Abstract
Neural synchronization between distant cell assemblies is crucial for the formation of new memories. To date, however, it remains unclear whether higher-order brain regions can adaptively regulate neural synchrony to control memory processing in humans. We explored this question in two experiments using a voluntary forgetting task. In the first experiment, we simultaneously recorded electroencephalography along with fMRI. The results show that a reduction in neural synchrony goes hand-in-hand with a BOLD signal increase in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) when participants are cued to forget previously studied information. In the second experiment, we directly stimulated the left dlPFC with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation during the same task, and show that such stimulation specifically boosts the behavioral forgetting effect and induces a reduction in neural synchrony. These results suggest that prefrontally driven downregulation of long-range neural synchronization mediates goal-directed forgetting of long-term memories.
Introduction
Memories are thought to be stored within synaptic connections among widespread cortical networks (Fuster, 1997), with the strength of these connections being modified by neural synchrony (Markram et al., 1997). Phase synchronization establishes communication between distant brain areas (Fries, 2005), presumably shaping neural plasticity by facilitating long-term potentiation (Buzsáki, 2006). Consistently, previous electrophysiological studies in humans have reported enhanced phase synchronization in memory tasks to be associated with memory formation (Fell et al., 2001; Summerfield and Mangels, 2005; Fell and Axmacher, 2011). However, it is unknown whether long-range synchronization can be regulated by higher-order brain regions in a voluntary, task-relevant manner. We here investigate the impact of the prefrontal cortex on neural synchrony during voluntary forgetting.
Although forgetting is usually viewed as a failure of memory, it can help us to remove outdated or unwanted information to free up memory capacity, rendering our memory system flexible and adaptive (Bjork, 1989; Levy and Anderson, 2002). Indeed, people can intentionally forget episodic memories when cued to do so, as is shown in the directed-forgetting task (Bjork, 1970). In the list method of this task, participants study two lists of items and receive a cue to either forget or continue to remember the preceding list (Fig. 1a). On a later memory test, participants are asked to recall all of the previously presented items, including to-be-forgotten items. During study of the first list, participants do not know whether the items of the list are to be forgotten later. Even so, the forget cue impairs recall of the items of this list, reflecting goal-directed forgetting of the obsolete List 1 information. A previous study found directed forgetting to be reflected by a sustained decrease in phase synchronization in the upper alpha–lower beta frequency range (11–13 Hz) during second-list encoding (Bäuml et al., 2008). Additionally, the forget cue also improves recall of the new, second-list items, a behavioral effect that has been linked to stimulus-induced alpha power decreases (Bäuml et al., 2008; Pastötter et al., 2008). Consistent with the cognitive literature, this improvement effect therefore appears to be dissociable from the forgetting effect (Sahakyan and Delaney, 2003; Pastötter and Bäuml, 2010).
Lesion and fMRI studies suggest that memory control depends on the engagement of the dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC) (Conway and Fthenaki, 2003; Anderson et al., 2004; Depue, 2012). Because voluntary forgetting requires control over contents in memory, we performed two experiments to test whether the dorsolateral PFC mediates voluntary forgetting specifically by downregulating long-range neural synchrony. In the first experiment, EEG was recorded simultaneously with fMRI while participants performed a directed-forgetting task. In the second experiment, repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS), a technique used to directly stimulate specific brain regions, was applied at the dlPFC during the same forgetting task while recording the EEG. If indeed dlPFC downregulates neural synchrony to induce voluntary forgetting, (1) enhanced activity in the dlPFC should go hand-in-hand with a reduction in neural synchrony, and (2) stimulation of the dlPFC should specifically modulate neural synchrony and the behavioral forgetting effect.
Materials and Methods
Participants (Experiments 1 and 2).
In the first (EEG–fMRI) experiment, 22 healthy participants (mean age, 23.05 years; range, 20–29; 7 males) remained in the analysis after excluding 2 participants due to a too high number of EEG artifacts. In the second (rTMS–EEG) experiment, 44 participants (mean age, 22.2; range, 18–28; 18 males) remained in the analysis, after excluding 4 participants due to poor EEG data. Subjects were randomly assigned to the dlPFC or vertex (control) stimulation group, with 22 subjects remaining in each group (dlPFC: mean age, 21.95 years; range, 18–28; 10 males; vertex: mean age, 22.41 years; range, 18–27; 8 males). All participants gave their written informed consent, and the experimental protocol was approved by the local ethical review board.
Material and procedure (Experiments 1 and 2).
As study material, 240 words were drawn from the Medical Research Council Psycholinguisitic Database (Coltheart, 1981) and translated into German. The word material was split into 24 lists of 10 words each. The lists were matched according to word frequency (mean, 52.95; SD, 51.12), number of letters (mean, 5.36; SD, 1.15), syllables (mean, 1.69; SD, 0.54), concreteness (mean, 542.9; SD, 42.5), and imageability (mean, 563.24; SD, 32.3). Each of the 24 lists was equally often used across the four conditions (Forget–List 1, Forget–List 2, Remember–List 1, and Remember–List 2).
