Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Brief Communications

An Effect of Bilingualism on the Auditory Cortex

Volker Ressel, Christophe Pallier, Noelia Ventura-Campos, Begoña Díaz, Abeba Roessler, César Ávila and Núria Sebastián-Gallés
Journal of Neuroscience 21 November 2012, 32 (47) 16597-16601; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1996-12.2012
Volker Ressel
1Speech Acquisition and Perception Group, Center for Brain and Cognition, Department of Technology, Pompeu Fabra University, 08018 Barcelona, Spain,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Christophe Pallier
2INSERM, CEA, Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, Neurospin Center, F-91191 Paris, France,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Noelia Ventura-Campos
3Department of Psychology, Jaume I University, 12071 Castellón de la Plana, Spain, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Begoña Díaz
4Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, 04103 Leipzig, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Abeba Roessler
1Speech Acquisition and Perception Group, Center for Brain and Cognition, Department of Technology, Pompeu Fabra University, 08018 Barcelona, Spain,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
César Ávila
3Department of Psychology, Jaume I University, 12071 Castellón de la Plana, Spain, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Núria Sebastián-Gallés
1Speech Acquisition and Perception Group, Center for Brain and Cognition, Department of Technology, Pompeu Fabra University, 08018 Barcelona, Spain,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Two studies (Golestani et al., 2007; Wong et al., 2008) have reported a positive correlation between the ability to perceive foreign speech sounds and the volume of Heschl's gyrus (HG), the structure that houses the auditory cortex. More precisely, participants with larger left Heschl's gyri learned consonantal or tonal contrasts faster than those with smaller HG. These studies leave open the question of the impact of experience on HG volumes. In the current research, we investigated the effect of early language exposure on Heschl's gyrus by comparing Spanish–Catalan bilinguals who have been exposed to two languages since childhood, to a group of Spanish monolinguals matched in education, socio-economic status, and musical experience. Manual volumetric measurements of HG revealed that bilinguals have, on average, larger Heschl's gyri than monolinguals. This was corroborated, for the left Heschl's gyrus, by a voxel-based morphometry analysis showing larger gray matter volumes in bilinguals than in monolinguals. Since the bilinguals in this study were not a self-selected group, this observation provides a clear demonstration that learning a second language is a causal factor in the increased size of the auditory cortex.

Introduction

The brain's shape and volume vary considerably between individuals (Ono et al., 1990). Genetic, environmental, and experience-dependent factors all play a role (Golestani et al., 2011; Yoon et al., 2011). In two studies of experience-dependent brain plasticity, Maguire et al. (2000) reported that the size of the hippocampus correlated positively with years of professional practice in taxi drivers, and Draganski et al. (2004) observed increased gray matter (GM) density in the mid-temporal area after juggling training. More recently, Abdul-Kareem et al. (2011) reported a correlation between GM volume in Broca's area and years of musical performance in musicians and Carreiras et al. (2009) showed structural brain differences between literates and illiterates.

Concerning the auditory cortex, it is known that deafness or tinnitus can have a negative impact on its development (Emmorey et al., 2003; Schneider et al., 2009). Additionally, an enriched environment, such as musical education, can lead to enlargement of the Heschl's gyrus (HG) (Schneider et al., 2002). One can wonder whether language experience also plays a role. Very little research exists on that question. A first attempt was made by Kochunov et al. (2003), who compared English and Chinese speakers and reported differences of several brain areas. However, because the Chinese speakers in this study were Asians born in China and the English speakers were Caucasians born in the United States, the linguistic contrast was confounded with ethnic and cultural differences. More recently, Crinion et al. (2009) compared Chinese and English speakers, controlling for ethnicity, and reported higher GM and white matter (WM) densities in the right anterior temporal lobe and the left posterior insula of the Chinese speakers. The authors propose that this may be the result of training to process linguistic tones.

A last hint that language experience can affect language-related brain development comes from Mechelli et al. (2004), who compared bilingual to monolingual speakers. Using voxel-based morphometry (VBM) analyses, they observed that the mastery of a second language correlated with higher GM density in the left posterior inferior parietal cortex, a region involved in vocabulary acquisition (see also Lee et al., 2007).

