Abstract
The pond snail Lymnaea stagnalis is capable of learning taste aversion and consolidating this learning into long-term memory (LTM) that is called conditioned taste aversion (CTA). Previous studies showed that some molluscan insulin-related peptides (MIPs) were upregulated in snails exhibiting CTA. We thus hypothesized that MIPs play an important role in neurons underlying the CTA–LTM consolidation process. To examine this hypothesis, we first observed the distribution of MIP II, a major peptide of MIPs, and MIP receptor and determined the amounts of their mRNAs in the CNS. MIP II was only observed in the light green cells in the cerebral ganglia, but the MIP receptor was distributed throughout the entire CNS, including the buccal ganglia. Next, when we applied exogenous mammalian insulin, secretions from MIP-containing cells or partially purified MIPs, to the isolated CNS, we observed a long-term change in synaptic efficacy (i.e., enhancement) of the synaptic connection between the cerebral giant cell (a key interneuron for CTA) and the B1 motor neuron (a buccal motor neuron). This synaptic enhancement was blocked by application of an insulin receptor antibody to the isolated CNS. Finally, injection of the insulin receptor antibody into the snail before CTA training, while not blocking the acquisition of taste aversion learning, blocked the memory consolidation process; thus, LTM was not observed. These data suggest that MIPs trigger changes in synaptic connectivity that may be correlated with the consolidation of taste aversion learning into CTA–LTM in the Lymnaea CNS.
Introduction
Formation of long-term memory (LTM) after associative learning is dependent on both protein synthesis and altered gene activity in neurons that play a critical role in memory formation (Inda et al., 2005; Lee et al., 2008; Rosenegger et al., 2010). The pond snail Lymnaea stagnalis is a good model in which to elucidate the causal mechanisms that underlie LTM formation (Ito et al., 1999, 2012a; Sakakibara, 2006; Lukowiak et al., 2008; Nikitin et al., 2008; Kemenes and Benjamin, 2009). In conditioned taste aversion (CTA), a form of associative learning, an appetitive stimulus (sucrose) is used as the conditioned stimulus (CS), and an aversive stimulus (KCl) is used as the unconditioned stimulus (US). The CS increases the feeding response in snails, whereas the US inhibits feeding. In CTA training, the CS is paired with the US. After repeated paired presentations, the CS no longer elicits the feeding response, and this aversive conditioning persists as LTM (Kojima et al., 1996).
We identified candidate genes necessary for the establishment of CTA–LTM in Lymnaea and found that some genes were upregulated while others were downregulated (Azami et al., 2006). Some of the upregulated genes after LTM consolidation were the molluscan insulin-related peptide (MIP I, II, and others) genes. However, it is unclear whether MIPs are necessary for memory consolidation, and if they are, what is their role in the consolidation process.
Peptide purification of MIP I–III and V and the additional finding of a MIP VII transcript indicate that five types of MIPs function in Lymnaea (Li et al., 1992a, b, c; Smit et al., 1991, 1993, 1996). Protein structures for the insulin superfamily peptides in invertebrates and vertebrates are also well conserved in these MIPs, and their expression is observed in the growth-controlling neuroendocrine light green cells (LGCs) and canopy cells of the cerebral ganglia (Meester et al., 1992; Smit et al., 1992, 1998). The cDNA structure of a putative tyrosine kinase receptor for MIPs has also been clarified (Roovers et al., 1995). Many of the typical insulin receptor features, including a cysteine-rich domain, a single transmembrane domain, and a tyrosine kinase domain, are also conserved in the predicted 1607 amino acid protein in Lymnaea. Extensive screening of cDNA and genomic libraries together with Southern blot analyses have revealed the presence of a single putative MIP receptor gene and suggested that different MIPs bind to the same receptor (Smit et al., 1996).
In the present study, we sought to clarify possible roles of MIPs in the CTA memory consolidation process. We first delineated the distribution of MIP II and MIP receptor mRNAs in the CNS in Lymnaea. We then investigated the effect of the exogenous application of partially purified MIPs on plasticity of synapses that play a role in CTA. Finally, we inhibited behavioral CTA–LTM by injection of the insulin receptor antibody into the snail before training. Our data are consistent with the hypothesis that MIPs play a key role in memory consolidation.
Materials and Methods
Snails.
Specimens of L. stagnalis (L.) with a 15–25 mm shell [young adults (Sadamoto et al., 2000)] were obtained from our snail-rearing facility (original stocks from Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam). All snails were maintained in dechlorinated tap water (i.e., pond water) under a 12 h light/dark cycle at 20°C and fed ad libitum on a kind of turnip leaf, Brassica rapa var. peruviridis [Komatsuna (in Japanese)], and a spiral shell food (Nisso) every other day. Lymnaea exhibit good growth and reproduction under these feeding conditions. Snails were anesthetized with 25% Listerine before dissection (Kojima et al., 1997). To obtain good marks for behavioral training, snails were not fed for 1 d in the behavioral experiments (Sugai et al., 2007). This protocol motivates snails for taste aversion training.
In situ hybridization.
As one example, we performed in situ hybridization for MIP II among MIPs. We prepared the digoxigenin (DIG)-labeled cRNA probes for MIP II antisense, MIP II sense, MIP receptor antisense, and MIP receptor sense. The sequences of MIP II and MIP receptor mRNAs were obtained from the NCBI Nucleotide database (MIP II, X59302; MIP receptor, X84994). The PCR products of MIP II (466 bp) and MIP receptor (445 or 528 bp) were subcloned with TOPO TA Cloning kit dual promoter (Invitrogen). After linearization with EcoRV or BamHI, the cRNA probes were synthesized by run-off transcription from T7 promoter for the MIP II sense and the MIP receptor antisense or from Sp6 promoter for the MIP II antisense and the MIP receptor sense with DIG RNA labeling mixture (Roche Diagnostics).
