 3368 mm3, right: 3944 mm3). The resultant correlation maps were then converted to z (r) values, which are unbiased estimators of the population correlation coefficients, using Fisher's r-to-z transformation. We conducted a one-sample group mean analysis using the FreeSurfer general linear model command (mri_glmfit with the –osgm flag), which tests whether the correlation z (r) value in each voxel is significantly >0 for the group, producing a group-level statistical significance map (Fig. 2). Next, we searched this significance map to select MNI coordinates approximating voxels of peak significance within each cortical region of interest.
Topography of intrinsic connectivity for the whole amygdala in the discovery sample (N = 89). Intrinsic connectivity statistical significance maps are displayed for left and right amygdala seed regions in the discovery sample. Significance maps were thresholded at p < 0.01, averaged across hemisphere for display purposes, and overlaid on Freesurfer's “fsaverage” surface template (a) and a T1 MNI152 2 mm template brain in radiologic convention (b).
We then created spherical seed ROIs, 3 mm in radius, around each peak voxel and computed a Pearson's product moment correlation coefficient, r, between fluctuations in BOLD signal within these cortical seeds and all voxels in the brain for each participant. We converted the resultant correlation maps to z (r) values and conducted contrast analyses on the z (r) maps from each cortical seed ROI (i.e., cACC > vmPFC and lOFC; vmPFC > cACC and lOFC; lOFC > vmPFC and cACC). Finally, we assigned each voxel within the amygdala (within the Harvard–Oxford 25% probability maps of the whole amygdala) to the cortical region with which it shared the strongest connectivity. We also conducted this analysis using data processed with 2 and 4 mm smoothing kernels to determine whether this would enable finer grained parcellation of amygdala subregions. Because both kernels produced virtually equivalent results to the data smoothed with a 6 mm kernel, we only present the latter results.
Delineating large-scale networks connected to each amygdala subregion.
Next, we constructed spherical seed ROIs, 2 mm in radius, approximating the peak voxel location within each amygdala subregion (making sure that the spheres were centered in the amygdala): dorsal amygdala subregion (MNI coordinates +/−22, −4, −12), medial amygdala subregion (+/−14, −4, −20), and ventrolateral amygdala subregion (+/− 28, −4, −22). We used these seed ROIs to produce resting-state functional connectivity maps by computing Pearson's product moment correlation coefficient, r, between fluctuations in BOLD signal within each amygdala subregion seed ROI, averaged between hemispheres, and all other voxels in the brain for each participant in our discovery sample. We also analyzed the data using the larger subregions of the amygdala as seeds, and found that this method produces virtually equivalent maps (data not shown) to that of the smaller spherical seeds, although there is more overlap between the maps, which is to be expected as a result of autocorrelation effects of adjacent voxels at the borders of the subregions. Thus, we chose to present the maps derived from the smaller spherical seeds. We then converted the resultant correlation maps to z (r) values and conducted a one-sample group mean generalized linear model (GLM) as above. We binarized each amygdala subregion's significance map at p < 10−5 (uncorrected for multiple comparisons with a cluster size constraint of 10 contiguous voxels) to explore the overlapping and unique topography of functional connectivity associated with each subregion. Each of the three maps (excluding voxels that fell within the amygdala ROI) was used as a mask in the brain-behavior analysis below.
Replication of amygdala subregions and networks in an independent sample
To examine the reliability of our findings in the discovery sample, we conducted additional analyses in the independent replication sample (n = 83) with higher resolution scans (2 mm isotropic). For these analyses, we used the same procedures as described for our discovery sample. First, we used the three cortical seed regions (lOFC, vmPFC, cACC) to identify voxels with the strongest connectivity within the amygdala, aiming to replicate the connectional parcellation of the amygdala into subregions. Next, we used this analysis to generate three seeds within the amygdala and then performed an exploratory whole-brain analysis to identify maps of regions sharing functional connectivity with each amygdala seed. In addition to visual inspection of the results, we quantified the similarity of each of the three connectivity maps in the discovery sample to the map from the same seed in the replication sample, calculated using η2 as described in detail previously (Cohen et al., 2008). This is a measure calculated from a pair of maps and indicates the degree of similarity between two maps, with values ranging from 0 (not at all similar) to 1 (identical). The formula computes, on a point-by-point basis, the fraction of the variance in one measure that is accounted for by the variance in another measure.
