Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Deficits in Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Group 1 Metabotropic Glutamate Receptor Function Mediate Resistance to Extinction during Protracted Withdrawal from an Extensive History of Cocaine Self-Administration

Osnat Ben-Shahar, Arianne D. Sacramento, Bailey W. Miller, Sierra M. Webb, Melissa G. Wroten, Hannah E. Silva, Amanda L. Caruana, Evan J. Gordon, Kyle L. Ploense, Jennifer Ditzhazy, Tod E. Kippin and Karen K. Szumlinski
Journal of Neuroscience 9 January 2013, 33 (2) 495-506a; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3710-12.2013
Osnat Ben-Shahar
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences and the Neuroscience Research Institute, University of California at Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, California 93106-9660
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Arianne D. Sacramento
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences and the Neuroscience Research Institute, University of California at Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, California 93106-9660
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Bailey W. Miller
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences and the Neuroscience Research Institute, University of California at Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, California 93106-9660
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Sierra M. Webb
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences and the Neuroscience Research Institute, University of California at Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, California 93106-9660
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Melissa G. Wroten
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences and the Neuroscience Research Institute, University of California at Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, California 93106-9660
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Hannah E. Silva
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences and the Neuroscience Research Institute, University of California at Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, California 93106-9660
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Amanda L. Caruana
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences and the Neuroscience Research Institute, University of California at Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, California 93106-9660
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Evan J. Gordon
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences and the Neuroscience Research Institute, University of California at Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, California 93106-9660
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Kyle L. Ploense
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences and the Neuroscience Research Institute, University of California at Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, California 93106-9660
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jennifer Ditzhazy
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences and the Neuroscience Research Institute, University of California at Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, California 93106-9660
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Tod E. Kippin
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences and the Neuroscience Research Institute, University of California at Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, California 93106-9660
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Karen K. Szumlinski
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences and the Neuroscience Research Institute, University of California at Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, California 93106-9660
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Anomalies in prefrontal cortex (PFC) function are posited to underpin difficulties in learning to suppress drug-seeking behavior during abstinence. Because group 1 metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGluRs) regulate drug-related learning, we assayed the consequences of extended access to intravenous cocaine (6 h/d; 0.25 mg/infusion for 10 d) on the PFC expression of group 1 mGluRs and the relevance of observed changes for cocaine seeking. After protracted withdrawal, cocaine-experienced animals exhibited a time-dependent intensification of cue-induced cocaine-seeking behavior and an impaired extinction of this behavior. These behavioral phenomena were associated with a time-dependent reduction in mGluR1/5 expression within ventromedial PFC (vmPFC) of cocaine-experienced animals exposed to extinction testing but not in untested ones. Interestingly, pharmacological manipulations of vmPFC mGluR1/5 produced no immediate effects on cue-induced cocaine-seeking behavior but produced residual effects on a subsequent test for cocaine seeking. At 3 d withdrawal, cocaine-experienced rats infused intra-vmPFC with mGluR1/5 antagonists, either before or after an initial test for cocaine seeking, persisted in their cocaine seeking akin to cocaine-experienced rats in protracted withdrawal. Conversely, cocaine-experienced rats infused with an mGluR1/5 agonist before the initial test for cocaine-seeking at 30 d withdrawal exhibited a facilitation of extinction learning. These data indicate that cue-elicited deficits in vmPFC group 1 mGluR function mediate resistance to extinction during protracted withdrawal from a history of extensive cocaine self-administration and pose pharmacological stimulation of these receptors as a potential approach to facilitate learned suppression of drug-seeking behavior that may aid drug abstinence.

Introduction

In cocaine addicts, the presentation of cues that have been associated with cocaine consumption induces drug craving and may cause relapse to drug use (Childress et al., 1999, 2008; Volkow et al., 1999; Garavan et al., 2000; Goldstein et al., 2009b; O'Brien, 2009). The presentation of such cues to cocaine addicts also results in heightened activation of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) (Grant et al., 1996; Childress et al., 1999, 2008; Volkow et al., 1999; Garavan et al., 2000; Goldstein and Volkow, 2002; but see Bonson et al., 2002), an area important for the inhibition of inappropriate behaviors and for behavioral flexibility as it pertains to the adjustment of a behavior according to its consequences (Rolls et al., 1994; Damasio, 1996; Konishi et al., 1998; Clark et al., 2004). Finally, the mPFC exhibits hypofunctionality in cocaine addicts (Volkow et al., 1992, 1993; London et al., 2000; Franklin et al., 2002; Matochik et al., 2003; Goldstein et al., 2009a). These observations are consistent with the hypothesis that excessive cocaine consumption results in neural adaptations within the mPFC that render the addict especially vulnerable to cue-induced relapse.