The CNSs were dissected from snails in ice-cold Lymnaea saline. The Lymnaea saline consisted of (in mm) 50 NaCl, 1.6 KCl, 2.0 MgCl2, 3.5 CaCl2, and 10 HEPES, pH 7.9 (Straub et al., 2002). The dissected CNSs in OCT compound (Sakura Finetek, Tokyo, Japan) were frozen in liquid nitrogen. Then the specimens were cut on a cryostat. The horizontal frozen sections (10–14 μm thick) were thaw-mounted on glass slides precoated with Vecta-Bond (Vector Laboratories). The method of in situ hybridization was modified from Matsuo and Ito (2009). The sections were fixed in 10% formaldehyde neutral buffer, pH 7, rinsed with PBS, and treated with 0.3% Triton X-100 and 0.2 m HCl. Then the sections were treated with 2 μg/ml proteinase K, fixed in 10% formaldehyde neutral buffer, pH 7, and treated with 2 mg/ml glycine in PBS. After prehybridization in 5× SSC containing 50% formamide, the hybridization was performed at 52°C overnight in a hybridization buffer containing 50% formamide, 5× SSC, 100 μg/ml yeast tRNA, 0.1% Tween 20, 50 μg/ml heparine, and DIG-labeled cRNA probes. Subsequently, the slides were rinsed in 50% formamide in 4× SSC at 52°C, treated with RNase A (0.02 mg/ml) in buffer (0.5 m NaCl, 10 mm Tris-HCl, pH 7.5) for 30 min at 37°C, and washed sequentially with 50% formamide in 1× SSC for 1 h at 52°C. The slides were rinsed three times in buffer (0.1 m maleic acid, pH 7.5, and 0.15 m NaCl) at room temperature for a total of 10 min and rinsed in buffer [1% blocking reagent (Roche Diagnostics), 0.1 m maleic acid, pH 7.5, and 0.15 m NaCl] for 30 min. Hybridization signals were visualized using a nucleic acid detection kit (Roche Diagnostics) with alkaline phosphatase-conjugated anti-DIG antibody and nitroblue tetrazolium, according to the manufacturer's instructions.
Real-time PCR.
The real-time PCR primers of MIP II (forward, 5′-gggccaatcatcttgcagttt-3′; reverse, 5′-ggaagccagccaaattcga-3′) and MIP receptor (forward, 5′-agacagactactatagaaaaggaggtaaagga-3′; reverse, 5′-acaactccatatgaccaaacatctga-3′) were designed using Primer Express Software version 3.0 (Applied Biosystems).
The CNSs were dissected out from anesthetized Lymnaea, separated into three parts (buccal ganglia, cerebral ganglia, and other ganglia), and immediately frozen in liquid nitrogen. Total RNA of each part of the CNS was extracted using the RNAqueous-Micro kit (Applied Biosystems/Ambion). Twenty microliters of cDNA samples were obtained by reverse transcription of 50 ng of total RNA that was taken from each part of the CNS. Real-time PCR was performed on the 7900HT Fast real-time PCR system (Applied Biosystems). We applied the absolute quantification method with the DyNAmo SYBR Green qPCR kit with ROX passive reference dye (Finnzymes). We also prepared standard samples [each solution containing 0.5 × 102 − 0.5 × 106 copies of plasmid DNA (MIP II or MIP receptor)]. Each 10 μl reaction contained 5 μl of Master Mix (Finnzymes), 0.5 μm forward primer, 0.5 μm reverse primer, 0.2 μl of 50× ROX passive reference dye, and 1 μl of sample (cDNA sample or plasmid DNA standard sample). The PCR conditions were as follows: 1 cycle at 95°C for 10 min, followed by 40 cycles at 95°C for 15 s and 58°C for 1 min. After amplification, dissociation curves were obtained to ensure the specificity of PCR.
Secretions from the light green cells.
The isolated CNS with the median lip nerves that include the axons of the LGCs (Li et al., 1992c) were pinned down in the chamber containing 5 ml of Lymnaea saline that included a protease inhibitor, 5 μm Pefabloc SC (4-(2-aminoethyl) benzenesulfonyl fluoride hydrochloride) (Roche Diagnostics). The median lip nerve was electrically stimulated with the hook electrodes every 5 s for 4 h at room temperature. One set of electrical stimulation consisted of 10 pulses with the amplitude of 1 V, a duration of 50 ms, and an interpulse interval of 100 ms. This solution was filtered to retain MIPs, whose molecular mass is 6.9 kDa. Therefore, two ultrafiltration membranes, 3000 and 10,000 normal molecular weight limit (Ultra-0.5 ml and YM-10; Millipore), were used to collect proteins weighing between 3 and 10 kDa. The 50 μl of condensed solution, which was finally adjusted to contain the secretions from two CNSs, was used for the following electrophysiological studies. This solution was now called the “CNS factor solution” and was expected to contain MIPs. Furthermore, in some of our experiments, we immersed the CNSs with the median lip nerves in a small amount of Lymnaea saline at 4°C for >20 h and obtained the CNS factor solution. The above two methods seemed to offer the same results in the electrophysiological experiments.
On the other hand, the CNSs without the cerebral ganglia (i.e., the CNSs without the LGCs or median lip nerves) were immersed in a small amount of Lymnaea saline at 4°C for >20 h. This solution was used as the control solution. As well as the CNS factor solution, the 50 μl solution, which was adjusted to contain the secretions from two CNSs, was used for the electrophysiological studies.
SDS-PAGE.
SDS-PAGE was performed using the NuPAGE electrophoresis system (Invitrogen). Reduced samples that contained the CNS factor solution or the control solution were electrophoresized on NuPAGE 10% gels with MES–SDS running buffer. Proteins in gels were stained with Oriole gel stain (Bio-Rad) at room temperature for 90 min. Stained gels were photographed with a LAS3000 digital imaging analyzer (GE Healthcare). We should note that protein amounts on the gels could not be normalized for both the CNS factor solution sample and the control solution sample by measurement of an absorbance of 280 nm. This issue was because the secretions from the CNS contained not only proteins but also miscellaneous things. Therefore, we adjusted the application amounts to the case that the condensed bands for the control solution samples were clearly visible on the gels.
