Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Articles, Systems/Circuits

Cancelling Prism Adaptation by a Shift of Background: A Novel Utility of Allocentric Coordinates for Extracting Motor Errors

Motoaki Uchimura and Shigeru Kitazawa
Journal of Neuroscience 24 April 2013, 33 (17) 7595-7602; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5702-12.2013
Motoaki Uchimura
1Dynamic Brain Network Laboratory, Graduate School of Frontier Biosciences, and
2Department of Brain Physiology, Graduate School of Medicine, Osaka University, Suita, Osaka 565–0871, Japan,
4Department of Neurophysiology, Graduate School of Medicine, Juntendo University, Bunkyo, Tokyo 113-8421, Japan
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Shigeru Kitazawa
1Dynamic Brain Network Laboratory, Graduate School of Frontier Biosciences, and
2Department of Brain Physiology, Graduate School of Medicine, Osaka University, Suita, Osaka 565–0871, Japan,
3Center for Information and Neural Networks, National Institute of Information and Communications Technology and Osaka University, Suita, Osaka 565-0871, Japan, and
4Department of Neurophysiology, Graduate School of Medicine, Juntendo University, Bunkyo, Tokyo 113-8421, Japan
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Figures

  • Figure 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 1.

    A novel utility of the allocentric memory of a target position. a, Allocentric memory in terms of the background frame has been believed to be useful in memorizing “where to go” in delayed reaching when a blank (delay) is as long as 2 s (left). We propose a novel use of the allocentric memory in an immediate reach (right). We hypothesize that the allocentric memory is essential for dissociating error attributable to the motor system (red arrow) from error attributable to target motion by encoding “where I intended to go” (dotted circle). b, Basic design of the experiments. A large apparent error (black arrow) was introduced using a lateral displacing prism. The frame remained still (left) or was displaced in the direction of prism displacement (ipsilateral shift, middle) or in the opposite direction (contralateral shift, right) during a reaching movement. Assuming that our hypothesis is correct, the original target position decoded in terms of the frame would shift with the frame (dotted circle), and the motor error (red arrow) would be underestimated in the ipsilateral condition and overestimated in the contralateral condition.

  • Figure 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 2.

    Task procedures. Each column schematically shows the status of the visual field (oval shape) and the position of the hand relative to the button and the screen. The shutters opened at 0 (Start) when the participant pressed the button. A target and background figures appeared after a random delay (900–1200 ms) at 1. Vision was blocked from 2 (Release) to 3 (Touch) and allowed again for 300 ms. The background figures were presented both before (encoding) and after (decoding) the reach in experiments 1–3 (a); they were presented only after the reach in experiment 4 (b).

  • Figure 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 3.

    Effects of ipsilateral and contralateral shifts of the background (experiment 1). The data from an individual participant (a–c) and from all participants (d–f) are shown. Each circle represents the median of two (a–c) and 12 (d–f, 2 directions × 6 participants) responses plotted against trial sequence (abscissa). Errors in the direction of prism displacement (right or left) are indicated as positive. The thick black lines indicate the model predictions using model equation in Materials and Methods. Note the difference in the asymptotes during the exposure period (red dotted lines) and in the aftereffects (black arrows) across three conditions: ipsilateral shift (a, d), no-shift (b, e), and contralateral shift (c, f). The gray shaded areas in d–f show the 25th and 75th percentiles. g, h, Distributions of the asymptotes during the exposure period (g) and aftereffects during the posttest period (h) estimated for each session (n = 12). Each box plot shows the 10th, 25th, 50th, 75th, and 90th percentiles. Brackets with asterisks indicate significant differences (*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001) after corrections for multiple comparisons (Ryan's method after one-way repeated-measures ANOVA).

  • Figure 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 4.

    Effects of the size of the background (experiment 2) on cancelling. a, Illustrations of background figures of different sizes. The size of the center square was 0 (no background), 8, 40, 80, or 120 mm. The top and bottom panels show the rightward shift of the background between the encoding (top) and decoding (bottom) phases. The vertical lines show the size of the shift (20 mm). b–f, Prism adaptation with five different sizes of background. Note the maximal cancelling effects at 40 mm. Each circle represents the median of 12 responses (6 participants × 2 directions). g, h, Distributions of the asymptotes during the exposure period (g) and aftereffects during the posttest period (h). The daggers in h show that the mean aftereffect was significantly different from zero after correction for multiple comparisons (†p < 0.05; ‡p < 0.01; t test, Bonferroni's correction). Other conventions are the same as in Figure 3.

  • Figure 5.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 5.

    Effects of the complexity of the background on cancelling (experiment 3). a, Illustrations of background figures with different complexities: no background, a single line, parallel lines, a single square, triple squares, and multiple squares. The size of the center figure was fixed at 80 mm. b–g, Prism adaptation with background figures of different complexities. Note the maximal cancelling effects with multiple squares. Each circle represents the median of 12 responses (12 participants × 1 direction). h, i, Distributions of the asymptotes during the exposure period (h) and aftereffects during the posttest period (i). Other conventions are the same as in Figure 3.

  • Figure 6.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 6.

    Necessity of the background during the encoding phase (experiment 4) for cancelling. a–c, Prism adaptation in the ipsilateral shift (a), post-presentation (b), and no background conditions. Note the contrast between the ipsilateral shift condition and the post-presentation condition. Each circle represents the median of 12 responses (6 participants × 2 directions). d, e, Distributions of the asymptotes during the exposure period (d) and aftereffects during the post-test period (e). Other conventions are the same as in Figure 3.

Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 33 (17)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 33, Issue 17
24 Apr 2013
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Cancelling Prism Adaptation by a Shift of Background: A Novel Utility of Allocentric Coordinates for Extracting Motor Errors
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Cancelling Prism Adaptation by a Shift of Background: A Novel Utility of Allocentric Coordinates for Extracting Motor Errors
Motoaki Uchimura, Shigeru Kitazawa
Journal of Neuroscience 24 April 2013, 33 (17) 7595-7602; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5702-12.2013

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Cancelling Prism Adaptation by a Shift of Background: A Novel Utility of Allocentric Coordinates for Extracting Motor Errors
Motoaki Uchimura, Shigeru Kitazawa
Journal of Neuroscience 24 April 2013, 33 (17) 7595-7602; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5702-12.2013
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Articles

  • Memory Retrieval Has a Dynamic Influence on the Maintenance Mechanisms That Are Sensitive to ζ-Inhibitory Peptide (ZIP)
  • Neurophysiological Evidence for a Cortical Contribution to the Wakefulness-Related Drive to Breathe Explaining Hypocapnia-Resistant Ventilation in Humans
  • Monomeric Alpha-Synuclein Exerts a Physiological Role on Brain ATP Synthase
Show more Articles

Systems/Circuits

  • Macroscale Traveling Waves Evoked by Single-Pulse Stimulation of the Human Brain
  • Multimodal Correspondence between Optogenetic fMRI, Electrophysiology, and Anatomical Maps of the Secondary Somatosensory Cortex in Nonhuman Primates
  • Impact of Rod-Dominant Mesopic Conditions on Spatial Summation and Surround Suppression in Early Visual Cortex
Show more Systems/Circuits
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.