Bilinguals not only have to master two lexicons but also two phonological systems. This begs the question of whether the parts of the brain supporting phonetic/phonological processes, in the auditory cortex and its immediate vicinity (Jacquemot et al., 2003), differ between bilinguals and monolinguals. The two studies mentioned in the abstract (Golestani et al., 2007 and Wong et al., 2008) have reported a positive correlation between the volume of HG and the ability to perceive foreign speech sounds or tones (see also Sutherland et al., 2012). But it is unknown whether these differences in HG's volume were innate or due to experience. Mechelli et al.'s (2004) study comparing bilinguals to monolinguals did not report any difference between the two groups in the auditory cortex, but their whole-brain approach may have lacked sufficient sensitivity. Therefore, we decided to directly measure the size of auditory cortices in matched groups of bilinguals and monolinguals.

Materials and Methods

Participants.

Forty-four right-handed participants, students at the University Jaume I of Castellón de la Plana (Spain), with no known auditory or neurological deficits, took part in the study. Twenty-two were monolingual Spanish speakers (11 women; mean age = 23.1.years, SD = 4.75 years) and 22 were highly fluent Catalan–Spanish bilinguals (11 women; mean age = 21.5 years, SD = 2.7 years). The study followed the ethical protocol of the University Jaume I of Castellón and all the participants were paid for their participation.

The monolinguals had been exposed to Catalan for 3 months at most, since their arrival in Castellon; the bilingual participants had learned both Catalan and Spanish before the age of 7 years and used them daily (for information about their linguistic background, see Table 1). As the participants had received formal education in English or French in school, starting at age 8.9 years (SD = 3.5 years), we asked them to self-rate their comprehension, reading, fluency, pronunciation, and writing skills in French and in English (on a four-point scale: 1 = perfect, 2 = good, 3 = sufficient, 4 = poor). The scores showed that no participant had a much proficiency in any of these languages (range of average ratings: 2.4–3.9). Both groups of participants reported a poor command of French (average ratings: bilinguals: 3.7, SD = 0.6; monolinguals: 3.9, SD = 0.3; t(42) = 1.4) and a moderate command of English (bilinguals' scores were higher than monolinguals'; average ratings: 2.7and 3.3, SD = 0.3 and 0.25, respectively; t(42) = 3.4 p < 0.05).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Spanish–Catalan bilingual participants' linguistic background

Finally, information about participants' musical education was collected over the phone or through e-mail a few months after the scanning session. Four participants (2 bilinguals and 2 monolinguals) could not be contacted a posteriori. The following questions were asked: Have you ever played an instrument? When did you start? For how many years did you play? How many hours per week do you currently play an instrument? Additional information about attendance, starting age, and years spent in a music school was collected. The groups did not significantly differ in musical expertise.

Image acquisition and processing.

Structural MRI data were acquired with a 1.5-T Siemens Avanto. Participants' heads were immobilized with cushions to reduce motion artifacts. The 3D anatomical volumes were acquired using a T1-weighted gradient echo pulse sequence (TR/TE = 11/4.9 ms, matrix = 256 × 224 × 176; voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm).

Segmentation of Heschl's gyri.

The anatomical images were spatially normalized (affine registration only) onto the Montreal Neurological Institute MNI152 template using SPM5 (The Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK).

Hesch's gyri were then manually segmented on the anatomically normalized images using Anatomist software (http://brainvisa.info), displaying sagittal, axial, and coronal planes simultaneously. Each HG was first identified in the axial plane following the procedure described in Penhune et al. (1996). The HG was delimited with a line from the medial end of the first transverse sulcus to the medial end of the Heschl sulcus. The inferior boundary of the HG was identified in the sagittal and coronal planes with a line from the depth of the Heschl sulcus to the first transverse sulcus in the stem of the HG. The superior boundary was defined in the coronal and sagittal planes by the visible ending of the HG. However, multiple or split HG are frequently present. It has been shown that primary auditory areas are located in the most anterior gyri whenever there are multiple transverse gyri (Rademacher et al., 2001). In the case of split HG (at least half of its length), the primary auditory areas may also be located in the most anterior gyral subregion (Rademacher et al., 2001). Therefore, when multiplications of HG or split HG were present, only the most anterior gyri or gyral subregion was segmented. Two trained raters manually drew the HG. They were blind to group (monolinguals and bilinguals), gender, and hemisphere (left and right). The volumes of HG were calculated from the number of voxels in the manually drawn masks. Moreover, the masks were intersected with individual segmented white and gray matter images computed by SPM5 to provide volumes of white and gray matter, respectively. Finally, an inverse normalization was performed on the HG mask to compute the volumes in the native space. Inter-rater reliability was assessed by comparing the global volumes of all HG. The Pearson's r correlation between the volumes measured by the two raters was 0.87 (n = 88, p < 0.001).