Experiment 2: amygdala connectivity and social network size
Participants
A third independent sample consisting of 30 young adults (19 females, age M = 24.2, SD = 2.89, range = 19–32 years) was used for the brain-behavior analyses reported in Experiment 2. Using standard self-report measures, participants described their social network size and complexity (Cohen et al., 1997), satisfaction with life (Diener et al., 1985), and availability of social support (Russell et al., 1984). Each also completed structural and resting-state MRI scans. One female participant was excluded from brain-behavior analyses because her computed social network size was 3 SDs greater than the group mean. Amygdala volume and self-report data from 19 participants have been previously published (Bickart et al., 2011). The remaining participants were run after the publication of Bickart et al. (2011). All participants in this sample fulfilled the same inclusion criteria and consent procedures as the two samples reported as part of Experiment 1.
Self-report data acquisition
To assess the size and complexity of participants' social networks in our brain-behavior sample, we used the Social Network Index (SNI) (Cohen et al., 1997), a 13-item questionnaire containing two subscales of interest. The Number of People in Social Network Subscale indexed the size of participants' social networks by counting the total number of people that they interacted with at least once every 2 weeks. The Number of Embedded Networks Subscale indexed the complexity of participants' social networks by counting the total number of different groups with at least four members with whom each participant regularly interacted (e.g., family, friends, church/temple, school, work, neighbors, volunteering, and others). The Number of High-Contact Roles Subscale indexed the diversity of participants' social networks by counting the number of roles that the participant, himself or herself, performed (e.g., mother, child, employer, etc.). Scoring procedures for these subscales can be found online (http://www.psy.cmu.edu/∼scohen/SNIscore.html). As control variables, we also measured participants' reported levels of social support and life satisfaction. We used the Social Provisions Scale (Russell et al., 1984) to assess perceived availability of social support based on participants' views of their current relationships. The scale consists of 24 items (e.g., “I feel part of a group of people who share my attitudes and beliefs.”) rated on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). The Social Provisions Scale provides a summary score as well as a score for six provisions of social relationships including guidance (advice or information), reliable alliance (assurance that others can be counted on in times of stress), reassurance of worth (recognition of one's competence), attachment (emotional closeness), social integration (a sense of belonging to a group of friends), and opportunity for nurturance (providing assistance to others). We used the Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener et al., 1985) to assess participants' global life satisfaction. The scale consists of five items (e.g., “So far I have gotten the important things I want in life.”) rated on a scale from 1 (strongly agree) to 7 (strongly disagree) and provides an overall life satisfaction score.
Structural MRI data acquisition and morphometric analysis
Imaging data for the brain-behavior sample were collected on a 3T Magnetom Tim Trio system at Massachusetts General Hospital (Siemens), using a 12-channel phased-array head coil. Structural MRI data were acquired using a T1-weighted 3D MPRAGE sequence (TR/TE/flip angle = 2.53 s/3.5 ms/7°, resolution = 1 mm isotropic). To measure amygdala volume, we used FreeSurfer's (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu) automated segmentation method, which employs a manually labeled atlas dataset from 40 individuals to automatically segment and assign neuroanatomic ROI labels to 40 different brain structures (including the amygdala) based on probabilistic estimations. Corrections were made for differences in head size by dividing each participant's amygdala volume by estimated total intracranial volume (Buckner et al., 2004). This automated segmentation procedure has been widely used in volumetric studies and was shown to be comparable in accuracy to that of manual labeling (Fischl et al., 2002) and is reliable across sessions within the same scanner (Jovicich et al., 2009). In the present study, each anatomic dataset was processed using the fully automated algorithm and then the amygdala segmentation was manually verified. A trained operator, blind to the hypothesis, manually inspected the results of the automated segmentation. In the present study, no adjustments, modifications, or edits were made; the results of the automated segmentation were verified as accurate without need for correction. The criteria used for this inspection with regard to the amygdala are an in-house laboratory manual of the boundaries of the amygdala (Wright et al., 2006; Entis et al., 2012).
