Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Strength of Coupling within a Mnemonic Control Network Differentiates Those Who Can and Cannot Suppress Memory Retrieval

Pedro M. Paz-Alonso, Silvia A. Bunge, Michael C. Anderson and Simona Ghetti
Journal of Neuroscience 13 March 2013, 33 (11) 5017-5026; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3459-12.2013
Pedro M. Paz-Alonso
1Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, California 94720,
2Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis, California 95616,
3Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language, Donostia-San Sebastián, Gipuzkoa 20009, Spain,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Silvia A. Bunge
1Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, California 94720,
4Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, California 94720,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Michael C. Anderson
5MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge CB2 7EF, United Kingdom, and
6Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Simona Ghetti
2Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis, California 95616,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Figures

  • Figure 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 1.

    Behavioral evidence of mnemonic control during the TNT phase as measured by subsequent memory recall (i.e., percentage Recalled) on the Same-Probe task. A, Behavioral data from our previous behavioral study (N = 70; Paz-Alonso et al., 2009). The bar graph shows the percentage of word–word associations recalled as a function of age group and study condition (Think, Baseline, No-Think). B, Behavioral data for the present fMRI study sample (N = 43) as a function of age group and study condition. C, Behavioral data for the present study as a function of individual differences in mnemonic control (nonsuppressors, suppressors) and study condition. B, C, Dotted outlines indicate the condition (Baseline) for which fMRI data were not collected.*p < 0.05. **p < 0.01. ***p < 0.001.

  • Figure 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 2.

    Whole-brain contrasts comparing attempts with retrieve versus suppress retrieval, across all participants. A, Task effects: No-Think > Think (red) and the opposite Think > No-Think (blue) contrasts in slice sections along the y-axis. B, Regions associated with successful suppression. Brain renderings and slice sections showing activation for successful retrieval suppression relative to successful retrieval (i.e., No-Think-forgotten > Think-remembered trials).

  • Figure 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 3.

    Modulation of the MTL as a function of task instructions and subsequent memory, across all participants. Yellow represents voxels showing the standard subsequent memory effect: remembered Think trials > forgotten Think trials; light blue, voxels modulated by task instructions: Think > No-Think; dark blue, voxels modulated by task instructions, limited to successful trials: Think-remembered > No-Think-forgotten; medium blue, overlap between the last two contrasts. All these contrasts are featured at a q < 0.05 FDR-corrected threshold.

  • Figure 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 4.

    Functional connectivity analyses for all trials relative to Baseline. A, Brain renderings showing clusters identified from the conjunction analysis of the clusters tightly correlated, across the entire brain and participants, with both left and right hippocampus seeds for All > Null. B, Average coupling strength between all the clusters tightly correlated with hippocampus as a function of Suppression group, Task, and Memory. Average coupling strength was higher for No-Think-forgotten than for Think-forgotten conditions, only for those participants who did exhibit behavioral memory suppression effects. C, ROIs tightly correlated with hippocampus used in subsequent functional connectivity pairwise correlations, including left and right hippocampus (Hipp), middle cingulate (mCing), posterior cingulate (pCing), right lateral posterior parietal cortex (lPPC), and right dlPFC seeds. D, Correlation matrices showing differences in pairwise correlation strength between pairs of ROIs described in C for No-Think-forgotten–Think-forgotten for suppressors versus nonsuppressors.

Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 33 (11)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 33, Issue 11
13 Mar 2013
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Strength of Coupling within a Mnemonic Control Network Differentiates Those Who Can and Cannot Suppress Memory Retrieval
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Strength of Coupling within a Mnemonic Control Network Differentiates Those Who Can and Cannot Suppress Memory Retrieval
Pedro M. Paz-Alonso, Silvia A. Bunge, Michael C. Anderson, Simona Ghetti
Journal of Neuroscience 13 March 2013, 33 (11) 5017-5026; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3459-12.2013

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Strength of Coupling within a Mnemonic Control Network Differentiates Those Who Can and Cannot Suppress Memory Retrieval
Pedro M. Paz-Alonso, Silvia A. Bunge, Michael C. Anderson, Simona Ghetti
Journal of Neuroscience 13 March 2013, 33 (11) 5017-5026; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3459-12.2013
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Articles

  • Choice Behavior Guided by Learned, But Not Innate, Taste Aversion Recruits the Orbitofrontal Cortex
  • Maturation of Spontaneous Firing Properties after Hearing Onset in Rat Auditory Nerve Fibers: Spontaneous Rates, Refractoriness, and Interfiber Correlations
  • Insulin Treatment Prevents Neuroinflammation and Neuronal Injury with Restored Neurobehavioral Function in Models of HIV/AIDS Neurodegeneration
Show more Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Cortico-striatal activity characterizes human safety learning via Pavlovian conditioned inhibition
  • Brain functional connectivity mapping of behavioral flexibility in rhesus monkeys
  • Accumulation System: Distributed Neural Substrates of Perceptual Decision Making Revealed by fMRI Deconvolution
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2022 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.