A schematic depiction of the directed-forgetting task is shown in Figure 1a. Within each run (Remember or Forget), two lists of 10 words each were presented sequentially on a computer screen, which was visible to the participants via a mirror attached to the head coil. The words were written in black on a gray background. Each word was presented for 2.5 s, preceded by a fixation cross with a variable duration of 1.5–2.5 s. After List 1, a cue was presented to either continue remembering the words, or to forget the words from this list. The cue was shown for 5 s and was followed by the presentation of the second list of words (same timing parameters as List 1). The second list was always followed by a cue to remember the list. Thereafter, a visual feature detection task was performed as a distracter task (∼3 min), during which arrays of randomly oriented Gabor patches were presented. One-half of the arrays contained a path of 10 collinearly oriented Gabor elements, and the task was to indicate whether or not an array contained a Gabor path (Field et al., 1993). Finally, a free recall test was performed in which participants were asked to recall all to-be-remembered items of the current run, consistent with the forget instruction. Only in the last run of the Forget condition, participants were asked to recall the words from both lists, including those that they had been instructed to forget (see Fig. 1b). Whether the Forget or the Remember condition was performed in the last run of the experiment was counterbalanced across participants. Each free recall test was followed by a 30 s resting period, which served as an fMRI baseline for task-related activity. In the first experiment, verbal responses were digitally recorded using a MRI-compatible microphone (MR confon), and the scanner noise was later removed from the resulting audio files using the free software Audacity (http://audacity.sourceforge.net/). In the second experiment, the verbal responses were manually recorded by the experimenter outside of the EEG booth (transmitted via the intercom).
fMRI recording and analysis (Experiment 1).
Imaging was performed using a 3 T MR head scanner (Siemens Allegra). For the functional series, 2226–2286 whole-brain volumes, consisting of 34 axial slices, were continuously acquired using an interleaved, standard T2*-weighted echo-planar imaging sequence [time repetition (TR), 2000 ms; time echo (TE), 30 ms; flip angle, 90°; 64 × 64 matrices; in-plane resolution, 3 × 3 mm; slice thickness, 3 mm]. High-resolution sagittal T1-weighted images were acquired after the functional scans, using a MP-RAGE (TR, 2250 ms; TE, 2.6 ms; 1 mm isotropic voxel size) to obtain a 3D structural scan.
Image preprocessing and statistical analysis was performed using SPM5 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK; www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm), running under MATLAB (MathWorks). After discarding the first few images of each session, time series were corrected for differences in slice acquisition time, unwarped, and spatially realigned to the first image of the session. The mean functional image was coregistered with the structural image, which was then normalized to a Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) (www.mni.mcgill.ca) template in standard stereotactic space. The resulting normalization parameters were applied to all functional images, which were subsequently smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm (FWHM).
fMRI data were recorded and analyzed in a blocked manner. The single-subject hemodynamic responses were modeled by convolving a boxcar function covering the duration of each word list or resting period with a first-order canonical hemodynamic response function (Friston et al., 1995). The resulting time series were then used as regressors in a voxelwise, fixed-effects general linear model. The resulting data were high-pass filtered at 256 s. Time series were modeled by four blocked covariates, corresponding to the encoding periods of the experimental conditions Forget–List 1, Forget–List 2, Remember–List 1, and Remember–List 2, and a further blocked covariate corresponding to the resting periods. Covariates modeling the free recall periods, the distracter task, session-specific effects, and movement parameters determined during realignment were also included in the model.
On a single-subject level, the four conditions of interest (Forget–List 1, Forget–List 2, Remember–List 1, Remember–List 2) were contrasted separately against the resting periods. T maps derived from these comparisons were then entered into a second-level full-factorial ANOVA, with the factors CONDITION (Forget vs Remember) and LIST (List 1 vs List 2). Planned comparisons within this model were conducted between the Forget and Remember condition during List 2 encoding, using one-sided t tests (p < 0.001, uncorrected). Analysis was focused on an anatomically defined region of interest (ROI) of the dorsolateral PFC (BA 9 and BA 46), using the WFU PickAtlas toolbox (www.fmri.wfubmc.edu/cms/software). Only clusters within this region that survived a cluster-level correction (p < 0.05) are reported. Cluster-level correction refers to whether finding a significant cluster with a given extent of k (or more) voxels, at a given threshold for the whole search volume (p < 0.001), would be expected by chance (in the present dataset, k = 20 voxels). A cluster-level-corrected p value of 0.05 denotes that the likelihood of finding an equally sized (or bigger sized) cluster by chance is 5%. Results of an exploratory whole-brain analysis with the same statistical threshold (p < 0.001, uncorrected; cluster-level correction, p < 0.05) are reported in Table 1.
Psychophysiological interaction analysis (Experiment 1).
To investigate connectivity on the fMRI data level, psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analysis was performed (Friston et al., 1997). This analysis permits investigation of the correlation between two physiological variables (e.g., BOLD signal in two brain regions) and its interaction with a psychological variable (Forget vs Remember). Following our results from the simple contrasts (Forget > Remember), BA 9 was taken as a seed region. Four subjects had to be excluded from this analysis because they either did not show significant activation of this region (F > R; p < 0.05, uncorrected; N = 2), or the clusters of activation were too remote from the cluster obtained on the group level (>4 cm; N = 2). Before the PPI analysis, the fMRI data was concatenated across the three sessions to allow simultaneous extraction of the dlPFC time series across all sessions. For each subject, an 8 mm sphere was built around a cluster showing the maximal activation (F > R) in the dlPFC (BA 9). If more than one significant cluster was obtained, the cluster closest to the peak voxel obtained in the group analysis was taken (MNI coordinates: −45, 6, 39). The mean distance from this peak voxel across subjects was 1.15 cm (SD, 0.64). PPI analysis was performed as implemented in SPM5. The main purpose of this analysis was to investigate the interaction of dlPFC activity with hippocampal activity during voluntary forgetting, as suggested by a prior fMRI study (Anderson et al., 2004). We therefore restricted the statistical analysis to an anatomically defined ROI comprising the left and right hippocampus (using WFU Pickatlas).