Preclinical studies provide additional evidence for the importance of the mPFC for cue-induced cocaine seeking. Cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior, as well as incubation of craving, are paralleled by mPFC activation (Neisewander et al., 2000; Ciccocioppo et al., 2001; Hamlin et al., 2008; Hearing et al., 2008; Zavala et al., 2008; Koya et al., 2009), and inactivation of this brain area attenuates cue-induced reinstatement (McLaughlin and See, 2003; Fuchs et al., 2005), as well as the incubation of cocaine craving (Koya et al., 2009). More specifically, glutamatergic projections from the mPFC to the nucleus accumbens are hypothesized to be important for cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior (Kalivas and O'Brien, 2008; Uys and LaLumiere, 2008; Van den Oever et al., 2010), and chronic cocaine self-administration produces alterations in indices of mPFC glutamate function (Robinson et al., 2001; Tang et al., 2004; Ben-Shahar et al., 2009, 2012). One component of the glutamatergic system that was shown to play a critical role in cocaine reinforcement and addiction are the group 1 metabotropic glutamate receptors (subtypes mGluR1 and mGluR5) (Chiamulera et al., 2001; Kenny and Markou, 2004; Olive, 2010). Especially relevant to the current project are demonstrations that systemic administration of the mGluR5 antagonists 2-methyl-6-(phenylethynyl)pyridine hydrochloride (MPEP) or 3-((2-methyl-1,3-thiazol-4-yl)ethynyl)pyridine hydrochloride (MTEP) significantly attenuate cue-induced reinstatement (Bäckström and Hyytiä, 2006; Iso et al., 2006; Kumaresan et al., 2009) and that systemic administration of mGluR5 agonist CDPPB [3-cyano-N-(1,3-diphenyl-1H-pyrazol-5-yl)benzamide] facilitated the extinction of cocaine seeking (Cleva et al., 2011). Together, these data suggest that one type of neural adaptation within the mPFC that might develop after excessive cocaine use and be important for cue-induced relapse involves changes in group 1 mGluR function. The present study sought to test this hypothesis by first monitoring changes in the expression group 1 mGluRs within the mPFC after various withdrawal periods and then manipulating such changes to test their functional significance for the incubation of cue-mediated cocaine-seeking behavior (Tran-Nguyen et al., 1998; Grimm et al., 2001, 2002).

Materials and Methods

Procedure

The general procedure for all experiments described here was identical and consisted of the following steps in chronological order: (1) lever responding (food) training; (2) surgery; (3) self-administration of saline [1 h (Sal1h) or 6 h (Sal6h) daily session, depending on the specific experiment] or cocaine [6 h (Coc6h) daily sessions] across 10 daily sessions; and then (4) 3 or 30 d of withdrawal (rats kept in their home cages). After the completion of saline/cocaine self-administration, the specific procedures varied depending on the particular experiment. For immunoblotting studies, brain tissue was harvested from withdrawn rats at the two different withdrawal time points or animals were subjected to a 2 h cue test, followed by harvesting of brain tissue. For some neuropharmacological studies, animals received intra-ventromedial PFC (vmPFC) microinjections, followed by a 30 min cue test, and then brains were harvested for histological verification of microinjector placements. For other neuropharmacological studies addressing extinction learning, animals received intra-vmPFC or dorsomedial PFC (dmPFC) microinjections, followed by a 30 min cue test, and then were tested, 24 h later, in a second 30 min cue test in the absence of any additional intracranial manipulation, followed by histological verification of microinjector placement. In the following subsections, these various steps are described in detail. For clarity, this general procedure is outlined in Figure 1.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Schema of general procedures and experimental design. A, Immunoblotting studies. After self-administration (SA) (10 daily sessions of 1 or 6 h access to saline or 6 h access to cocaine), animals were divided to two conditions: 3 or 30 d of withdrawal. After withdrawal, animals were either subjected to a 2 h cue test, immediately killed, and the brains harvested for immunoblotting analysis, or were simply killed (with no other experimental manipulation) with brains harvested for immunoblotting. B, Neuropharmacological studies. The procedure was the same as that illustrated in A with one exception: treatment after withdrawal. Here, cohorts of subjects received one or two 30 min cue tests, and brains were harvested for histological verification of microinjector placements.

Subjects

The subjects were male Sprague Dawley rats weighing 275–325 g at the beginning of each experiment and obtained from Charles River Laboratories. Rats were housed in a colony room controlled for temperature (25°C) and humidity (71%), under a 12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 8:00 P.M.). Animals were given ad libitum access to food and water, except during operant training for food reinforcement (see below, Lever response training). The animals were allowed to acclimate to the colony room for at least 3 d after arrival. All experimental protocols were consistent with the National Institutes of Health Guide for Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (Publication 80-23, revised 1996) and were reviewed and approved by the University of California, Santa Barbara, Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.