Resting-state fMRI data acquisition and preprocessing
fMRI data for the brain-behavior sample were acquired during rest using a gradient-echo, echo-planar sequence sensitive to BOLD contrast (128 contiguous volumes; TR = 2000 ms; TE = 30 ms; flip angle = 90°, 33 slices, matrix = 64 × 64; FOV = 200 mm; acquisition voxel = 3.1 × 3.1 × 5.0). During all resting-state fMRI runs, participants were directed to keep their eyes open without fixating and to remain as still as possible. Resting-state fMRI runs were interleaved with task-based fMRI runs, which are unrelated to this study. All preprocessing procedures were identical to that of the discovery and replication samples.
A priori ROI-based calculation of connectivity strengths in amygdala-based networks
For brain-behavior analyses in Experiment 2, we measured the strength of intrinsic connectivity between each of the three amygdala subregions and an average of the signal from all the voxels within its respective large-scale network mask defined independently in the discovery and replication samples. This resulted in three amygdala-network intrinsic connectivity strength (z (r)) values for each participant: z (r)ventrolateral amygdala subregion-perception network, z (r)medial amygdala subregion-affiliation network, and z (r)dorsal amygdala subregion-aversion network.
Amygdala-network intrinsic connectivity and social network size analysis
To test the hypothesis that individual differences in the strength of intrinsic amygdala-network connectivity predict larger social network size, over and above amygdala volume, we conducted a series of linear regression analyses using both amygdala volume (adjusted for intracranial volume) and intrinsic connectivity strength as independent variables and social network size as the dependent variable. For each of the two networks that produced significant results in this hypothesis-driven analysis, we also explored the localization of amygdala subregion connectivity within each large-scale network that best predicted social network size. Using FreeSurfer's implementation of GLM analysis, we entered social network size values as the independent variable and the amygdala-network connectivity strength values as the dependent variable. The resultant map was masked by the network mask defined in the discovery sample as above and results were considered significant if they met the criteria of p < 0.01 with a cluster size constraint of 10 contiguous voxels.
Finally, to assess the discriminant validity of the a priori hypothesized relationships, we tested whether the strength of connectivity within two social-relevant but nonamygdala-based networks (serving as controls for the prior analyses) predicted variance in social network size and complexity. We defined a mentalizing network composed of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (MNI coordinates 0, 50, 24), precuneus (0, −64, 40), and temporoparietal junction (+/− 50, −58, 24) and a mirror network composed of the ventral premotor cortex (+/− 40, 4, 44), posterior STS (+/− 50, −58, 8), and intraparietal sulcus (+/− 40, −44, 46) using coordinates derived from a recent meta-analysis of mentalizing and mirroring tasks in fMRI studies (Van Overwalle and Baetens, 2009). We created spherical nodes, 3 mm in radius, around each of these MNI coordinates and computed pairwise correlations z (r) between the averaged BOLD signal time course in each node. We then computed a composite connectivity strength for each of these networks by averaging across all pairwise z (r) correlations between nodal pairs within the network. We only included nodal pairs that demonstrated pairwise z (r) correlations that were reliably greater than zero in both samples (n = 89 and n = 29) in the calculation of composite connectivity strength scores.
As another test of discriminant validity, we examined whether the strength of connectivity in any of the amygdala networks correlated with other self-report measures of sociality including perceived social support and life satisfaction.
Brain-behavior analyses were conducted using PASW Statistics 18, Release Version 18.0.0 (SPSS, 2009, Chicago, IL, www.spss.com). For these analyses, we selected an α of 0.05.