Voxel-based morphometry analysis.

VBM was performed using the Diffeomorphic Anatomical Registration through Exponentiated Lie algebra toolbox (DARTEL) procedure implemented in SPM8 (Ashburner, 2007; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). The MR images were segmented into GM, WM, and CSF using the standard unified segmentation model in SPM8 (Ashburner and Friston, 2005). The clean-up procedure was applied to the segmented GM images to remove nonbrain tissue. The GM and WM segmented images were normalized, respectively, to the GM and WM population templates generated from our complete database image set (200 subjects) using the DARTEL registration method (Ashburner, 2007). This template is an affine registration in MNI fields. Individual images were normalized to the template, keeping a voxel resolution of 1 × 1 × 1 mm. Spatially normalized images were modulated to ensure that the overall amount of each tissue class was not altered by the spatial normalization procedure, thus allowing the comparison of volume differences. Finally, the images were smoothed with an 8 mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel. The statistical analyses were performed using SPM8, with linear models that included, beside the main variable Group (Monolingual vs Bilinguals), whole-brain volumes (sum of GM and WM volumes computed by SPM) and gender as covariates.

Results

Figure 1 shows the distributions of individual measurements of HG volumes in the normalized space. Bilinguals had, on average, larger HG than monolinguals, both in the left and in the right hemisphere. This was confirmed by an ANOVA with the factors Group (Monolinguals vs Bilinguals), Gender (Male vs Female), and Side (Left vs Right), in which the effects of Group (Bilinguals > Monolinguals; F(1,40) = 5.4; p < 0.05) and Gender (Female > Male; F(1,40) = 5.8; p < 0.05) were significant.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Distributions of individual Heschl's gyri volumes. Heschl's gyrus volumes (in mm3; normalized space) as a function of Side (left vs right hemisphere) and Group (monolingual vs bilingual). Left, Each line represents one Heschl's gyrus for each participant. The distribution of the volumes, the mean, and the SE are shown. Right, Probability maps of Heschl's gyrus.

The difference between the bilinguals and monolinguals was also observed for measurements in native space (NS), and for normalized GM and WM volumes. The GM measurement showed a Group (Bilinguals > Monolinguals; F(1,40) = 5.5; p < 0.05) and Gender (Female > Male; F(1,40) = 7.1; p < 0.05) effect without interaction. The WM and NS measurements only showed a significant Group effect (Bilinguals > Monolinguals; WM: F(1,40) = 4.5, p < 0.05; NS: F(1,40) = 7.5, p < 0.01).

Table 2 displays the gross morphological features of HG (single, split, duplicate, or triple gyri) for the two groups. There is a trend toward more frequent split HG in monolinguals compared with bilinguals, although this result is not significant (Fisher's exact test: left HG: p = 0.30; right HG: p = 0.21).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Gross morphological features of HG (single, split, duplicate, or triple gyri)

No significant differences in the normalized HG volumes were found between participants playing an instrument and those who reported not to play any (for all t tests, p > 0.05). Also, no significant differences were found between participants who had attended a music school and those who had not (for more information, see Table 3). Additionally, no significant correlation between HG volumes and the variables onset of playing, years of playing, or hours per week of playing were observed.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3.

Normalized Heschl's gyrus volumes (in mm3, means and SD) as a function of musical education

Regarding English and French as a foreign language, no significant correlation between HG volumes and self-reported language scores were seen.