Lever response training

To promote lever-pressing behavior for subsequent saline and cocaine reinforcement, rats were first trained to lever press on a fixed-ration 1 (FR1) schedule of food reinforcement (45 mg pellets; Noyes) in sound-attenuated operant conditioning chambers (30 × 20 × 24 cm high; Med Associates) during either a 16 h overnight training session or daily 1 h training sessions (depending on availability of our chambers) as described previously by our group (Kerstetter et al., 2008; Ben-Shahar et al., 2009). For this, rats were food deprived for 24 h before the initiation of training and maintained on a restricted diet for the duration of food training. The operant chambers were equipped with two retractable levers, a stimulus light above each lever, a food pellet dispenser outside the operant box and a food trough between the levers, a house light on the wall opposite to the levers, and a speaker connected to a tone generator (ANL-926; Med Associates). During the session, each lever press on the active lever resulted in delivery of one food pellet. Lever presses on the inactive lever had no programmed consequences. Rats that failed to exhibit the criterion of a minimum of 200 responses on the active lever during the overnight session or a minimum of 100 responses on the active lever during two consecutive 1 h sessions received additional lever response training sessions until criterion was met. After successful acquisition of lever-pressing behavior (one or two overnight sessions; 5–7 d of 1 h daily sessions), food was available ad libitum for the remainder of the study.

Surgery

After lever response training, animals underwent surgical procedures to implant chronic intravenous catheters and bilateral guide cannulae into the PFC as described previously by our group (Ben-Shahar et al., 2008, 2009). Under ketamine/xylazine anesthesia (56.25 and 7.5 mg/kg, respectively; Abbott Laboratories), rats were implanted with a chronic Silastic catheter (13 cm long; 0.3 mm inner diameter, 0.64 mm outer diameter; Dow Corning) into the right jugular vein. Atropine (0.04 mg/kg) was administered intramuscularly to minimize respiratory congestion during anesthesia and banamine (2 mg/kg; a non-opiate analgesic) was injected subcutaneously to treat postsurgical pain. Each catheter ran subcutaneously around the shoulder to back where it was secured to a threaded 22 gauge metal guide cannula (Plastics One), which emerged from the midline of the animal's back perpendicular to the dorsal surface. An obturator covered the open end of the cannula to protect from contamination and the cannula was held in place via a small swatch of Bard Mesh (C. R. Bard) to which it was cemented. The mesh was, in turn, laid flat subcutaneously on the animal's back.

Immediately after the intravenous catheterization and while still under anesthesia, rats in the behavioral studies were transferred to a stereotaxic apparatus and implanted with 30 gauge guide cannulae (Plastics One) above the vmPFC [anteroposterior (AP), +2.5 mm; mediolateral (ML), ±1.0 mm; dorsoventral (DV), −2.0 mm from bregma] or above the dmPFC (AP, +2.5 mm; ML, ±1.0 mm; DV, −1.0 mm from bregma), according to the atlas of Paxinos and Watson (2007), using procedures similar to those described previously by our group (Ben-Shahar and Ettenberg, 1998; Zayara et al., 2011). Four small screws and cranioplastic cement secured the guide cannulae to the skull. Stylets (Plastics One) were placed into each cannula to prevent occlusion. All animals were allowed a minimum of 5 d for recovery, and intravenous catheter patency was maintained by flushing with 0.1 ml of sterile heparin plus timentin/saline (60 IU/ml and 100 mg/ml, respectively) solution each day.

Self-administration training

After surgery, rats (n = 10–15 per group per cohort at the start of each experiment) were trained to intravenously self-administer cocaine (0.25 mg/0.1 ml/infusion; a generous gift from the National Institute on Drug Abuse) during daily 6 h sessions on an FR1 schedule of reinforcement. At the start of each session, the rat's catheter was connected to a motorized pump (located outside of the sound-attenuated chamber) via a liquid swivel as described previously (Ben-Shahar et al., 2008; Kerstetter et al., 2008). Active lever presses resulted in a 5 s activation of the infusion pump and a 20 s presentation of a stimulus complex, consisting of activation of the white stimulus light above the active lever and the tone generator (78 dB, 2 kHz), during which responses on the active lever had no consequences. Responses on the inactive lever were recorded but had no programmed consequences. Rats were trained to self-administer cocaine for 10 sessions. To prevent overdose, the number of cocaine infusions permitted during the first 2 d of training was capped at 100 (day1) and 120 (day2), and rats failing to meet self-administration criterion (minimum of 50 infusions per 6 h session for the last 3 d of training) were excluded from the study.