Results
Experiment 1: identifying amygdala-based intrinsic connectivity networks
As predicted, using the three cortical ROIs as seed regions, the connectional analysis revealed three voxel clusters within the amygdala (Fig. 3b). As predicted, we found the strongest connectivity between the lOFC and the ventrolateral amygdala, the vmPFC and the medial amygdala, and the cACC with the dorsal amygdala. These connectionally defined amygdala subregions strongly resemble the subregions of the amygdala depicted in Figure 1a as well as cytoarchitectonically defined nuclear groups of the amygdala (Fig. 4).
Three connectionally defined subregions of the amygdala. a, A priori seed regions were placed within the vmPFC, cACC, and lOFC. b, c, Each voxel in the amygdala was assigned to the seed region with which it demonstrated the strongest connectivity in the discovery sample (N = 89). Connectivity data are overlaid on a T1 MNI152 2 mm template brain in radiologic convention (b) and depicted in a 3D rendering (C).
Connectionally defined amygdala subregions from this study and cytoarchitectonically defined amygdala subregions from the Juelich Histological Atlas. Connectionally defined amygdala subregions from our study (a) and cytoarchitectonically defined amygdala subregions obtained from FSL's Juelich Histological Atlas (b) are overlaid on coronal slices of a T1 MNI152 2 mm template brain in radiologic convention. MNI coordinates can be seen in the middle of the figure. The cytoarchitectonically defined subregions are based on probabilistic maps created by Amunts et al. (2005). They include only the voxels that had at least a 50% chance of being assigned to one nuclear group (laterobasal, yellow; superficial, red; centromedial, blue), as used in a previous fcMRI study of these amygdala subregions (Roy et al., 2009).
Using spherical seed ROIs placed within each amygdala subregion (Fig. 5a), we next delineated partially distinct large-scale intrinsic functional connectivity maps (Fig. 5b–d) for a network supporting social perception, a network supporting social affiliation, and a network supporting social aversion; these networks largely resemble the hypothesized networks derived from animal tract-tracing studies and human task-related fMRI studies (Fig. 1b). As predicted, the ventrolateral amygdala subregion showed strongest connectivity with areas important for perceptual processes including the fusiform gyrus and neighboring areas of the ventromedial temporal cortex extending to the pole, as well as the rostral STS, and caudal, medial, and lateral OFC. The medial amygdala subregion showed strongest connectivity with limbic areas important for affiliative behaviors including the vmPFC and neighboring subgenual and rostral ACC, the ventromedial striatum localized largely in the nucleus accumbens, and the ventromedial hypothalamus. Finally, the dorsal amygdala subregion showed strongest connectivity with areas important for aversion behaviors including the cACC, the insula and somatosensory operculum, the ventrolateral striatum localized in the putamen, the caudolateral hypothalamus, and regions in the thalamus and brainstem.
Each amygdala subregion anchors one of three distinct large-scale corticolimbic networks. a, Seeds are localized within the connectionally defined amygdala subregions shown in Figure 2. b–d, One sample group mean statistical significance map for each amygdala seed is displayed in standard views (b) as well as views highlighting specific cortical (c) and subcortical brain regions in the discovery sample (N = 89) (d). The maps are binarized at p < 10−5 and overlaid on a T1 MNI152 0.5 mm template brain in radiologic convention to demonstrate the distinct and shared connectivity across maps. A color key is displayed at the bottom of the figure. cACC, caudal anterior cingulate cortex; Ins, insula; SS, somatosensory operculum; TP, temporal pole; FG, fusiform gyrus; MTL, medial temporal lobe.
We replicated all three amygdala subregions and networks in our independent replication sample. The three initial cortical seed regions (lOFC, vmPFC, and cACC) identified clusters of voxels within the amygdala that were highly similar to those identified within our discovery sample (Fig. 6a,b). Seeds within these three subregions produced large-scale connectivity maps in the replication sample that were reliable with those identified within the discovery sample (Fig. 6c,d). The η2 coefficients were 0.88, 0.82, and 0.86 for lateral, medial, and dorsal amygdala seeds, respectively.