The VBM analysis comparing gray matter between bilinguals and monolinguals (and including whole-brain volume and gender as covariates) did not detect significant differences at the threshold t = 5.22 [p < 0.05, family-wise error (FWE) corrected for multiple comparisons], but at a lower threshold of t = 3.3 (p < 0.001 voxel-based, uncorrected), differences (Bilinguals > Monolinguals) were detected in the left HG (MNI coordinates: x = −52, y = −13, z = 5; t = 3.52; cluster size = 65 mm3) and in the right anterior/middle cingulum (MNI coordinates: x = 9, y = −1, z = 38; t = 4.69; cluster size = 649 mm3). The inverse contrast Monolinguals > Bilinguals did not yield any difference even at the p < 0.001 uncorrected level. Restricting the analysis (using small volume correction) within the left and right HG areas defined from the toolbox WFU Pickatlas 2.5.2 (Maldjian et al., 2003) yielded significant group differences [(p FWE corrected for multiple comparisons (pFWE-corr) < 0.05] in GM volume in the left HG (MNI coordinates: x = −52, y = −13, z = 6; t = 3.52; cluster size = 41 mm3; pFWE-corr = 0.02). No significant differences were observed between bilinguals and monolinguals in similar analyses of white matter. The inverse contrast Monolinguals > Bilinguals did not yield any significant difference. The inclusion of gross morphologies of left and right Heschl gyri (coding: 1 = single, 1.5 = split, 2 = duplicate, 3 = triple) as additional covariates did not alter these results (MNI coordinates: x = −52, y = −13, z = 6; t = 3.48; cluster size = 33 mm3; pFWE-corr = 0.03).

Discussion

In the present study, we investigated the influence of early language exposure on brain morphology. We found significantly larger HG volumes in bilinguals compared with monolingual participants. This was true both for gray and white matter volumes, for both hemispheres, and for measurements in native and normalized spaces. These results are worth comparing with previous studies looking at effects of language-training experience (Golestani et al., 2011) and language learning (Crinion et al., 2009).

Golestani et al. (2011), comparing expert phoneticians to controls, reported differences in HG morphology and in the size of the pars opercularis. Yet, the effect of years of practice was only observed in the pars opercularis. The authors argue that HG morphology (especially gyrification) cannot be modified by linguistic experience or training in adulthood (Golestani et al., 2011). Here, we found that early linguistic experience (being a bilingual or a monolingual) had an impact on the volumes of HG. We also observed a trend for more splitting of HG in monolinguals than in bilinguals that could reflect early experience-dependent plasticity. It remains an open question whether similar patterns could be found between late second-language learners and monolinguals.

Language characteristics can also influence brain morphology. Crinion et al. (2009) have shown structural differences in bilingual and monolingual speakers of Chinese and English, studying differences in brain morphology due to expertise. They were especially interested in identifying differences in pitch perception and production of these two languages of tonal (Chinese) and nontonal (English) nature. The results of the study showed an increase in gray and white matter in the right temporal lobe and left insula in those subjects who spoke Chinese compared with those who did not. Both groups, native Chinese and European subjects who learned Chinese as a second language, showed this effect, discarding an ethnicity bias. Similar to Crinion et al. (2009), in our study we were interested in brain morphological differences due to expertise controlling for an effect of ethnicity. Additionally, we controlled for language characteristics. We tested participants from Spain who were monolingual (Spanish) or bilingual (Spanish and Catalan). Here it is important to mention that both languages in our study are not tonal and are typologically closely related, which gives additional information on brain differences with respect to language characteristics. We suggest that our results show a clear difference in HG volumes between monolinguals and bilinguals, which can be related to lifelong expertise in language (monolingual/bilingual) and eliminates other factors like language characteristics in pitch perception (tonal/nontonal), ethnicity, or late second language learning.

As mentioned in the introduction and closer to the present study, Mechelli et al. (2004) compared bilinguals (first language: English, second language: other European language) to monolinguals using VBM and observed an increase in gray matter density in a posterior inferior parietal region that, they suggested, might be linked to vocabulary acquisition (Lee et al., 2007). A similar VBM analysis performed on our participants' scans did not detect a difference in the parietal region but confirmed the HG difference. Such a discrepancy could result from the specific similarities and differences between Spanish and Catalan. Catalan and Spanish overlap significantly at the lexical level but differ in phonology; therefore, it is likely that Spanish–Catalan bilinguals need to recruit regions close to the HG, related to the processing of phonological information (Jacquemot et al., 2003). It is important to note that both the VBM analyses and our direct measurements of HG cannot precisely target the primary auditory cortex (Rademacher et al., 2001). Therefore, we do not imply that bilinguals versus monolinguals differences are restricted to the primary auditory cortex. Beside HG, at a lower, uncorrected, statistical threshold, differences in GM are also observed in the right cingulate cortex, which has been implicated in language switching (Wang et al., 2007; Abutalebi et al., 2012).