For immunoblotting studies, additional groups of rats were trained to intravenously self-administer saline (0.1 ml/infusion) during daily 1 or 6 h sessions (n = 12 per control group per cohort at the start of the experiment). Two saline controls groups were included in the immunoblotting study because, for practical reasons, we wanted to replicate the results of previous studies indicating that extended (6 h) access to intravenous saline does not produce behavioral or protein changes distinct from those observed in animals with short (1 h) access to intravenous saline (Ben-Shahar et al., 2004). Our behavioral and immunoblotting results confirmed that there were no differences between the two saline controls (see Results), and thus, only 1 h saline controls were included in the behavioral neuropharmacological studies. All self-administration training and testing occurred during the dark phase of the circadian cycle. After completion of the 10 d of self-administration training, animals remained in their home cages in the colony room for either 3 or 30 d withdrawal. At these time points, PFC subregions were harvested from a subset of animals, whereas another subset of animals were tested for cue-elicited cocaine seeking over a 2 h period, followed by harvesting of PFC subregions.

Tests for cue-elicited cocaine seeking

With the exception of the one subset of withdrawn rats used for immunoblotting, saline and cocaine self-administering rats were subjected to a test for cue-elicited cocaine seeking under extinction conditions. For the immunoblotting experiments, the cue tests were 2 h in duration to capture potentially relevant changes in receptor protein expression. To statistically evaluate the presence/absence of response incubation during withdrawal and provide a behavioral correlate for our immunoblotting data, the number of lever responses on the 2 h cue tests were analyzed using an intravenous treatment (Sal1h, Sal6h, Coc6h) × withdrawal (3 vs 30 d) × lever (active vs inactive) ANOVA. For the behavioral neuropharmacological experiments, the cue tests were 30 min in duration, as conducted by others (Koya et al., 2009). The cue test conditions were the same regardless of the duration of testing; testing involved tethering the animals and the lever-press response-contingent presentation of the tone–light cue previously paired with saline/cocaine infusions but no saline/cocaine infusions were delivered.

Immunoblotting studies

Our first immunoblotting study was aimed to investigate the influence of a history of cocaine self-administration experience on the protein expression of mGluR1/5 within PFC subregions at early versus protracted withdrawal. For this study, within each cohort of rats, separate groups of Sal1h, Sal6h, and Coc6h rats underwent a 2 h cue test session under extinction conditions at either 3 or 30 d withdrawal (n = 12 per group at the start of the experiment) to assay for cue-reinforced lever-pressing behavior. Immediately after completion of this cue test, animals were killed by rapid decapitation. The dmPFC (anterior cingulate and dorsal prelimbic cortices) and vmPFC (ventral prelimbic and infralimbic cortices) were dissected out over ice in a manner identical to that described by others (Ghasemzadeh et al., 2009). The tissue derived from animals in this study was immunoblotted in two separate ways. First, comparisons were made across the tissue from the three different self-administration groups separately for each time point (i.e., Sal1h, Sal6h, and Coc6h rats killed at 3 d withdrawal were compared in one assay, and a parallel assay compared Sal1h, Sal6h, and Coc6h rats killed at 30 d withdrawal in a separate assay). This “single time point” analysis was done to enable a comparison of protein expression between the Sal1h and Sal6h rats at each time point, as well as to determine whether or not a history of cocaine self-administration influenced protein expression relative to a history of saline self-administration at that particular withdrawal time point. For these assays, the immunoreactivity of the mGluR1 and mGluR5 bands from the Sal6h and Coc6h animals was normalized to the average of that for the Sal1h animals run on the corresponding gel (n = 4–5 per gel), and the data were expressed as a percentage of that averaged Sal1h signal for each gel. The data for the 3 and 30 d assays were analyzed separately using an univariate ANOVA across the three different self-administration groups to determine the presence/absence of a cocaine effect.

The next analysis examined for proteomic correlates of the increased lever pressing exhibited by both saline and cocaine self-administering animals at 30 d of withdrawal, by comparing tissue from the Sal6h and Coc6h animals killed at both withdrawal time points on the same gel. For this “time course” assay, the immunoreactivity of the mGluR1 and mGluR5 bands were normalized to the average of the Sal6h-3d withdrawal group run on the corresponding gel (n = 3–4 per gel), and the data were expressed as a percentage of that averaged signal for each gel. The data were analyzed using orthogonal comparisons, within each self-administration group, to determine the presence/absence of time-dependent changes in protein expression.