EEG recording and preprocessing (Experiment 1).
The EEG was recorded inside the scanner using an MR-compatible 64-channel EEG system (Brain Products). Sixty-two channels were used to record scalp EEG and were mounted in an elastic cap (EasyCap) positioned according to the international 10–10 system. FCz was used as reference electrode, and impedances were kept below 20 kΩ. Note that the MR-compatible electrode caps have an inbuilt impedance of 5 kΩ. Vertical eye movements were recorded with an additional channel placed below the left eye, and the electrocardiogram (ECG) was recorded by an electrode placed below the left scapula to facilitate off-line removal of cardioballistic artifacts. The signals were amplified between 0.1 and 100 Hz, with a notch filter at 50 Hz. The EEG data were sampled at 5000 Hz, and the clock of the EEG amplifier was synchronized to the clock output of the MR scanner using a “SynchBox” device, manufactured by Brain Products, to facilitate off-line removal of the MR gradient artifact.
When the EEG is recorded inside the MR scanner, the data are contaminated by (1) the MR gradient artifact and (2) the cardioballistic artifact, which have to be removed by various preprocessing steps. Both artifacts were removed using the FMRIB plug-in for EEGLAB (Delorme and Makeig, 2004; Niazy et al., 2005), running under MATLAB (MathWorks).
The first artifact is generated by the switching of the MR gradient each time a new image is collected. To remove this artifact, a template was constructed separately for each MR gradient artifact and for each EEG channel. The exact onset of the artifact was known by triggers delivered from the MR scanner every time a new volume was acquired. The average template was then subtracted from the actual artifact. For template construction, a moving average of 21 neighboring images and a linear combination of the major principal components describing the residual artifacts were used. These were determined automatically by means of sorted eigenvalues. The corrected data were then downsampled to 500 Hz and high-pass-filtered (using a FIR filter) at 0.5 Hz. Bad stretches of data in the continuous EEG, due to incomplete gradient artifact removal or other artifacts, were identified and removed by careful visual inspection.
The second artifact, the cardioballistic artifact, is generated by heartbeats that show a characteristic deflection in the ECG electrode denoted as QRS complex (Debener et al., 2008). For detection of the QRS onsets, the algorithm implemented in the FMRIB plug-in was used. This algorithm performs a temporal principal component analysis separately for each EEG channel. The first three components were taken as an optimal basis set for describing the artifact shape, amplitude, and scale. This set was fitted to and then subtracted from each artifact occurrence. As with the removal of the MR gradient artifacts, this was performed separately for each channel.
In a last step, the cleaned EEG data were subjected to an infomax independent component analysis (ICA) to correct for residual artifacts. Main sources of artifacts were eyeblinks, eye movements, tonic muscle activity, as well as residual cardioballistic and gradient artifacts. Components that corresponded to one of these artifacts were identified by visual inspection and removed. The remaining components were then backprojected into EEG signal space. The data were then segmented into epochs ranging from −2500 to 2500 ms relative to word onset. Before EEG analysis, the single trials were visually inspected and rejected if they contained residual artifacts. An average of 41.4 (range, 28–54), 44.3 (range, 28–57), 39.4 (range, 19–55), and 42.6 (range, 24–56) trials remained for analysis of the four conditions Forget–List 1, Forget–List 2, Remember–List 1, and Remember–List 2, respectively.
EEG recording and preprocessing (Experiment 2).
EEGs were recorded from 128 electrodes with active shielding mounted in an elastic cap with an equidistant montage (ANT; www.ant-neuro.com). Signals were recorded in a shielded booth with a DC amplifier (ANT), with a sampling rate of 2048 Hz. Preprocessing was performed using Fieldtrip (http://www.ru.nl/neuroimaging/fieldtrip) (Oostenveld et al., 2011). The data during List 2 encoding were epoched into segments of 975 ms, time-locked to the TMS pulse (25–1000 ms after TMS). The first 25 ms were discarded to eliminate the TMS artifact. Thereafter, the data were corrected for blinks and eye movements, using ICA. Remaining artifacts, due to muscle activity or poor EOG correction, were excluded by careful visual inspection. Before phase-locking analysis, the data were downsampled to 512 Hz.
rTMS procedure (Experiment 2).
An overview of the experimental procedure is shown in Figure 6a. TMS pulses were delivered with a Magstim Rapid2 stimulator via a figure-of-eight-shaped air film cooled coil (magstim; www.magstim.com). To ensure that the TMS pulses were stimulating the target brain region with high anatomical precision, TMS was guided by a neuronavigation system, which coregisters the individual MRI with the position of the TMS coil using a 3D tracking device (ANT-Visor; www.ant-neuro.com). Individual high-resolution T1-weighted MRIs were acquired from a Philips 1.5 T scanner (T1-TFE; TR, 10.656 ms; TE, 4.99 ms; flip angle, 8°; 1 mm isotropic voxel size). The coordinates for dlPFC stimulation were derived from the peak voxel showing the strongest effect in BOLD signal in our previous EEG–fMRI experiment (MNI coordinates: x = −45, y = 6, z = 39; see Fig. 3). The MNI coordinates for vertex stimulation were as follows: x = 0, y = −10, z = 80, following Miranda et al. (2006). TMS pulses were delivered at a rate of 1 Hz during encoding of List 2 items after both forget and remember instructions. There was no systematic temporal coupling between the delivery of the TMS pulses and the List 2 items. A total of 45 TMS pulses at an intensity of 90% of the individual resting motor threshold were applied during the full length of List 2 encoding, thus leading to a duration of 45 s of 1 Hz rTMS stimulation.