To further confirm whether the cerebral ganglia possess the same small peptide as that secreted from the median lip nerves as mentioned above, we performed another SDS-PAGE using the CNS homogenate by tricine SDS-PAGE. The CNSs were dissected out from six snails and homogenized in 20 μl of 0.1 m acetic acid on ice. After centrifugation at 15,000 rpm at 4°C for 10 min, the lysate was applied for tricine SDS-PAGE. As the control sample, the CNS lysate without the cerebral ganglia was prepared in the same way. The CNS lysate containing 18 μg of total protein was subjected to tricine SDS-PAGE. The reduced samples containing the CNS lysate were electrophoresized on 16% polyacrylamide gel with the Tris-tricine buffer system. Proteins in gels were stained with Oriole gel stain (Bio-Rad) at room temperature for 90 min. The stained gels were photographed with a LAS3000 digital imaging analyzer (GE Healthcare). Human insulin was purchased from MP Bio Japan.
Purification of MIPs.
Extraction and purification of MIPs were conducted, in principle, following the procedures described by Li et al. (1992a). Forty-seven median lip nerves, stored at −80°C, were sonicated in 1 ml of 1 m acetic acid over ice and centrifuged for 15 min at 21,500 × g at 4°C. The supernatant was collected, and the precipitate was extracted again with 0.5 ml of 1 m acetic acid and centrifuged. The supernatants were combined and applied to a C18 solid-phase extraction column (Sep-Pak Vac 6cc; Waters). The bound materials were eluted with a mixture of 60% acetonitrile (AcN)/7 mm trifluoroacetic acid (TFA) and subjected to high-performance gel permeation chromatography (HPGPC). The materials were injected into a column (TSKgel G2000SW, 7.5 × 300 mm; TOSOH) equilibrated with 30% AcN 7 mm TFA and eluted at a flow rate of 1 ml/min. Fractions were collected every 1 min. The fractions with the same elution position of bovine insulin were collected, freeze dried, and redissolved in 120 μl of 30% AcN 7 mm TFA. This sample was subjected to reverse-phase high-performance chromatography (rpHPLC) using a C18 column (TSKgel ODS-120T, 4.6 × 250 mm; TOSOH) equilibrated with 7 mm TFA at a flow rate of 1 ml/min. The concentration of AcN in the 7.0 mm TFA was raised to 21% in 5 min, to 39% in an additional 35 min, to 45% in 5 min, followed by a raise to 60% in the final 5 min. One-minute fractions were collected, and a fraction regarded to contain partially purified MIPs was concentrated and used for electrophysiological experiments. The amount of peptides in the concentrated fraction was measured with the Quant-iT Protein Assay kit (Invitrogen). The same purification procedures were repeated again starting with 141 median lip nerves, and the rpHPLC fractions with the same elution pattern were obtained. The fraction regarded to contain partially purified MIPs was subjected to mass spectrometric analyses.
Mass spectrometry.
The rpHPLC fractions were analyzed using an autoflex III matrix-assisted laser desorption/ionization–time of flight/time of flight mass spectrometry (MALDI-TOF/TOF MS; Bruker Daltonik) according to the method of Ohnishi et al. (2009). To confirm a presence of disulfide bonds, peptides were pretreated according to the method of Fukuyama et al. (2006).
Electrophysiology.
We examined the properties of the chemical synapses between the cerebral giant cell (CGC) and its ipsilateral B1 motor neuron in the isolated Lymnaea CNS. The paired CGCs play a key role in the nervous control of the feeding system in Lymnaea (McCrohan and Benjamin, 1980; Kyriakides and McCrohan, 1989; Kojima et al., 1997, 2001; Nakamura et al., 1999a; Elliott and Vehovszky, 2000; Straub et al., 2007; Yeoman et al., 2008; Ito et al., 2012a), and the B1 motor neuron receives a monosynaptic excitatory input from the CGC (McCrohan and Benjamin, 1980; Kawai et al., 2011), is activated during the radula-protraction phase in the feeding cycle (Rose and Benjamin, 1979), and innervates the salivary gland (Benjamin et al., 1979). The CGC also plays a major role in the mediation of LTM for both food aversive and food appetitive learning (Kojima et al., 1997; Benjamin et al., 2000). Kojima et al. (1997) showed that activity in the CGC caused a large and long-lasting polysynaptic inhibitory effect on the N1M cell, which is one of the feeding central pattern generator neurons, in snails exhibiting CTA–LTM. This can be explained by action of the cAMP–protein kinase A (PKA)–cAMP response element-binding protein (CREB) pathway in the CGC (Nakamura et al., 1999b; Sadamoto et al., 2004, 2010, 2011; Wagatsuma et al., 2005, 2006). These results suggest that CGC-B1 synapses serve as a good model in which to elucidate how changes in synaptic transmission amplitude, for example, serve as a neural correlate of behavioral memory.
For the experiments with commercially available bovine insulin (Sigma-Aldrich) and the CNS factor solution, we stimulated the CGC for 2 s by the injection of depolarizing current into the neuron by a current-clamp method and recorded the compound EPSP in the B1 motor neuron. Because a single EPSP in the B1 motor neuron was very small (<3 mV), we often failed to detect it depending on the preparation. We thus noticed a large compound EPSP in the B1 motor neuron. We adjusted the intensity of the injection current to the CGC on the initial trial in the normal Lymnaea saline to produce 9–11 spikes. This procedure was repeated three times to obtain an average EPSP amplitude. The bovine insulin was applied at the concentration of 5 μm. The stock solution of bovine insulin was prepared as 2 mg/ml (i.e., 349 μm) in 1 mm HCl, and thus the final concentration of HCl included in the bovine insulin solution was 14 μm. After incubation with 5 μm bovine insulin in the Lymnaea saline for 15, 30, 45, 60, 75, 90, and 120 min, we electrically stimulated the CGC for 2 s and recorded the compound EPSP in the B1 motor neuron. The intensity of electric stimulation was not changed for each CNS sample during a series of experiments. As the control experiments, we dissolved 14 μm HCl as the vehicle into the saline. The experiments for bovine insulin or the HCl control solution were performed in the chamber of 500 μl.