A second, follow-up, study assayed vmPFC and dmPFC tissue from Sal1h and Coc6h rats, killed at 3 versus 30 d withdrawal, in the absence of any cue testing. This study was conducted to determine the extent to which our cocaine-induced changes in mGluR1/5 protein expression reflected alterations in the basal expression pattern of these receptors or some interaction between cocaine experience and the cue-reinforced testing conditions of our animals. The data analysis for this study was identical to that used for the time course study above.

The immunoblotting procedures for detection of total mGluR1 and mGluR5 levels in brain tissue homogenate were identical to those described recently by our group (Cozzoli et al., 2009, 2012; Goulding et al., 2011). In brief, tissue was homogenized in a solution consisting of 0.32 m sucrose, 2 mm EDTA, 1% w/v sodium dodecyl sulfate, 50 μm phenylmethylsulfonyl fluoride, and 1 μg/ml leupeptin, pH 7.2, and 1 mm sodium fluoride, 50 mm sodium pyrophosphate, 20 mm 2-glycerol phosphate, 1 mm p-nitrophenyl phosphate, 1 mm orthovanadate, and 2 μm microcystin LR were included to inhibit phosphatases. After centrifugation at 10,000 × g for 20 min, the supernatant of the homogenates were quantified for protein content using the Bio-Rad DC protein assay and stored at −80°C. Protein samples (20 μg/lane) were subjected to SDS-PAGE on Tris-acetate gradient gels (3–8%) (Invitrogen). Wet polyvinylidene difluoride (Bio-Rad) membrane transfer was used, and membranes were preblocked with PBS containing 0.1% (v/v) Tween 20 and 5% (w/v) nonfat dried milk powder for a minimum of 2 h before overnight incubation with primary antibody. An anti-mGluR5 rabbit polyclonal antibody (1:1000 dilution; Millipore) and an anti-mGluR1a mouse polyclonal antibody (1:500 dilution; Millipore) were used for receptor detection. A rabbit primary anti-calnexin antibody (1:1000 dilution; Stressgen) was used as a control to ensure even protein loading and transfer. Membranes were washed, incubated with horseradish peroxidase-conjugated goat anti-rabbit secondary antibody (1:5000–1:10,000 dilution; Millipore) or anti-mouse secondary antibody (1:10,000; Jackson ImmunoResearch) for 90 min, and washed again, and immunoreactive bands were detected by enhanced chemiluminescence using either ECL Plus (GE Healthcare) or Pierce SuperSignal West Femto (Thermo Fisher Scientific). NIH Image J was used to quantify immunoreactivity levels.

Behavioral neuropharmacological studies

Intra-vmPFC delivery of group 1 mGluR compounds: acute effects on drug seeking.

A series of behavioral neuropharmacological experiments were conducted to determine the functional relevance of reduced vmPFC group 1 mGluR expression (see Figs. 2, 3) for the time-dependent increase in cue-seeking behavior during withdrawal (see Fig. 2A). For these experiments, Sal1h and Coc6h animals were trained to self-administer for 10 d and were then bilaterally infused intra-vmPFC with compounds targeting group 1 mGluRs immediately before cue testing using microinjection procedures similar to those described previously (Ben-Shahar and Ettenberg, 1998; Zayara et al., 2011; Ben-Shahar et al., 2012). It was hypothesized that, if the reduction in vmPFC group 1 mGluR expression observed at 30 d withdrawal mediated the time-dependent increase in lever pressing, then mimicking this reduction earlier in withdrawal should potentiate cocaine-seeking behavior (i.e., render a 3-d-withdrawn animal more similar to a 30-d-withdrawn animal). Conversely, hyperstimulating these receptors later in withdrawal should attenuate the heightened cocaine-seeking behavior (i.e., render a 30-d-withdrawn animals more similar to a 3-d-withdrawn animal). To test this hypothesis with respect to mGluR5, groups of 3-d-withdrawn saline and cocaine self-administering rats (n = 5–8 for saline animals and n > 10 for cocaine animals at the outset of experiment/replicate) were infused at 0.5 μl/min with the selective mGluR5 antagonists MPEP (3 μg/side; Sigma-Aldrich) or MTEP (3 μg/side; Tocris Bioscience). A parallel study assayed the effects of the selective mGluR1 antagonist JNJ 16259685 [(3,4-dihydro-2H-pyrano[2,3-b]quinolin-7-yl)-(cis-4-methoxycyclohexyl)-methanone] (30 ng/side; Tocris Bioscience). To test the converse hypothesis, groups of 30-d-withdrawn saline or cocaine self-administering rats (n > 8 per group per replicate) were infused with the mGluR1/5 agonist (S)-3,5-dihydroxyphenylglycine (DHPG) (27.5 ng/side; Tocris Bioscience). The antagonist doses were selected based on published reports indicating an attenuation of drug-taking or drug-seeking behaviors when microinjected into the brain in doses at, or lower than, the doses used in the present studies (Bäckström and Hyytiä, 2007; Cozzoli et al., 2009, 2012; Gass and Olive, 2009a; Kumaresan et al., 2009; Xie et al., 2010, 2012). The dose of DHPG was selected because it elicits a marked rise in locomotor activity and glutamate release when infused into brain (Swanson and Kalivas, 2000; Swanson et al., 2001; Szumlinski et al., 2004; Melendez et al., 2005). Injectors (projecting 2 mm below the tip of the guide cannulae) were left in place for 1 min after infusion, and then animals were tested for cue-maintained lever-pressing behavior in a 30 min test immediately after the intracranial injection. Control animals received intra-vmPFC infusions of 0.1% DMSO vehicle (VEH) (for JNJ 16259585) or water (for MPEP, MTEP, and DHPG). The data for this reiteration of the antagonist study were analyzed using an intravenous (saline vs cocaine) × intracranial (VEH, MPEP, MTEP, JNJ 16259685) ANOVA. Because only cocaine-experienced animals were assayed in this reiteration of the DHPG study, the data were analyzed using t tests across the intracranial factor. α = 0.05 for all analyses.