Analysis of phase synchronization (Experiments 1 and 2).
Phase synchronization here refers to synchrony between distant brain regions as reflected in synchrony between electrode sites. For both experiments, the same analysis was performed using in-house MATLAB scripts. For time–frequency analysis, the EEG epochs were subjected to a Gabor transformation, transforming a signal into a complex time–frequency signal, from which the phase information can be extracted. The data were filtered in a frequency range of 4–45 Hz. The filter parameter for time–frequency resolution (gamma) was set to 1 for the lower frequency range (4–20 Hz), and to π for the higher frequency range (20–45 Hz), ensuring that the time–frequency characteristics of the different frequency bands are optimally detected.
Before phase synchrony calculation, a current source density transformation was applied to the EEG data using the CSD toolbox (http://psychophysiology.cpmc.columbia.edu/Software/CSDtoolbox/index.html). This was done to minimize effects of volume conduction (Lachaux et al., 1999). The phase synchrony values were calculated following the procedure of Lachaux et al. (1999) using the phase estimates from the Gabor transformation. This procedure delivers a value that ranges from 0 to 1, indicating maximal phase variability and maximal phase synchrony, respectively. Phase synchronization values were calculated for all possible pairs of electrodes in a frequency range from 4 to 45 Hz. Because trial numbers can bias phase synchronization measures, we checked whether there was a significant difference between trial numbers across the four conditions, by means of a Friedman ANOVA. This analysis revealed no significant difference in trial numbers across the four conditions. Additionally, a control analysis was conducted in which the number of trials were equated, by means of randomly selecting the minimum amount of available trials. This control analysis revealed similar significant effects (pcorr < 0.05). One specific hypothesis was formulated for the upper alpha–lower beta frequency range as derived from a prior EEG study (Bäuml et al., 2008), but statistical comparisons were calculated across the whole frequency range to determine the specificity of this effect.
For the second (rTMS–EEG) experiment, the analysis was focused on phase coupling in a frequency range between 11 and 18 Hz informed by the results of Experiment 1. Furthermore, the analysis was restricted to the time window of 500–1000 ms after the TMS pulse, because TMS at the prefrontal cortex induces a strong evoked response in the beta range (lasting ∼400 ms), which is likely to overshadow more subtle cognitive effects (Rosanova et al., 2009). For this analysis, the number of trials was equated across the two conditions by randomly selecting a minimum number of trials that was available across the two conditions. On average, 194 (range, 55–242) and 199 (83–256) trials remained for the dlPFC and Vertex groups, respectively. Note that the trials in this experiment had shorter epochs (500 ms) and were time-locked to the TMS pulse leading to higher trial numbers than in the first experiment.
Analysis of EEG power (Experiment 1).
Before analysis of oscillatory power modulation (μv2), the EEG data were re-referenced to average reference. For time–frequency analysis, a Gabor transformation with the same parameters as for the phase synchrony analysis was used. To quantify event-related power changes, the percentage of poststimulus power change in relation to a prestimulus baseline period was calculated. The baseline period was set to −0.5 s to stimulus onset for the lower frequency range (4–20 Hz), and to −0.25 s to stimulus onset for the higher frequency range (20–45 Hz). Before statistical analysis, the signal change values were collapsed to obtain six frequency bands: theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (9–11 Hz), beta 1 (12–19 Hz), beta 2 (20–29 Hz), and gamma (30–45 Hz); and four time windows: 0–0.5, 0.5–1.0, 1.0–1.5, and 1.5–2.0 s. Based on prior studies, specific hypotheses were formulated with respect to the alpha frequency band. Statistical comparisons, however, were conducted across all frequency bands to investigate the specificity of the effects.
EEG–fMRI analysis (Experiment 1).
Combined EEG–fMRI analysis was performed to investigate whether the experimentally induced effects on the BOLD signal and brain oscillations covaried on a block-by-block level. The toolbox MARSBAR (http://marsbar.sourceforge.net) was used to first extract the fMRI signal from the preprocessed EPI images, averaged across all voxels constituting the left dlPFC region of interest. To isolate the signal corresponding to a given condition (Forget–List 2 and Remember–List 2, respectively), these time series were then fit with the condition regressors, that is, the predicted time courses in the GLM design matrix. A mean value for each block was obtained by averaging across those segments of the fitted time series that corresponded to the duration of a block of interest, as defined by the predicted time course turning positive at the beginning of a block, and negative at the end of a block. This procedure resulted in six values for the Remember condition, and six values for the Forget condition. In a last step, these values were z-transformed to account for global signal differences between participants.
To obtain the mean levels of phase synchronization in each block, the same procedure as in the study by Hanslmayr et al. (2007) was applied. This procedure estimates the mean level of phase synchrony for each block by calculating the single-trial phase deviation from the mean phase, applying the circular variance procedure proposed by Fisher (1993). These single-trial phase deviation values were then collapsed across those electrode pairs, time points (−2.0 to 2.0 s), and frequency bands (11–18 Hz), in which significant effects emerged. To obtain the level of phase synchrony for each block, the phase deviation values were collapsed across artifact-free single trials within a block. For each block, a maximum of 10 single trials was available; however, <10 trials were available in most of the cases due to artifacts. If less than five single trials were available, the block was discarded. Otherwise, the median across artifact-free single trials was taken as an estimate of the average phase synchronization within a block. Two participants were excluded from the combined EEG–fMRI analysis because less than three blocks remained for analysis of one of the two conditions of interest (Forget–List 2, Remember–List 2). For the remaining 20 participants, an average of 5.5 and 5.3 blocks remained for the Forget and Remember conditions, respectively. Before correlation with the BOLD signal, the data were z-transformed to normalize the data across subjects. Z-transformation was performed such that the mean level of BOLD/phase synchrony was subtracted from the individual values of the respective blocks, which were then divided by the SD of these values.