For the experiments using the CNS factor solution, we used a small chamber, 50 μl rather than the 500 μl chamber used in the bovine insulin experiments. That is, this 50 μl solution included the secretions prepared from two CNSs. We incubated the isolated CNS with the CNS factor solution or the control solution to examine whether synaptic efficacy would change at the synapses between the CGC and the B1 motor neuron. The control solution used in this experimental set consisted of secretions from the CNSs without the cerebral ganglia.
To further confirm the effects of activation of MIP receptor on the synaptic efficacy between the CGC and the B1 motor neuron, we designed an experiment using insulin receptor antibody. We used mouse monoclonal [47-9] antibody to an insulin receptor α subunit (ab982; Abcam) for the following three reasons.
1. Insulin receptors are so homologous cross phyla, even in human (Jonas et al., 1996). In particular, ligand-binding sites are well conserved (see CAA59353 for Lymnaea, AAA59174 for human, and AAA39318 for mouse).
2. The antibody used here was produced when human insulin receptor was used as the antigen (Soos et al., 1986).
3. This antibody recognizes the extracellular domain of human insulin receptor and acts as an antagonist. Thus, this antibody blocks the binding between insulin and human insulin receptor (Taylor et al., 1987).
Thus, we thought that this insulin receptor antibody is suitable for the blockade of MIP receptor in Lymnaea (Ito et al., 2012b). To block the function of MIP receptor, we incubated the isolated CNS with this insulin receptor antibody in Lymnaea saline at the final concentration of 25.6 μg/ml (175 nm) for 90 min before the electrophysiological recording and changed this antibody solution to the CNS factor solution that still contained the antibody of 25.6 μg/ml (175 nm) for the electrophysiological experiments.
The experiments using the partially purified MIP solution were performed in a similar manner as those for the CNS factor solution. There were only three differences: (1) the recording chamber was coated with 5% bovine serum albumin (BSA); (2) 0.1% BSA was also applied to the CNS with the partially purified MIP solution; and (3) the concentration of an insulin receptor antibody in Lymnaea saline was 2.56 μg/ml (17.5 nm) because the concentration of partially purified MIPs was so low (100 nm) compared with that of bovine insulin (5 μm). The first two procedures were performed to prevent MIPs from attaching to the plastic recording chambers. The application method for the insulin receptor antibody was the same.
To digest MIPs and other peptides in the CNS factor solution, we incubated the CNS factor solution with 10 μm trypsin (T-1426; Sigma-Aldrich) at room temperature for 30 min. Then we applied 100 μm trypsin inhibitor (from soybean; Wako) to the trypsin-treated solution to stop the action of trypsin, at room temperature for 10 min. Such concentrations and durations of trypsin and trypsin inhibitor were decided by confirming that 5 μm bovine insulin was digested with these chemicals. As a control experiment for a trypsin treatment, we applied only 100 μm trypsin inhibitor to the CNS factor solution. This experiment was done to prove that a trypsin inhibitor did not affect the synaptic transmission.
Previously, it was demonstrated using mammalian cell lines (i.e., HEK 293 and CHO cells) that the expressed Lymnaea MIP receptor was not cleaved into α and β chains and it was autophosphorylated even in a nonligand bound state (Smit et al., 1998). These results were thought to have been due to incorrect processing and sorting of MIP receptor in the mammalian cells. In other words, the expressed structure of the MIP receptor is abnormal in a mammalian cell line. Thus, we cannot provide evidence for the binding of bovine insulin to the Lymnaea MIP receptor and the specificity of the antibody by expressing the Lymnaea MIP receptor in an exogenous mammalian cell line.
Training paradigm for taste aversion learning.
To examine whether the insulin receptor antibody purchased commercially blocks behavioral LTM, we injected the insulin receptor antibody [final concentration of 2.56 μg/ml (17.5 nm)] in Lymnaea saline into the abdominal cavity of a snail. In this procedure, the total volume of this injection solution (i.e., antibody plus saline) was 40 μl. The calculated volume of hemolymph in a snail with a 20 mm shell length was 0.4 ml. One hour after injection of the antibody, the training, the pretest, and the posttests for LTM were performed in polystyrene Petri dishes (diameter, 35 mm) (Sugai et al., 2006). All snails were first given a pretest. In this observation period (1 min), the number of feeding responses (rasping movements of the buccal mass) was counted in distilled water after a 15 s application of 10 mm sucrose (the CS) to the lips of the snail. Taste aversion learning in Lymnaea was brought about by pairing the 10 mm sucrose CS and the 10 mm KCl US. The duration of both the CS and the US was 15 s, with an interstimulus interval between the onsets of the CS and US of 15 s. A 10 min intertrial interval was interposed between each pairing of the CS–US. Snails received 10 paired CS–US trials. The timing and duration of injection of the insulin receptor antibody are indicated in Results and in the figure legends.
We used a backward conditioning (US–CS) control group and a naive control group to validate associative learning. For the naive control group, only distilled water was applied to the lips instead of the CS and US. In the posttest sessions, snails were again challenged with the CS, and the number of bites was recorded in the 1 min interval in distilled water after a 15 s application of the CS. All tests were performed blindly. As a control for the insulin receptor antibody, we injected the saline or 1.92 μg of mouse IgG (whole molecule; Jackson ImmunoResearch Laboratories) into the abdominal cavity. This weight was the same as that of the insulin receptor antibody.
Statistics.
The data are expressed as mean ± SEM. For the electrophysiological data, the significant difference at p < 0.05 between two groups was examined by two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, and that between the data at each time point in two groups was examined by t test. For the behavioral data, the significant difference was examined by one-way ANOVA and the post hoc Scheffé's test.