Intra-vmPFC infusion of group 1 mGluR compounds: residual effects on drug seeking.

We next tested the possibility that the reduction in vmPFC group 1 mGluR expression observed at 30 d withdrawal from cocaine self-administration (see Figs. 2D, 3A) might render an animal more resistant to extinction if tested repeatedly. The following was done to test this hypothesis: (1) animals in the acute MTEP experiment were subjected to a second drug-seeking test (day 4 of withdrawal) in the absence of any additional intracranial manipulations; (2) the JNJ 16259585 study was replicated in a separate group of animals, which were tested again for effects of vmPFC mGluR1 blockade on day 3 of withdrawal, followed by a second cue test in the absence of any additional pretreatment on day 4 of withdrawal; and (3) the DHPG was replicated in a separate group of animals, which were tested again for effects of vmPFC group 1 mGluR stimulation on day 30 of withdrawal, followed by a second cue test in the absence of any additional pretreatment on day 31 of withdrawal. To determine whether or not the effects of vmPFC group 1 mGluR antagonism on behavior on the second cue test reflected an impairment in operant learning during the first cue-reinforced test session or an impairment in memory consolidation post session, additional cocaine-experienced rats were infused intra-vmPFC with 30 ng/side JNJ 16259585 immediately after the first test session on day 3 of withdrawal and then tested subsequently for cue-induced lever-pressing behavior the next day in the absence of any additional intracranial manipulation. Finally, to confirm that the behavioral effects that we observed resulted from manipulating group 1 mGluR signaling specifically within the ventral region of the medial PFC, additional groups of Coc6h rats were infused intra-dmPFC (injectors projecting 1 mm below the tip of the guide cannulae; see Fig. 6) with 30 ng/side JNJ 16259585 or VEH immediately before a test for cue seeking at 3 d withdrawal, followed by a second cue in the absence of any additional pretreatment on day 4 of withdrawal. The data from these neuropharmacological studies were analyzed initially using an intravenous (saline vs cocaine) × intracranial (VEH, MTEP, JNJ 16259685 or VEH, DHPG) × test (test 1 vs test 2) ANOVA (results not reported). In all cases, we observed significant three-way interactions (p < 0.05), and thus, the data were deconstructed first along the intravenous factor and analyzed using an intracranial × test ANOVA to determine whether or not intra-PFC manipulations depended on the drug experience of the animal. To statistically confirm the presence/absence of extinction, significant intracranial × test interactions were then deconstructed along the intracranial factor and analyzed using a repeated-measures ANOVA across the two cue test sessions (α = 0.05 for all analyses). A significant reduction in lever responding from test 1 to test 2 indicated the presence of extinction, whereas a lack of statistical differences were interpreted to reflect an extinction impairment.

Histology

After the completion of all behavioral pharmacological experiments, animals were killed for histological verification of microinjector placement within the vmPFC or dmPFC. Only animals exhibiting correct placement within ventral prelimbic or within the infralimbic cortices were included in the statistical analyses of the vmPFC neuropharmacological data. Only animals exhibiting correct placement within the dorsal prelimbic or within the anterior cingulate were included in the statistical analyses of the dmPFC neuropharmacological data. Examples of microinjector tip placements from several of the experiments in this study are included in Figure 6. For the most part, two to three animals per group were excluded from the statistical analyses of the behavioral data as a result of misplaced microinjection sites or occlusion of the guide cannulae, and the final sample sizes used in the statistical analyses of the data are indicated in the appropriate figure or table legend.