Replication of amygdala subregions and networks in an independent sample (N = 83). a, A priori seed regions were placed within the vmPFC, cACC, and lOFC. b, Each voxel in the amygdala was assigned to the seed region with which it demonstrated the strongest connectivity in the replication sample (N = 83). The cluster maps are overlaid on a T1 MNI152 2 mm template brain in radiologic convention. c, Seeds within the connectionally defined amygdala subregions. d, One sample group mean statistical significance maps for each amygdala seed displayed in standard views. The maps are binarized at p < 10−5 and overlaid on a MNI152 T1 1.0 mm template brain in radiologic convention. A color key is displayed at the bottom of the figure.
Experiment 2: amygdala connectivity and social network size
As hypothesized, individual differences in the strength of connectivity within the networks supporting social perception and affiliation predicted social network size over and above variations in amygdala volume (which predicted 15% of the variance in social network size). The results demonstrated that participants with stronger amygdala connectivity within these networks had larger social networks relative to those individuals with weaker connectivity within these networks (Fig. 7b). Individual differences in the strength of the amygdala's connectivity within the network supporting social aversion did not predict differences in social network size (Fig. 7b), however. Furthermore, using a multiple linear regression analysis, we found that stronger amygdala connectivity within the networks supporting social perception and affiliation each contributed independently to larger social network size (along with amygdala volume), predicting a total of 41% of its variance (see Table 1). We found similar patterns of results for individual differences in the complexity participants' social networks (i.e., the number of groups in which participants have at least four network members) and the diversity of their networks (i.e., the number of roles participants play within their networks); this is not surprising given that both social network complexity and diversity were strongly correlated with social network size (Tables 2, 3).
A larger social network is predicted by stronger connectivity between amygdala subregions and corticolimbic regions important for perception and affiliative behavior. a, Each amygdala subregion and its intrinsic connectivity network were independently defined in the discovery sample (shown here) and then used in the brain-behavior sample to compute the connectivity strength between each amygdala subregion and the rest of the network. b, Scatter plots show that social network size (y-axis) is predicted by the strength of connectivity between two of the three amygdala subregions and their respective networks (x-axis), over and above amygdala volume, in the brain-behavior sample. *p < 0.05; †p = 0.06; **the x-axis displays the residual variance in the strength of the resting-state connectivity measure (Fisher's r-to-z) after partialling out its shared variance with amygdala volume.
Larger amygdala volume and stronger intrinsic connectivity of the ventrolateral and medial amygdala subregions each contributed independently to larger social network size in our brain-behavior sample (N = 29)
Larger amygdala volumes and stronger intrinsic connectivity for the ventrolateral and medial amygdala subregions each contributed independently to more complex social networks in our brain-behavior sample (N = 29)
Larger amygdala volumes and stronger intrinsic functional connectivity for the ventrolateral amygdala subregion each contributed independently to more diverse social networks in our brain-behavior sample (N = 29)
We next explored the specific regions within the networks supporting social perception and affiliation that were driving the relationship between intrinsic connectivity and social network size. As seen in Figure 8, people with larger social networks had stronger connectivity between the ventrolateral amygdala and the STS and fusiform gyrus within the network supporting social perception. They also had stronger connectivity between the medial amygdala and the vmPFC within the network supporting social affiliation. See Table 4 for MNI coordinates of these and additional regions that demonstrated correlations with social network size.
Exploratory analyses revealed that the connectivity between the amygdala and specific regions within the networks supporting social perception and affiliation are the best predictors of social network size. a, Brain images show location of voxels within the medial and ventrolateral amygdala's intrinsic connectivity networks (defined in the discovery sample) that correlated with social network size at p < 0.01 in the brain-behavior sample, uncorrected with a cluster size constraint of 10 voxels. Color bars indicate the p values (10−2–10−4) of correlated voxels, which are overlaid on slices of a T1 MNI152 0.5 mm template brain in radiologic convention. b, Scatter plots show the effects for peak voxels with the strength of intrinsic connectivity on the y-axis and social network size on the x-axis; **p < 0.001.