Results
In situ hybridization of MIP II and MIP receptor
The distribution of MIP II mRNA in the Lymnaea CNS has been shown previously (Meester et al., 1992; Smit et al., 1992), but that of the MIP receptor has not. With regard to the MIP receptor, only the deduced amino acids were reported (Roovers et al., 1995). We performed in situ hybridization for both MIP II and MIP receptor and compared their distributions (Figs. 1, 2).
As expected for MIP II, only the cells in the cerebral ganglia showed the in situ hybridization signals (Fig. 1A). On the basis of their size and position, we identified these cells as the neuroendocrine LGCs (as per Meester et al., 1992). When using the sense probe for MIP II, we did not find any in situ hybridization signals (Fig. 1B). In contrast to the results with MIP II signals, the in situ hybridization signals for the MIP receptor consisted of many pale purple “dots” that were found in neurons throughout the CNS (Fig. 2). The dots were localized around the nucleus of a cell body, e.g., the CGC (Fig. 2A), whereas no signal was observed for preparations using the sense probe (Fig. 2B). A similar pattern of signals (i.e., dots) was observed surrounding the nucleus of the B1 motor neuron (Fig. 2C). However, a positive signal was not observed in the neuropil regions (Fig. 2D). On the basis of these findings, we concluded that these dots represented the distribution of MIP receptor mRNA.
As we were most interested in determining whether insulin altered the ability of snails to learn and form memory (i.e., CTA–LTM), we paid particular attention to the buccal and the cerebral ganglia (Fig. 2E–H). We obtained positive signals for MIP receptor in the identified feeding motor neurons (B1, B2, and B3) in the buccal ganglia (Fig. 2E,F). The CGC and other neurons in the cerebral ganglia were also found to contain the MIP receptor mRNA (Fig. 2G). Control experiments using the sense probe did not show any positive signal in the cerebral ganglia (Fig. 2H).
Real-time PCR of MIP II and MIP receptor
The in situ hybridization data showed that MIP II was only expressed in the cerebral ganglia, whereas MIP receptor was expressed throughout the entire CNS. Because the MIP receptor is a candidate receptor for all the MIPs including MIP II, and because MIPs are secreted in a hormone fashion, it is not surprising that the MIP receptor is expressed on neurons throughout the CNS.
To further confirm this assumption, we used real-time PCR for the quantification of these mRNAs (Fig. 3). We found that MIP II mRNA is localized to only the cerebral ganglia (Fig. 3A) and that the copy numbers in the other ganglia, including the buccal ganglia, are under our detection limit (<10 copies; Fig. 3B; see Wagatsuma et al., 2005). In contrast, MIP receptor mRNA was in equal numbers in the cerebral and other ganglia, including the buccal ganglia (Fig. 3A,C). That is, we conclude that the MIP receptor is expressed throughout the CNS.
Contents of secretions from the light green cells
Because MIPs are synthesized and secreted from the LGCs in the cerebral ganglia (Meester et al., 1992; Smit et al., 1992), we attempted to obtain MIPs, including MIP I and II, by stimulation of the LGCs. We expected that the stimulation of the median lip nerves would result in the secretion of MIPs, including MIP I and II, into the recording chamber (see Materials and Methods). Thus, the solution in the chamber was referred to as the CNS factor solution. To determine whether MIPs were in the CNS factor solution, we focused on the stained bands between 3000 and 6000 molecular mass on the gel for the separation of molecules contained in the CNS factor solution.
MIPs in a reduction state are divided into two chains: A-chain of 3.0 kDa and B-chain of 3.9 kDa. We found a band of ∼4 kDa only in the data for the CNS factor solution, and not in the control solution (Fig. 4A). Here we note that the control solution was prepared as the secretion of the CNSs without the cerebral ganglia that were immersed in a small amount of Lymnaea saline. This band of 4 kDa is thought to correspond to the size of the B-chain. The band of 3 kDa corresponding to the size of the A-chain could not be observed (Fig. 4A). We think that the 3 kDa peptide was so small that it was below the level of detection. We thus expected that MIPs are in the CNS factor solution.
In the tricine SDS-PAGE procedure, we also found a band of ∼4 kDa, but only in the lysate of the CNS that included the cerebral ganglia (i.e., LGCs present) and not in the control sample (i.e., LGCs not present; Fig. 4B). This band of 4 kDa is thought to correspond to the B-chain of some MIPs. The 3 kDa band corresponding to the A-chain could not be observed. We conclude that the small peptide secreted from the median lip nerves (Fig. 4A) is the same as that contained in the cerebral ganglia (Fig. 4B).
Effects of insulin on long-term synaptic plasticity
Our previous results showed that some MIP genes were upregulated in snails exhibiting “good” CTA–LTM (Azami et al., 2006). Here we examined the effects of MIPs and bovine insulin on synaptic plasticity in the Lymnaea CNS. In particular, we focused on changes in synaptic efficacy that occur at the CGC-B1 synapses (Nakamura et al., 1999b).
Because previous studies using another mollusc Aplysia demonstrated that application of 5 μm bovine insulin activated the bag cell–neuron insulin receptor by stimulating its autophosphorylation on tyrosine residues (Jonas et al., 1996) and evoked the egg-laying hormone secretion (Jonas et al., 1997), we first used bovine insulin in our experiments to attempt to induce synaptic plasticity and then used the CNS factor solution (Fig. 5). Like insulin in vertebrates, MIPs are composed of two short amino acid chains, which are A-chain and B-chain linked by disulphide bridges, and its structure was thought to resemble that of bovine insulin (Kits et al., 1990; Li et al., 1992b).
When the CNS was incubated with 5 μm bovine insulin, a significantly larger EPSP was found compared with the control ones (Fig. 5A; p < 0.05, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA). The significantly larger EPSP was observed for at least 2 h. This significantly larger and long-lasting increase in EPSP amplitude allowed us to conclude that a long-term change in synaptic efficacy occurred at the CGC-B1 synapses. In control experiments using just the vehicle, a significant increase in EPSP amplitude was not observed. The number of spikes evoked in the CGC over the course of the 2 h experiment decreased slightly in both the insulin and vehicle experiments (Fig. 5B). There were no significant differences in the number of spikes elicited in the bovine insulin and control experiments.