Results

Time-dependent intensification of cue-induced drug seeking occurs after protracted withdrawal only in cocaine self-administering animals

An analysis of the behavior exhibited by our three intravenous treatment groups during a 2 h test for cue-reinforced behavior conducted under extinction conditions at 3 versus 30 d withdrawal revealed a significant intravenous treatment (Sal1h, Sal6h, Coc6h) × withdrawal (3 vs 30 d) × lever (active vs inactive) interaction (F(2,58) = 12.09, p < 0.0001). Animals with a 10 d history of extended-access intravenous cocaine self-administration (Coc6h) exhibited a time-dependent increase (∼50%) in the number of active lever presses emitted for the contingent presentation of the tone–light stimulus complex previously paired with cocaine delivery when tested in the absence of additional cocaine delivery (Fig. 2A), and there was no concurrent change in the number of inactive lever presses (Fig. 2B) (withdrawal × lever ANOVA, F(1,16) = 12.55, p = 0.003; for active lever, F(1,16) = 5.29, p = 0.04; for inactive lever, p = 0.72). In addition, because no differences were observed in self-administration behavior during training, between the Coc6h groups tested (and killed) at the different withdrawal time points, the time-dependent increase in cocaine-seeking behavior could not be explained by group differences in cocaine intake or behavioral responding for drug (Table 1). As expected, animals with a history of either brief or extended access to intravenous saline (respectively, Sal1h and Sal6h) exhibited significantly lower levels of active lever pressing than cocaine self-administering animals during both self-administration training (Table 1) and cue testing (Fig. 2). Importantly, in contrast to the cocaine groups, saline groups exhibited a time-dependent increase in responding on both active and inactive levers (for Sal1h: withdrawal effect, F(1,21) = 16.93, p < 0.0001; withdrawal × lever, p = 0.4; for Sal6h: withdrawal × lever, F(1,21) = 8.21, p = 0.009; one-way ANOVA for active lever, F(1,21) = 19.40, p < 0.0001; for inactive lever, F(1,21) = 4.11, p = 0.05).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Protracted withdrawal from self-administered cocaine results in increased reinforced responding concomitant with a reduction in vmPFC group 1 mGluR expression. Summary of the total number of active (A) and inactive (B) lever presses emitted by animals with a 10 d history of extended access (6 h/d) to either cocaine (Coc6h) or saline (Sal6h) and by animals with an equivalent history of limited access (1 h/d) to saline (Sal1h) on a 2 h test in which each active lever press resulted in the presentation of the tone–light stimulus complex previously paired with saline/cocaine infusion but no saline/cocaine delivery. Tests were conducted in separate groups of animals at either 3 or 30 d withdrawal from saline/cocaine self-administration, and sample sizes ranged from 9 to 12 per group per withdrawal time point. Immediately after completion of behavioral testing, the dmPFC and vmPFC were dissected out and immunoblotting conducted for mGluR1 and mGluR5. Group differences in vmPFC receptor expression were not observed when assayed at 3 d withdrawal (C), but Coc6h rats exhibited reduced mGluR1/5 when assayed at 30 d withdrawal (D). In contrast, immunoblotting on dmPFC of these same animals revealed elevated receptor levels in Coc6h animals at both 3 and 30 d withdrawal (E and F, respectively). Sample sizes are indicated in A (3 d/30 d withdrawal). For A, *p < 0.05 versus respective data at 3 d withdrawal. For B–F, *p < 0.05 versus respective Sal1h group.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Comparison of the number of active and inactive lever presses, as well as the number of infusions earned, by animals with extended access to cocaine (6 h/d; Coc6h) or to saline (Sal6h) and by animals with brief access to saline (1 h/d; Sal1h) during the last 3 d of self-administration training before being tested for cue-reinforced behavior and tissue harvest for immunoblotting at 3 and 30 d withdrawal

Cue-induced cocaine seeking produces subregionally distinct and time-dependent changes in PFC group 1 mGluR expression during withdrawal