Statistical analysis (Experiments 1 and 2).
For statistical comparisons of all behavioral data, t tests with the p level set to 0.05 were used. These tests were performed one-sided when a directed hypothesis was tested (e.g., testing for the forgetting and beneficial effects), and two-sided when no directed hypothesis was tested (e.g., testing for the effects of TMS on behavior).
Analysis of the EEG data in both experiments was fully based on nonparametrical Wilcoxon signed-rank tests (one-sided), and a randomization approach was used to correct for multiple comparisons (Hanslmayr et al., 2009a,b). This procedure was originally proposed by Blair and Karniski (1993) and works as follows: First, Wilcoxon's tests were calculated for each electrode pair to investigate how many pairs show a significant difference between the two conditions at a given p level (p < 0.005, one-sided). Second, 2000 randomization runs were performed in which the conditions were swapped randomly across participants. This procedure produces a distribution of the number of electrodes exceeding a certain statistical threshold (p < 0.005; in our case) under the null hypothesis (no systematic difference between conditions), and evaluates whether a given number of electrode pairs exhibiting a significant difference between two conditions at this threshold can be expected by chance. If the p value (pcorr) of this randomization test is <0.05, <5% of the permutation runs exhibited equal or more electrode pairs with a significant difference between the two conditions.
fMRI comparisons were conducted using one-sided t tests (p < 0.001, uncorrected), with the anatomically defined ROI of the dlPFC (BA 9 and BA 46). Only clusters within this ROI surviving cluster-level correction (p < 0.05) are reported as significant (see Table 1 for a whole-brain analysis). For statistical analysis of the correlations between the BOLD signal in the left dlPFC and phase synchronization, we followed a random-effects approach. One correlation coefficient (Spearman) was calculated to fit the data points (maximum, 6) of each single subject. These correlation coefficients were then Fisher z transformed. Thereafter, the mean (Fisher z-transformed) correlation coefficient across all subjects was tested against zero using one-sample t tests (one-sided). To examine the difference in BOLD–phase synchronization correlations between conditions, a dependent-samples t test was performed.
Results
Experiment 1: simultaneous EEG–fMRI
Behavioral results
Participants performed several runs of the directed-forgetting task (Fig. 1a,b). Each cue condition was repeated six times, in an unpredictable order. In the first five runs of the Forget condition, subjects were asked to recall only to-be-remembered (List 2) items at test, which is consistent with the instruction (Fig. 1b). Only in the last (sixth) run of the Forget condition, a surprise test was conducted, in which subjects were asked to recall all words of the current run, including the to-be-forgotten items of List 1 (Fig. 1b). The behavioral data of this sixth run revealed lower recall levels for List 1 items in the Forget condition (45.9%) compared with the Remember condition (65.9%; t(21) = −2.15, p < 0.05; Fig. 1c); and they revealed higher recall levels for List 2 items in the Forget condition (73.2%) than in the Remember condition (57.3%; t(21) = 3.43; p < 0.005; Fig. 1c). These results demonstrate that subjects were able to voluntarily forget the old, obsolete information, for the benefit of improved memory for the new, relevant information.
To rule out that the forgetting effect was caused by output order effects at test (i.e., the prior recall of the to-be-remembered items), average output positions were calculated for each subject [for details, see Bjork (1970)] and correlated with forgetting. This correlation was close to zero (r = 0.09; p > 0.5), suggesting that the forgetting in this experiment was not caused by differences in output order, which is consistent with prior work showing that output order has a negligible effect on directed forgetting (Geiselman et al., 1983). We additionally analyzed the intrusion rates for the to-be-forgotten List 1 items for the first five runs. Consistent with the literature (Pastötter and Bäuml, 2007), the mean level of intrusions was very low, 4.45% (SD, 5.9), suggesting that participants were well able to follow the instruction to only recall to-be-remembered items. The intrusion rates did not change across runs (F(4,84) = 0.87; p > 0.45), and subjects with low and high levels of intrusions (median split) did not differ with respect to their forgetting and enhancement scores (values of t(20) < 1.67; values of p > 0.1).