Next, we applied the CNS factor solution. Similar to what we observed with bovine insulin, there was a long-lasting increase in the amplitude of the EPSPs recorded in the B1 motor neurons (Fig. 5C).
In the control experiments, the compound EPSPs increased slightly over the course of the experiments (Fig. 5C). We found, however, that the increase in the EPSP amplitude by the CNS factor solution was statistically greater than by the control solution (Fig. 5C; p < 0.05 by two-way repeated-measures ANOVA). Because the EPSPs sometimes evoked spikes in the B1 motor neurons after 75 min, we show only data for 60 min. As seen in Figure 4, both the CNS factor and the control solutions include many proteins, saccharides, and nucleotides. We think that these molecules increase the membrane excitability of the B1 motor neurons. In any case, both bovine insulin and the CNS factor solution alter synaptic efficacy at the CGC-B1 synapses.
When we incubated the isolated CNS with the insulin receptor antibody for 90 min before the electrophysiological experiments and added the CNS factor solution, the EPSP recorded in the B1 motor neurons was not enhanced but, rather, depressed (Fig. 5C; p < 0.05, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA). That is, the insulin receptor antibody [25.6 μg/ml (175 nm)] blocked the long-term change in synaptic efficacy at the synapses that are probably involved in CTA–LTM.
The number of spikes elicited in the CGC over the course of the experiment did not change whether the CNS factor or the control solution was used (Fig. 5D). Together, we conclude that both bovine insulin and the CNS factor solution cause enhancement of the CGC-B1 synaptic connection. Moreover, the enhanced EPSP amplitude persists for at least 1 h. Our preliminary experiments also showed that such a long-term synaptic change, which was caused by a 10 min application of bovine insulin, continued after washout of the solution.
Next, we digested MIPs and other peptides in the CNS factor solution by trypsin to show that trypsin-treated CNS factor solution does not induce a long-term synaptic change in the Lymnaea CNS. We prepared two solutions. The first one consisted of the CNS factor solution plus trypsin plus trypsin inhibitor. That is, we incubated the CNS factor solution with 10 μm trypsin and applied 100 μm trypsin inhibitor to stop the action of trypsin. The other one consisted of the CNS factor solution plus trypsin inhibitor. This solution was used for a control experiment to confirm that a trypsin inhibitor does not affect the synaptic transmission. As expected, when we used the CNS factor solution plus trypsin plus trypsin inhibitor, the CGC-B1 synaptic connection was not enhanced (Fig. 5E), whereas when we used the CNS factor solution plus trypsin inhibitor, the synaptic enhancement occurred. These data are consistent with our hypothesis that MIPs and possibly other proteins in the CNS factor solution are capable of inducing enhancement of the CGC-B1 synapses.
We then determined whether bovine insulin or the CNS factor solution altered the resting membrane potential or other intrinsic membrane properties of the CGC or the B1 motor neuron. With bath application of either bovine insulin or the CNS factor solution, no change was observed in either cell type.
Effects of partially purified MIPs on long-term synaptic plasticity
We partially purified MIPs from median lip nerve extracts. An acetic acid extract (1 m) of median lip nerves was prepurified with the C18 extraction column and size fractionated using HPGPC (Fig. 6A). The fractions with the same elution position of bovine insulin were freeze-dried and rechromatographed using rpHPLC (Fig. 6B). Two peaks in a fraction eluted at 26–27 min after the injection were thought to contain MIP I and II based on the elution conditions of those peptides reported by Li et al. (1992a) (Fig. 6B, inset).
The two peaks in a fraction eluted at 26–27 min were analyzed independently by mass spectrometry (Fig. 6C, top and bottom, respectively). The molecular ion peak of the eluted peptide at 26 min in the spectrum was 6597.18 (m/z) (Fig. 6C, top), and two ion peaks, 2502.97 and 3690.10 were obtained after cleaving disulfide bonds. Thus, we concluded that this peptide is MIP I compared with the calculated value from the MIP I, 6587.52. Three molecular ion peaks, 6156.14, 6591.09, and 6772.30 (m/z), were observed at the eluted peptide of 27 min (Fig. 6C, bottom). The values of 6591.09 and 6772.30 were close to those of MIP I (6587.52) and MIP III (6752.76). Thus, we concluded that this fraction contained mainly MIP I and III.
We called the above fraction obtained by rpHPLC “the MIP solution” (Fig. 6B), in which MIP I was identified by mass spectrometry (Fig. 6C), and we decided to use it for the additional electrophysiological studies. We thus applied the MIP solution to the isolated CNS and determined whether it caused the synaptic enhancement. Because the MIP receptor is known to be of one kind in Lymnaea (Roovers et al., 1995; Smit et al., 1998), any MIPs would bind to the MIP receptor. We thus expected that this MIP solution would enhance the EPSPs in the B1 motor neurons. Moreover, we expected that this enhancement would be blocked by the simultaneous application of insulin receptor antibody. Similar to what we observed with bovine insulin or with the CNS factor solution, there was a long-lasting increase in the amplitude of the EPSPs recorded in the B1 motor neurons with the MIP solution (Fig. 6D,E). After examination of various concentrations of the MIP solution, we found that application of 100 nm MIP solution evoked long-term synaptic enhancement at the synapses between the CGCs and the B1 motor neurons. Finally, there was a significant difference between the data for the MIP solution and those for the MIP solution with the insulin receptor antibody [2.56 μg/ml (17.5 nm)] or for the insulin receptor antibody only (Fig. 6E; p < 0.01, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA) between the time of 0 and 60 min.
Effects of insulin receptor antibody on LTM consolidation
If our hypothesis is correct that insulin-like molecules (i.e., MIPs) play a significant role in memory consolidation, we should be able to block LTM formation by using the insulin receptor antibody as we did in the synaptic enhancement experiments described above. First, however, we performed two control experiments. In these experiments, we injected saline (0.04 ml) or IgG (1.92 μg in 0.04 ml of saline) into the abdominal cavity before taste aversion training. The amounts injected were adjusted to be the same as those described for the injection of the insulin receptor antibody [2.56 μg/ml (17.5 nm)].