The bilinguals in our study were early and highly skilled bilinguals; that is, they had acquired both languages in childhood and lived all their lives in a fully bilingual society. Because the two groups were not self-selected; that is, the bilinguals did not become bilinguals because they were specially gifted at learning foreign languages, one can conclude that the learning of a second language is most likely a causal factor in the increased size of the auditory cortex. It remains to be determined whether late learning, for example in adulthood, can lead to the same increase.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by grants from the Spanish Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación (JCI-2009-04492, PSI2010-20168; SEJ2009-09072, Consolider-Ingenio2010-CDS-2007-00012), the Catalan Government (SGR 2009-1521), and the French National Agency for Research (ANR 2010 BLAN 1403 01). N.S.-G. received the prize ICREA Acadèmia for excellence in research, funded by the Generalitat de Catalunya. We thank Kimberly Brink for the English correction of the manuscript.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Christophe Pallier, INSERM, CEA, Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, Neurospin Center, F-91191 Paris, France. christophe{at}pallier.org

References

  1. ↵
    1. Abdul-Kareem IA,
    2. Stancak A,
    3. Parkes LM,
    4. Sluming V
    (2011) Increased gray matter volume of left pars opercularis in male orchestral musicians correlate positively with years of musical performance. J Magn Reson Imaging 33:24–32.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Abutalebi J,
    2. Della Rosa PA,
    3. Green DW,
    4. Hernandez M,
    5. Scifo P,
    6. Keim R,
    7. Cappa SF,
    8. Costa A
    (2012) Bilingualism tunes the anterior cingulate cortex for conflict monitoring. Cereb Cortex 22:2076–2086.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    1. Ashburner J
    (2007) A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage 38:95–113.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Ashburner J,
    2. Friston KJ
    (2005) Unified segmentation. Neuroimage 26:839–851.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Carreiras M,
    2. Seghier ML,
    3. Baquero S,
    4. Estévez A,
    5. Lozano A,
    6. Devlin JT,
    7. Price CJ
    (2009) An anatomical signature for literacy. Nature 461:983–986.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Crinion JT,
    2. Green DW,
    3. Chung R,
    4. Ali N,
    5. Grogan A,
    6. Price GR,
    7. Mechelli A,
    8. Price CJ
    (2009) Neuroanatomical markers of speaking Chinese. Hum Brain Mapp 30:4108–4115.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Draganski B,
    2. Gaser C,
    3. Busch V,
    4. Schuierer G,
    5. Bogdahn U,
    6. May A
    (2004) Neuroplasticity: changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature 427:311–312.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Emmorey K,
    2. Allen JS,
    3. Bruss J,
    4. Schenker N,
    5. Damasio H
    (2003) A morphometric analysis of auditory brain regions in congenitally deaf adults. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100:10049–10054.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    1. Golestani N,
    2. Molko N,
    3. Dehaene S,
    4. LeBihan D,
    5. Pallier C
    (2007) Brain structure predicts the learning of foreign speech sounds. Cereb Cortex 17:575–582.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    1. Golestani N,
    2. Price CJ,
    3. Scott SK
    (2011) Born with an ear for dialects? Structural plasticity in the expert phonetician brain. J Neurosci 31:4213–4220.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    1. Jacquemot C,
    2. Pallier C,
    3. LeBihan D,
    4. Dehaene S,
    5. Dupoux E
    (2003) Phonological grammar shapes the auditory cortex: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci 23:9541–9546.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. ↵
    1. Kochunov P,
    2. Fox P,
    3. Lancaster J,
    4. Tan LH,
    5. Amunts K,
    6. Zilles K,
    7. Mazziotta J,
    8. Gao JH
    (2003) Localized morphological brain differences between English-speaking Caucasians and Chinese-speaking Asians: new evidence of anatomical plasticity. Neuroreport 14:961–964.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Lee H,
    2. Devlin JT,
    3. Shakeshaft C,
    4. Stewart LH,
    5. Brennan A,
    6. Glensman J,
    7. Pitcher K,
    8. Crinion J,
    9. Mechelli A,
    10. Frackowiak RS,
    11. Green DW,
    12. Price CJ