Phase synchrony
Both prior behavioral and prior EEG work indicate that the mechanisms responsible for the forgetting of the obsolete (List 1) information operate during encoding of the new (List 2) information (Bäuml et al., 2008; Pastötter and Bäuml, 2010) (i.e., after the forget or remember cue had been presented). Consistently, phase synchronization measures between the two cue conditions were contrasted by means of the phase-locking value (PLV) (Lachaux et al., 1999). In line with our previous study (Bäuml et al., 2008), data were pooled in a peristimulus interval (−2 to 2 s). The results show that the forget cue induced lower levels of phase synchrony than the remember cue (pcorr < 0.005; Fig. 2a). This effect was evident over widespread cortical regions in a frequency range within the upper alpha, lower beta band (11–18 Hz). No differences between the two conditions were found during List 1 encoding, and no changes in phase synchrony in the opposite direction (F > R) were observed. Further exploration of the data revealed that the forget cue induced a decrease in phase synchronization from List 1 to List 2 (Z = −3.72; p < 0.001), whereas no difference between the two lists emerged in the Remember condition (Z = 1.64; p > 0.1; Fig. 2b). The difference between List 1 and List 2 phase coupling was significantly higher in the Forget than in the Remember condition (Z = −3.33; p < 0.001). Examination of the time course of phase synchronization revealed no change between the prestimulus and poststimulus interval, showing that phase synchrony was tonically reduced throughout List 2 encoding, independent of the onset of List 2 items (Fig. 2c). No significant effects emerged in the higher beta (20–29 Hz) and gamma frequency band (30–45 Hz). Further analysis indicated that the effects of the forget cue on phase synchronization did not vary across the significant electrode pairs.
EEG power
Contrasting alpha power between the two conditions during encoding of List 2 items revealed lower levels of stimulus-induced alpha power in the Forget condition, compared with the Remember condition (pcorr < 0.05; Fig. 3a). This effect showed a frontoparietal topography and emerged at an early (0–0.5 s) and a later time window (1–1.5 s). Closer inspection of the effect revealed that alpha power increased from List 1 to List 2 in the Remember condition (Z = 3.12; p < 0.001), but stayed constant in the Forget condition (p > 0.5; Fig. 3b); the difference between the two lists in the Remember condition was significantly larger than in the Forget condition (Z = 2.94; p < 0.005). No significant effects between the two conditions emerged during List 1 encoding, and no effects were obtained in other frequency bands.
fMRI results
Analogous to the EEG analysis, we contrasted the BOLD signal between the two conditions during study of List 2. Following our hypothesis, the analysis was restricted to an anatomically defined ROI corresponding to the dorsolateral PFC (BA9 and BA46). Higher activity was found in the Forget compared with the Remember condition in the left dlPFC (Fig. 4a; BA 9; MNI coordinates: −45, 6, 39; voxel size, 21; pcorr < 0.05). The results of an exploratory whole-brain analysis are reported in Table 1. No brain region showed higher activity in the Remember compared with the Forget condition, and no differences between conditions were observed during List 1 study.
To investigate the influence of the forget instruction on functional connectivity on the fMRI data level, a PPI analysis was performed. This analysis was conducted in an attempt to replicate a prior fMRI study showing that voluntary memory suppression is also reflected in the interaction between dlPFC and the hippocampus (Anderson et al., 2004). The results of this PPI analysis revealed that the correlation between dlPFC and hippocampus activity was significantly modulated by the forget instruction (Fig. 4b; pcorr < 0.05; MNI coordinates: 21, −30, −3; voxel size, 5). This effect was due to a reduced dlPFC–hippocampus connectivity in the Forget (mean r = −0.002) compared with the Remember condition (mean r = 0.39).
Correlation between BOLD and phase synchrony
The results above suggest that the cue to forget irrelevant memories induced a decrease in phase synchrony along with an activation increase in the left dlPFC. Central to the aim of this study, we investigated whether the decrease in phase synchrony can be predicted by dlPFC activity on a block-by-block basis. To this end, the dlPFC BOLD signal was correlated with phase synchronization during List 2 encoding (for details, see Materials and Methods). To examine whether such a correlation is specifically driven by the experimental condition of interest, correlations across blocks (six blocks per condition) were performed separately for the Forget and Remember conditions. The results revealed that increased activation of the dlPFC correlated with the decrease in phase synchrony in the Forget condition (mean r = −0.33; t(19) = −2.19; p < 0.05; Fig. 5a), whereas no correlation between dlPFC activation and phase synchronization arose in the Remember condition (Fig. 5b; mean r = 0.034; t(19) = 0.28; p > 0.50); the correlation between dlPFC activity and phase synchrony was significantly higher in the Forget compared with the Remember condition (t(19) = −2.48; p < 0.05). Note that these correlations, although modest in magnitude, are well within the range of EEG–BOLD correlations obtained in other human EEG–fMRI studies (Mantini et al., 2007). Motivated by the results from the fMRI connectivity, we also correlated phase synchrony with the BOLD signal in right hippocampus (Fig. 4b). However, this analysis yielded no significant correlations in any of the two conditions (both values of p > 0.25). It should be noted, however, that the EEG–BOLD correlations were conducted on a block-by-block level with an average of five data points per subject, which is a slight limitation of this analysis. We therefore conducted a second rTMS–EEG experiment to further verify and extend these findings.
Experiment 2: simultaneous rTMS–EEG
The results of the first experiment show that increased activation in the dlPFC goes hand-in-hand with a decrease in neural synchrony during voluntary forgetting. To examine whether this relationship is of a causal more than just a correlational nature, we conducted a combined rTMS–EEG experiment and tested whether direct stimulation of dlPFC has a modulating effect on both the behavioral consequences of the forget instruction and phase synchronization. In this second experiment, 44 participants performed the same forgetting task as in the first experiment. Volunteers were randomly assigned to one of two groups, with one group (N = 22) receiving rTMS at dlPFC (MNI coordinates: −45, 6, 39), and the other group (N = 22) receiving rTMS at vertex (a typical control region for TMS studies). A total of 45 TMS pulses with an intensity of 90% resting motor threshold was delivered at a continuous rate of 1 Hz during List 2 encoding in both the Remember and Forget conditions (Fig. 6a).