Sixty minutes after the injection of saline or IgG, we subjected the snails to the taste aversion training procedure (i.e., CS–US paired) and also prepared backward conditioning control (i.e., US–CS paired) and naive control snails (Fig. 7A,B). In all groups of snails, a pretest to the CS (i.e., sucrose) was given and the number of bites per minute were ascertained. After the CS was paired 10 times with the US in the taste aversion training session, the feeding response to the CS was significantly reduced (p < 0.01, one-factor ANOVA) at the 10 min posttest, compared with that observed when the backward conditioning control procedure was used or when only the distilled water was presented (naive control) (Fig. 7A,B). This aversive behavior underwent consolidation into CTA–LTM, because the feeding response was still suppressed at the posttests of 1 h, 1 d, and 1 week (Fig. 7A,B).
We next injected the insulin receptor antibody into the snails 60 min before taste aversion training. In addition to this group of snails, we again used both a backward conditioning group and a naive group. A pretest to the CS was given in all three groups, and the number of bites per minute were recorded. There were no differences in the number of bites elicited by the CS in all three groups in the pretest (p > 0.05). After the taste aversion training, we found that the feeding response elicited by the CS in the posttest session (10 min later) of snails injected with the insulin receptor antibody was significantly reduced (p < 0.01, one-way ANOVA followed by the post hoc Scheffé's test) compared with their pretest session and with the posttest sessions of the backward conditioning control or the naive control group (Fig. 7C). Thus, snails injected with the insulin receptor antibody were capable of learning and remembering for 10 min.
Most importantly, however, there were no significant differences in the feeding response elicited by the CS in the posttest session (1 h later) compared with the pretest session in the insulin receptor antibody group (p > 0.05; Fig. 7C). In fact, the response elicited by the CS 1 h after training in this group was indistinguishable from that observed in either the backward conditioning or the naive control groups (p > 0.05; Fig. 7C). Similarly, no differences were observed 1 d or 1 week later (Fig. 7C). That is, CTA–LTM was not observed in any of the three groups. Thus, the feeding response elicited by the CS 1 h after the taste aversion training returned to the initial level seen before the training.
We obtained similar data on the ability of the insulin receptor antibody to block the formation of LTM when we injected snails with the antibody 3 h before taste aversion training. We still had a significant suppression in the feeding response in the 10 min posttest session (p < 0.01), but 1 h later there was no difference in the feeding response compared with the pretest session (Fig. 7C).
Finally, we examined the possibility that the injection of the insulin receptor antibody evoked extinction of taste aversion in snails. Here we performed two experiments to test for extinction. First, both the injection period (60 min) and concentration of insulin receptor antibody and the training paradigm were the same as those described above. However, we skipped the 10 min posttest to avoid that this CS could be an extinction session. We did not observe CTA–LTM in this experimental design (Fig. 7D). Second, we injected the insulin receptor antibody into snails in which CTA–LTM had already been consolidated. That is, the insulin receptor antibody was injected for 1 h into snails at the timing of 1 h after the taste aversion training was finished. In this experiment, LTM was observed. These data showed that once CTA–LTM was consolidated, the insulin receptor antibody did not affect recall (Fig. 7E). Thus, we believe that the injection of insulin receptor does not evoke the extinction of CTA–LTM. We conclude that taste aversion is acquired (i.e., learning) in the snails that are injected with the insulin receptor antibody but that the learning is not consolidated into LTM due to the action of the insulin receptor antibody.
Discussion
We hypothesized that some MIPs play a key role in the LTM consolidation process after the acquisition of taste aversion in Lymnaea. We tested this hypothesis at both the electrophysiological and behavioral levels. All of these data, as well as our anatomical data, are consistent with this hypothesis and add to the growing appreciation of the role played by insulin or insulin-like molecules in cognitive functioning.
Blockade of LTM by insulin receptor antibody
Our behavioral experiments showed that an injection of the insulin receptor antibody into the abdominal cavity did not inhibit the learning of taste aversion in Lymnaea. However, the data show that the insulin receptor antibody blocks the memory consolidation process; thus, CTA–LTM was not seen. This result was consistent with the electrophysiological data showing that synaptic enhancement caused by insulin or the secreted CNS factor (i.e., MIPs) could be blocked by the insulin receptor antibody. Together, the results reported here make a strong case for an insulin-like molecule having a causal role in LTM formation after taste aversion training.
In our taste aversion training paradigm, we used a 10-trial training procedure. That is, when we tested the taste aversion trained snails at the 10 min posttest, 100 min had indeed passed after the onset of taste aversion training. Furthermore, when we tested the taste aversion trained snails 1 h later, 150 min had passed. These data suggest that the consolidation period for LTM occurs ∼2 or 3 h later after the onset of taste aversion training in Lymnaea. Recent experiments using a one-trial training procedure followed by a quick cooling treatment (4°C for 30 min) showed that cooling must be performed within 10 min after the training onset to block both taste aversion learning and its LTM formation (Sugai et al., 2007). However, after taste aversion learning is acquired, we do not know when LTM underwent memory consolidation. Based on the data presented here, it is reasonable to believe that the consolidation period for CTA–LTM in Lymnaea occurs 2 or 3 h later after the training onset. These data are similar to those reported previously (Matsumoto et al., 2009; Amano and Maruyama, 2011; Braun and Lukowiak, 2011; Michel et al., 2011; Trannoy et al., 2011).
Do good learners constitutionally contain more MIPs than poor learners?
Correlational studies using CTA in Lymnaea had previously identified a possible role of MIPs in LTM formation. Azami et al. (2006) found that in so-called “good” learners, but not “poor” learners, that some of the upregulated genes found in the CNS were the genes encoding MIPs. Although they referred to snails that showed LTM as good learners, it probably would have been more appropriate to call them snails with good memory, because that was what they were measuring. Semantics aside, what those data suggested was that in snails possessing LTM for CTA, there was a correlated increase with activity of the genes that encode MIPs.