Regions within the ventrolateral and medial amygdala's intrinsic connectivity networks (defined in the discovery sample) that correlated with social network size in an exploratory GLM analysis in our brain-behavior sample (N = 29)
A final set of analyses characterized the anatomical and behavioral specificity of the results above. Intrinsic connectivity within two networks important for social cognition, but not involving the amygdala—the mentalizing and mirror networks—was not related to either social network size or complexity (r = 0.01–0.02; p > 0.3). Intrinsic amygdala connectivity was not related to other self-report measures of sociality, perceived social support or life satisfaction (r = −0.28–0.16; p > 0.15).
Discussion
Amygdala-based intrinsic connectivity networks
In this paper, we used resting-state fcMRI data in humans to refine our understanding of the topography of amygdala connectivity and investigate its relationship to social network size. In the first experiment, using a priori predictions derived from anatomical organization of the amygdala's connectivity in rodents and monkeys (Haber and Knutson, 2010; Price and Drevets, 2010; Barbas et al., 2011), we parsed the amygdala's intrinsic connectivity into three partially distinct large-scale networks. Specifically, we found distinct connectivity between three major subdivisions of the amygdala and limbic cortical structures as well as the ventral striatum, hypothalamus, and brainstem. This is congruent with prior work that directly demonstrated a correspondence between the topography of resting-state functional connectivity and anatomical connectivity in monkeys (Vincent et al., 2007). Moreover, our data-driven approach extends previous studies that have used similar approaches including tract-tracing in animals (McDonald, 1991a,b) and diffusion tensor imaging in humans (Bach et al., 2011; Saygin et al., 2011) to delineate subregions of the amygdala based on connectional profiles. The overall topography of the three amygdala-based networks identified here converges with and builds on previous human resting-state functional connectivity studies of the amygdala (Roy et al., 2009; van Marle et al., 2010; Kim et al., 2011).
Amygdala connectivity and social network size
A growing body of work is revealing that resting-state intrinsic connectivity reflects functional properties of the brain that relate to individual differences in a variety of abilities and behaviors (Fox et al., 2007; Seeley et al., 2007; van Marle et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2010; Touroutoglou et al., 2012). To date, no study has yet investigated whether people with larger social networks possess stronger connectivity between brain regions subserving adaptive social behaviors, although Sallet et al. (2011) did report that monkeys housed with larger (versus smaller) cohorts had stronger intrinsic connectivity between the STS and ACC; connectivity of the amygdala was not reported in that study. In the second experiment reported here, we discovered that people who fostered and maintained larger and more complex social networks not only had larger amygdala volumes, but also had stronger intrinsic connectivity between the amygdala and regions of the brain implicated in perceptual and affiliative, but not avoidant, aspects of social cognition. This finding suggests that the dorsal amygdala (associated with the aversion network, putatively motivating decisions about who to avoid, punish, or reject) might not be directly relevant to social network size or complexity. Stronger intrinsic connectivity between the amygdala and regions in this network has been recently observed in healthy participants in the acute aftermath of stress induction (van Marle et al., 2010), suggesting that the amygdala's role in social cognition and anxiety might be separable.
Our network and region-level findings for the network supporting social perception are consistent with a growing body of neuroimaging and neuropsychological work that implicates regions within this network in processing social cues, such as socially salient features in the human face including facial expressions, racial identity, and trustworthiness (Morris et al., 1996; Phelps et al., 2000; Winston et al., 2002; Cunningham et al., 2004). Although we did not directly test the functional role of this network, in the context of this prior work, our findings suggest that people with stronger intrinsic amygdala connectivity within the perception network, particularly with the STS and fusiform gyrus, might be better at detecting and decoding the meaning of these social cues and thus better able to navigate the dynamic and often-ambiguous nature of social interactions with more people in more social contexts. In line with this interpretation, findings from recent functional neuroimaging studies suggest that the amygdala plays a modulatory role within this network, capable of enhancing neural responses in visual areas and perceptual ability for affect-laden stimuli (Duncan and Barrett, 2007; Pessoa, 2011). For example, enhanced amygdala activity is linked to increased visual acuity (Lim et al., 2009) and greater visual cortex activation including area V1 (Padmala and Pessoa, 2008). Similarly, the normal enhancement of fMRI signal in the fusiform gyrus and STS to affective facial expressions is reduced in amygdala-damaged patients compared with controls (Vuilleumier et al., 2004). The magnitude of this reduction correlates with the degree of amygdalar damage.