Behavioral results
To investigate the impact of rTMS on behavior, the two stimulation site (dlPFC vs vertex) groups were contrasted with respect to their relative forgetting and enhancement scores. The forgetting score was calculated as the difference between List 1 words recalled in the Remember and Forget condition (R minus F), and the enhancement score was calculated as the difference between List 2 recall in the two conditions (F minus R). Analogous to Experiment 1, the behavioral data of the sixth run were analyzed for both enhancement and forgetting scores. The results are shown in Figure 6b. Participants who received rTMS at the dorsolateral PFC showed a significantly stronger forgetting effect than the control group who received rTMS at vertex (30.0 vs 6.4%; t(42) = 2.34; p < 0.05, two-tailed). No significant difference was observed for the enhancement effect (6.8 vs 10.0%; t(42) = −0.45; p > 0.5). Additionally, a two-way ANOVA with the factors GROUP (dlPFC vs vertex) and SCORE TYPE (forgetting vs enhancement) yielded a significant interaction (F(1,42) = 4.47; p < 0.05). These results demonstrate that rTMS at the dorsolateral PFC during List 2 encoding selectively boosts voluntary forgetting of List 1 items, without affecting the enhancement of List 2 items. The full pattern of the behavioral results is shown in Table 2, which shows that the increased forgetting scores in the dlPFC group were driven by a decrease in the Forget–List 1 condition, as well as by a moderate increase in the Remember–List 1 condition compared with the vertex group. However, none of these separate comparisons reached significance (t(46) > 1.49; p > 0.1).
EEG results
The effect of rTMS on phase synchronization was examined by contrasting phase synchrony during List 2 encoding between the Forget and the Remember conditions. Following the results of the prior experiment, the analysis was focused on a frequency range of 11–18 Hz. Note that active rTMS was applied in both conditions, thus controlling for unspecific cortical rTMS effects. Participants who were stimulated at the left dlPFC showed a robust decrease in phase synchronization at 13 Hz in the Forget compared with the Remember condition (pcorr < 0.005), whereas participants from the control (vertex) group showed only a marginally significant effect at 17 Hz (pcorr = 0.066; Fig. 7) in the same direction. Importantly, the number of electrode pairs exhibiting a significant reduction in phase synchronization differed significantly between the two groups (84 vs 54; X21 = 6.52; p < 0.05), showing stronger phase desynchronization in response to the forget cue in the dlPFC than the control group. No significant effects in the opposite direction (F > R) were observed in any of the two groups.
Discussion
We investigated whether prefrontal brain regions can regulate neural synchronization during long-term memory control, using a voluntary forgetting task. Using simultaneous EEG–fMRI recordings in the first experiment, we were able to demonstrate that an instruction to forget obsolete memories induces a reduction in long-range phase synchronization along with a BOLD signal increase in the left dorsolateral PFC. Importantly, the BOLD signal increase in the dlPFC correlated with a decrease in neural synchrony, specifically when subjects were asked to forget the previously encoded information. In the second experiment, we showed that direct stimulation of the dlPFC via rTMS selectively boosts voluntary forgetting and phase desynchronization, thus providing evidence for a causal role of dlPFC in driving forgetting and phase synchrony.
These results strongly suggest that prefrontally regulated neural synchrony reflects an active memory control process. Following the idea that memories are represented in widely distributed cortical networks (Fuster, 1997), a widespread decrease in phase synchronization might reflect the downregulation of the cortical network representing the obsolete, to-be-forgotten memories. This interpretation is corroborated by our finding that stimulating the dlPFC during List 2 encoding selectively impaired memory for the to-be-forgotten List 1 items, without affecting memory for the to-be-remembered List 2 items. This result is quite remarkable, given that rTMS was applied during List 2 encoding, but fits with prior work indicating that the forget cue induces two dissociable effects (Sahakyan and Delaney, 2003; Bäuml et al., 2008; Pastötter and Bäuml, 2010), which both operate during List 2 encoding (i.e., after the forget cue has been given) (Bäuml et al., 2008; Pastötter and Bäuml, 2010).
These results also replicate prior fMRI and lesion studies indicating that memory control is mediated by the dorsolateral PFC (Conway and Fthenaki, 2003; Anderson et al., 2004; Depue et al., 2007). Although the focus of dlPFC activity in our study is slightly more dorsal compared with the results by Anderson et al. (2004), our findings replicate previous results of Nyberg et al. (2009), reporting activation of a similar region in BA9 during a long-term memory updating task. Like Anderson et al. (2004), we also observed that voluntary forgetting affected BOLD connectivity between dlPFC and hippocampus, even though forgetting was associated with lowered dlPFC–hippocampus connectivity in the present study. Note that, in contrast to the think/no-think paradigm (Anderson and Green, 2001; Anderson et al., 2004), which requires subjects to suppress the retrieval of a previously learned association, the directed-forgetting task used in the present study is a long-term memory updating task in which old information has to be forgotten during the learning of new information, likely involving different prefrontal-hippocampal interactions.
In addition to decreased phase coupling, the EEG results of Experiment 1 also revealed increased power in the alpha band specifically when subjects were asked to remember the previously learned information. In contrast, no such changes occurred in the Forget condition (Fig. 3b). Several previous studies linked stimulus-induced alpha power decreases to item-specific information processing and long-term memory encoding (for review, see Hanslmayr et al., 2012). In particular, alpha power has been shown to increase with the amount of encoded information (Sederberg et al., 2006; Pastötter et al., 2008), mirroring decreased encoding quality (Underwood, 1978). In line with these findings, the alpha power decreases might indicate that the forget cue resets the neural activity back to first-list level, thus ensuring high encoding quality for the new information (Pastötter et al., 2008, 2011). This reset view is corroborated by a previous study (Bäuml et al., 2008) showing that the alpha power difference between the Forget and Remember condition specifically correlates with improved memory for List 2 items.