It is a still unresolved question as to whether MIPs in Lymnaea also play an important and/or necessary role in the memory-forming processes after other training procedures. It is tempting to speculate that MIPs play a role in appetitive food conditioning (Straub et al., 2006), because many of the same neuronal pathways are involved as in CTA.
Also unresolved is whether MIPs only play a permissive role in the consolidation process. That is, secretion of MIPs is not tied to the actual training procedure but rather MIPs are continually being synthesized and released. If MIPs are not present, consolidation does not occur. However, whether there is an increase in MIP synthesis or release as a result of the training procedure is not known. We do know that the genes encoding MIPs are upregulated in good learners (i.e., snails that exhibit good memory; Azami et al., 2006). However, it is possible that in the poor learners that in the naive state MIP levels were down. In other words, there was a difference in MIP gene activity before any training occurred. We do know in Lymnaea that there are different innate abilities to form LTM after training. Some snails show enhanced memory formation while others do not, and that difference is heritable (Orr et al., 2009; Dalesman and Lukowiak, 2010). It could very well be that differences in ongoing MIP gene activity could be a possible explanation for this experimental finding. Studies now underway will test this possibility in the different strains of Lymnaea that exhibit differing cognitive abilities.
Insulin and memory in other animals
It is becoming clear that insulin plays an important role in cognitive functioning in animals, including humans. Insulin receptors are found in abundance on neurons in the hippocampus, the entorhinal cortex, and the frontal cortex all areas important for memory formation and retrieval (Chiu et al., 2008; McNay et al., 2010). Moreover, a recent clinical study shows promise of using intranasal insulin administration in the treatment of Alzheimer's disease (Craft et al., 2012). Previous reports have also indicated that insulin or insulin-like peptides play key roles in the process of memory formation, as, for example, in Caenorhabditis elegans, rodents, and humans (Wu et al., 1999; Zhao et al., 1999; Izquierdo et al., 2000; Dou et al., 2005; Murakami et al., 2005; Ramsey et al., 2005; Tomioka et al., 2006; Haj-ali et al., 2009; Oda et al., 2011; Smith et al., 2011).
Insulin binding to the insulin receptor leads to intracellular recruitment of insulin receptor substrates and activation of phosphoinositide 3-kinase signaling, which is implicated in both the induction and maintenance of long-term potentiation and long-term depression in hippocampal slices (Sanna et al., 2002; Kim et al., 2011). Furthermore, because the growth hormone/insulin-like growth factor-I axis is involved in the regulation of brain growth and development (Chiu et al., 2008; Aberg, 2010), these factors are well positioned to be used for the remodeling of synapses or other morphological changes that are part of the memory-forming process.
Possible function of MIPs in a long-term synaptic enhancement
We previously demonstrated that the injection of cAMP into the CGC brought about a long-lasting change in synaptic plasticity at the CGC-B1 synapses (Nakamura et al., 1999b). The existence of activator and repressor types of CREB, CREB1 and CREB2, has also been demonstrated in the CGCs and other neurons (Ribeiro et al., 2003; Sadamoto et al., 2004, 2010, 2011; Wagatsuma et al., 2005, 2006). Moreover, prior injection of the CRE oligonucleotide or a PKA inhibitor into the CGC blocked the expression of this long-lasting change in synaptic plasticity at the CGC-B1 synapses (Nakamura et al., 1999b; Sadamoto et al., 2004). CREBs have been thought to play an important role in this long-lasting change in synaptic plasticity at the “presynaptic” sites. Whether MIPs also act at the presynaptic or postsynaptic site is still to be determined.
In culture neurons of GABA-mediated networks, application of insulin increased the expression of the GABAA receptor on the postsynaptic membranes of these neurons (Wan et al., 1997). Insulin may evoke a rapid recruitment of functional GABAA receptors to the postsynaptic plasma membrane, suggesting a fundamental mechanism for the generation of synaptic plasticity. At the CGC-B1 synapses in Lymnaea, serotonin appears to be the likely transmitter substance (Nakamura et al., 1999b), so it is possible that MIPs could result in an increase in serotonin receptors on the B1 motor neurons. This possibility is currently being examined.
Other functions of MIPs
The MIP genes are also expressed in the canopy cells in the lateral lobe near the cerebral ganglia (Smit et al., 1988). The canopy cells, as well as the LGCs, control body growth (Geraerts, 1976a, b). In both the canopy cells and the LGCs, complement receptor 3-like immunoreactivity is also detected (Hatakeyama et al., 2000). However, the exact function of MIPs and complement receptor 3 in the canopy cells is still unknown. Furthermore, MIP VII gene is known to be expressed in the buccal ganglia (Smit et al., 1996). MIP VII might be involved in feeding behavior. Our present findings do not exclude the possibility that MIPs other than MIP I, II, and III from the LGCs and even from the canopy cells play an important role in the establishment of the neural correlates of behavioral LTM. MIP II is known to stimulate neurite formation (Smit et al., 1988; Kits et al., 1990).
In conclusion, the present data are consistent with the hypothesis that MIPs secreted from the LGCs play an important and necessary role in the formation of LTM after the acquisition of associative learning (i.e., CTA). This conclusion is consistent with the hypothesis that insulin or insulin-like molecules play an important role in cognitive functions in animals, including humans. The Lymnaea model system then can serve as a test bed to determine the causal mechanisms of how insulin or insulin-like molecules exert their effects on cognition.
Footnotes
This work was supported by a KAKENHI grant from Japan Society for the Promotion of Science [Grants 21770081 (H.Sa.) and 21657022 (E.I.)] and a grant from Canadian Institutes of Health Research (No. MOP 64339 to K.L.).
- Correspondence should be addressed to Etsuro Ito, Laboratory of Functional Biology, Kagawa School of Pharmaceutical Sciences, Tokushima Bunri University, 1314-1 Shido, Sanuki 769-2193, Japan. eito{at}kph.bunri-u.ac.jp