Our network and region-level findings for the network supporting social affiliation are consistent with a growing body of neuroimaging and neuropsychological work in human social and moral judgment and decision making that implicates regions within this network in processing socially rewarding stimuli, generating sentiments of social attachment, and motivating prosocial behaviors involved in cooperation, trust, and altruism (Moll et al., 2005; Rilling and Sanfey, 2011). Although we did not directly test the functional role of this network, in the context of this prior work, our findings suggest that people with enhanced functional connectivity between the amygdala and this circuitry, particularly the vmPFC, might derive more value from connecting with others, which would motivate them to form and maintain more social relationships. In line with this interpretation, people who place a higher premium on connecting with others, or exhibit a heightened “propensity to connect,” tend to have larger social networks (Totterdell et al., 2008). Also supporting this interpretation, a recent study found that bonobos, characterized by their cooperative social nature, had larger tracts measured with diffusion-tensor imaging between the amygdala and the vmPFC than chimpanzees who are a more aggressive primate species (Rilling et al., 2012).
Furthermore, the link between intrinsic connectivity and social network size/complexity was anatomically specific to corticolimbic networks including the amygdala; we found no link between social network characteristics and intrinsic connectivity strength in the mentalizing and mirror networks that have been implicated in social cognition, but which do not routinely include the amygdala or other affective circuitry. This dissociation underscores the value of studying the component processes that contribute to social connectedness since there are clearly important divisions of labor. In this case, the size and complexity of a person's social network depends more on corticolimbic circuitry that is important for affective processing (Barrett and Bar, 2009), which in part evaluates the salience of signals from other people (Seeley et al., 2007), than on corticocortical networks that have more limited relevance for affective processing.
Conclusions
Humans with amygdalae that are more strongly connected to brain regions important for social perception and affiliation also have larger and more complex social networks. These findings begin to suggest the mechanisms that support larger and more complex social networks. More connected individuals might be better equipped to perceive social cues like facial expression and be more motivated to or receive more reward from responding to these cues in a manner that promotes social affiliation. A limitation in the present investigation and all similar human studies to date is that their design precludes causal inferences: we do not yet know whether these structural and functional properties of the social brain are inborn and thus endow an individual with the propensity to be more gregarious or whether they are potentially modifiable by experience. A recent study in monkeys suggests that brain structure changes with social experience (Sallet et al., 2011), although this conclusion is not firm because the monkeys were not randomly assigned to cages for living groups of different sizes. A parsing of social function into specific processes subserved by distinct brain networks will enable future research to focus on how these psychological processes and their neural correlates not only differ among healthy adults but also how they fail to develop or disintegrate in neuropsychiatric conditions marked by social impairment like autism, antisocial personality disorder, and frontotemporal dementia.
Footnotes
This study was supported by grants from the U.S. National Institutes of Health Director's Pioneer Award (DP1OD003312) and the U.S. National Institute on Aging (R01-AG030311, R01-AG029411 and P50-AG005134). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health or the National Institute on Aging. We thank Randy Buckner for providing the data used in the analysis of the discovery sample and the tools used for fcMRI preprocessing and Rebecca Dautoff for assistance.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Brad Dickerson, MGH Frontotemporal Disorders Unit, 149 13th Street, Suite 2691, Charlestown, MA 02129. bradd{at}nmr.mgh.harvard.edu