Importantly, the effects of the forget cue on alpha power and phase synchrony differed markedly in timing. Whereas the forgetting-induced decrease in phase synchrony was independent of the onset of the List 2 items (Fig. 2c), the decreases in alpha power were time-locked to the stimulus onset. This further suggests that the alpha power decreases reflect item-specific memory encoding processes of the List 2 items, whereas the tonically reduced phase synchronization reflects inhibition of List 1 items.
Regarding the cognitive processes underlying directed forgetting, one dominant view is that the forget cue triggers the inhibition of the original encoding context of the items, creating problems in retrieving these items when tested later (Geiselman et al., 1983; Anderson, 2005; Bäuml et al., 2008). This idea acknowledges the fact that directed forgetting is usually found in recall tasks but not in recognition tasks (Bjork, 1989), and that forgetting can be eliminated once the original encoding context gets reactivated (Bjork and Bjork, 1996; Bäuml and Samenieh, 2010, 2012). An analogy might be that, after directed forgetting, the files are still present on the hard disk, but the paths are temporarily lost. Our results are in line with this assumption, and suggest that reduced phase synchronization might be a correlate of this unbinding process, which impairs access to these items when tested later (Bäuml et al., 2010). Although being speculative, this interpretation fits with the idea that long-range synchrony acts to dynamically bind (and unbind) cortically distributed representations (Varela et al., 2001; Fries, 2005).
Another, noninhibitory, explanation of directed forgetting is the context change account (Sahakyan and Kelley, 2002). This hypothesis assumes that the forget cue induces a change in subjects' mental context between study of the two lists, which then impairs List 1 recall due to a mismatch between the context at encoding and the context at retrieval. Although this noninhibitory view can account for a number of behavioral findings (Sahakyan and Kelley, 2002), it is hard to reconcile with the present findings. In particular, the context change account cannot explain why phase synchronization is reduced below baseline level. Additionally, in a previous EEG study, we investigated the brain oscillatory correlates of the mental context change paradigm, as proposed by Sahakyan and colleagues, and did not find any decreases in phase synchronization during List 2 encoding (Pastötter et al., 2008).
It is also worth highlighting that, despite the fact that the EEG data from the first experiment were recorded inside the MR scanner, typically causing substantial distortions of the raw EEG signal, our finding of decreased phase synchrony in the upper alpha–lower beta frequency band perfectly replicates the results of a previous conventional EEG study (Bäuml et al., 2008). In this former directed-forgetting study, the decrease in phase synchronization specifically predicted the amount of List 1 forgetting, establishing a tight link between decreased levels of phase coupling during List 2 study and the forgetting of obsolete memories. As in this prior study, the effect in phase synchronization showed a widespread topography involving frontal, temporal, and parietal electrode sites. These independent results suggest that reliable EEG data were recorded despite the noisy scanner environment.
Our finding that slow rTMS (1 Hz) has a facilitatory effect on voluntary forgetting and phase desynchronization is perfectly in line with recent literature showing that slow rTMS applied at the prefrontal cortex can boost neural processing and cognitive performance (Li et al., 2004; Knoch et al., 2006; Ward et al., 2010). For instance, slow rTMS applied at the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex enhances blood flow and BOLD signal at the stimulated region (Li et al., 2004; Knoch et al., 2006) and has been found to increase cognitive performance in a selective attention task (Ward et al., 2010). This literature seems to be at odds with papers arguing that slow rTMS (<5 Hz) disturbs neural activity by creating virtual lesions (Chen et al., 1997; Thut and Pascual-Leone, 2010). The exact reasons for why slow rTMS is sometimes facilitatory and sometimes inhibitory are not yet known, but it has been suggested that the effects of rTMS vary depending on the stimulated region (Rosanova et al., 2009), the state of the stimulated region (Silvanto et al., 2008), and on whether rTMS is applied on-line (during task performance) or off-line (before task performance). Likely, future studies are needed to shed more light onto these open issues.
The impact of oscillatory activity on memory formation has long been demonstrated on the microlevel (i.e., in single-cell and multicell recordings in animals) (Buzsáki, 2010). It is now increasingly being recognized that brain oscillations, and large-scale phase synchrony in particular, also play a fundamental role for human long-term memory (Fell and Axmacher, 2011). While most studies analyzed oscillatory activity during single-list learning (Fell et al., 2001; Summerfield and Mangels, 2005), the current experiment went one step further, investigating the impact of prefrontal control regions on brain synchrony during goal-directed forgetting. Using simultaneous EEG–fMRI in the first experiment, and rTMS–EEG in the second experiment, we provide evidence that the prefrontal cortex can drive a widespread reduction in neural synchrony when previously encoded information becomes obsolete. These results suggest that prefrontally mediated regulation of long-range synchrony might be a general mechanism underlying memory control.
Footnotes
Parts of this research have been supported by a grant from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (Project HA 5622/1-1) awarded to S.H. We thank Anna Karmann, Stefanie Birkner, and Marina Koepfer for help with data acquisition.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Simon Hanslmayr, Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz, Box ZPR, 78457 Konstanz, Germany. simon.hanslmayr{at}uni-konstanz.de