    (2007) Anatomical traces of vocabulary acquisition in the adolescent brain. J Neurosci 27:1184–1189.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    1. Maguire EA,
    2. Gadian DG,
    3. Johnsrude IS,
    4. Good CD,
    5. Ashburner J,
    6. Frackowiak RS,
    7. Frith CD
    (2000) Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97:4398–4403.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Maldjian JA,
    2. Laurienti PJ,
    3. Kraft RA,
    4. Burdette JH
    (2003) An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. Neuroimage 19:1233–1239.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Mechelli A,
    2. Crinion JT,
    3. Noppeney U,
    4. O'Doherty J,
    5. Ashburner J,
    6. Frackowiak RS,
    7. Price CJ
    (2004) Neurolinguistics: structural plasticity in the bilingual brain. Nature 431:757.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Ono M,
    2. Kubik S,
    3. Abernathy CD
    (1990) Atlas of the cerebral sulci (Thieme, Stuttgart).
  18. ↵
    1. Penhune VB,
    2. Zatorre RJ,
    3. MacDonald JD,
    4. Evans AC
    (1996) Interhemispheric anatomical differences in human primary auditory cortex: probabilistic mapping and volume measurement from magnetic resonance scans. Cereb Cortex 6:661–672.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. Rademacher J,
    2. Morosan P,
    3. Schormann T,
    4. Schleicher A,
    5. Werner C,
    6. Freund HJ,
    7. Zilles K
    (2001) Probabilistic mapping and volume measurement of human primary auditory cortex. Neuroimage 13:669–683.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Schneider P,
    2. Scherg M,
    3. Dosch HG,
    4. Specht HJ,
    5. Gutschalk A,
    6. Rupp A
    (2002) Morphology of Heschl's gyrus reflects enhanced activation in the auditory cortex of musicians. Nat Neurosci 5:688–694.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Schneider P,
    2. Andermann M,
    3. Wengenroth M,
    4. Goebel R,
    5. Flor H,
    6. Rupp A,
    7. Diesch E
    (2009) Reduced volume of Heschl's gyrus in tinnitus. Neuroimage 45:927–939.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Sutherland ME,
    2. Zatorre RJ,
    3. Watkins KE,
    4. Hervé PY,
    5. Leonard G,
    6. Pike BG,
    7. Witton C,
    8. Paus T
    (2012) Anatomical correlates of dynamic auditory processing: relationship to literacy during early adolescence. Neuroimage 60:1287–1295.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Wang Y,
    2. Xue G,
    3. Chen C,
    4. Xue F,
    5. Dong Q
    (2007) Neural bases of asymmetric language switching in second-language learners: an ER-fMRI study. Neuroimage 35:862–870.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Wong PC,
    2. Warrier CM,
    3. Penhune VB,
    4. Roy AK,
    5. Sadehh A,
    6. Parrish TB,
    7. Zatorre RJ
    (2008) Volume of left Heschl's gyrus and linguistic pitch learning. Cereb Cortex 18:828–836.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    1. Yoon U,
    2. Perusse D,
    3. Lee JM,
    4. Evans AC
    (2011) Genetic and environmental influences on structural variability of the brain in pediatric twin: deformation based morphometry. Neurosci Lett 493:8–13.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 32 (47)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 32, Issue 47
21 Nov 2012
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
An Effect of Bilingualism on the Auditory Cortex
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
An Effect of Bilingualism on the Auditory Cortex
Volker Ressel, Christophe Pallier, Noelia Ventura-Campos, Begoña Díaz, Abeba Roessler, César Ávila, Núria Sebastián-Gallés
Journal of Neuroscience 21 November 2012, 32 (47) 16597-16601; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1996-12.2012

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
An Effect of Bilingualism on the Auditory Cortex
Volker Ressel, Christophe Pallier, Noelia Ventura-Campos, Begoña Díaz, Abeba Roessler, César Ávila, Núria Sebastián-Gallés
Journal of Neuroscience 21 November 2012, 32 (47) 16597-16601; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1996-12.2012
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Heteromodal Cortical Areas Encode Sensory-Motor Features of Word Meaning
  • Pharmacologically Counteracting a Phenotypic Difference in Cerebellar GABAA Receptor Response to Alcohol Prevents Excessive Alcohol Consumption in a High Alcohol-Consuming Rodent Genotype
  • Neuromuscular NMDA Receptors Modulate Developmental Synapse Elimination
Show more Brief Communications
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2022 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.