We next used conventional immunoblotting strategies to relate the time-dependent increase in cue-reinforced responding to the levels of mGluR1 and mGluR5 within PFC subregions. A comparison of vmPFC receptor levels between Sal1h, Sal6h, and Coc6h animals failed to indicate group differences for any protein when animals were killed immediately after the 2 h test session at 3 d withdrawal (Fig. 2C; one-way ANOVAs, p values >0.05); however, vmPFC levels of both receptors were reduced in Coc6h rats, relative to both saline groups, when animals were killed after testing at 30 d withdrawal (Fig. 2D) (mGluR1, F(2,33) = 13.78, p < 0.0001; mGluR5, F(2,33) = 5.74, p = 0.008; LSD post hoc tests, p < 0.05). Because these data suggested a time-dependent decrease in vmPFC mGluR1/5 levels selectively in animals with a history of cocaine self-administration, as well as no differences between the saline groups (p > 0.05), we conducted a second round of immunoblotting to directly compare for changes in protein expression after cue testing at 3 versus 30 d in Sal6h and Coc6h animals (Fig. 3). Orthogonal comparisons between the two withdrawal time points indicated no change in vmPFC mGluR1/5 expression in Sal6h rats (t tests, p > 0.05), whereas the levels of both receptors were significantly lower within the vmPFC at the 30 versus 3 d time point in Coc6h animals (Fig. 3A) (mGluR1, t(16) = 2.61, p = 0.02; mGluR5, t(16) = 2.20, p = 0.05). Interestingly, the influence of cocaine experience and withdrawal on vmPFC mGluR1/5 expression appeared to depend on the test history of the subjects because no group differences in vmPFC receptor expression were observed when protein was analyzed in test-naive rats (Fig. 3B; t tests, all p values >0.05). Thus, the opportunity to engage in cue-reinforced behavior after protracted cocaine withdrawal, and not cocaine withdrawal per se, reduced vmPFC group 1 mGluR expression.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Differential effects of testing for cue-reinforced behavior on mGluR1/5 expression within PFC subregions during protracted withdrawal from self-administered cocaine. Immunoblotting for the change in total mGluR1 and mGluR5 protein expression within the vmPFC and dmPFC was conducted on tissue derived from saline- and cocaine-experienced animals tested for cue-reinforced behavior over a 2 h period (Cue Test) or left undisturbed in their home cages (No Cue Test) at 3 or 30 d after a 10 d history of extended access (6 h/d) to either cocaine (Coc6h) or saline (Sal6h). Sample sizes ranged from 9 to 12 per group per withdrawal time point. A, A direct comparison of receptor protein expression within the vmPFC of Sal6h and Coc6h animals across the two withdrawal time points confirmed a time-dependent reduction in mGluR1/5 expression in Coc6h animals that were tested for cue-reinforced behavior before tissue harvest. B, No statistically reliable change in vmPFC receptor expression was apparent in Coc6h rats that were not tested for cue-reinforced behavior before tissue collection. C, In contrast to the vmPFC, immunoblotting on dmPFC of these same animals revealed elevated receptor levels in Coc6h animals tested for cue-reinforced behavior at both 3 and 30 d withdrawal. D, However, as also observed for the vmPFC, the changes in dmPFC mGluR1/5 expression was not apparent in animals left undisturbed before tissue harvest. Sample sizes are indicated in parentheses. *p < 0.05 versus respective data at 3 d withdrawal.

In contrast to the vmPFC, dmPFC levels of mGluR1 and mGluR5 were elevated in Coc6h animals relative to both saline self-administration groups at both 3 d (Fig. 2E) (mGluR1, F(2,29) = 3.64, p = 0.04; mGluR5, F(2,29) = 4.23, p = 0.03; LSD post hoc tests) and 30 d (Fig. 2F) (mGluR1, F(2,32) = 27.98, p < 0.0001; mGluR5, F(2,32) = 5.83, p = 0.007; LSD post hoc tests) withdrawal. A direct comparison of dmPFC tissue from Sal6h rats across both withdrawal time points (Fig. 3C) revealed no time-dependent changes in mGluR5, but a significant time-dependent reduction in mGluR1 (t(21) = 2.19, p = 0.04). No time-dependent changes in dmPFC receptor expression were apparent in cocaine-experienced animals (t test, p < 0.05), despite their obvious increase in mGluR1/5 expression relative to saline-experienced animals. Although there was a trend for test-naive, cocaine-experienced animals to exhibit elevated dmPFC mGluR1 expression at both withdrawal time points, group differences in basal mGluR1 or mGluR5 expression were not statistically reliable (Fig. 3D; p values >0.05).

Intra-vmPFC pharmacological manipulations of group 1 mGluR function do not acutely affect cue-induced seeking behavior during withdrawal

The average number of infusions earned across the last three self-administration sessions were similar in the different intra-vmPFC treatment groups (Table 2; p values >0.05). Although an intra-vmPFC infusion of the mGluR1a antagonist JNJ 16259685 appeared to increase the number of active lever presses emitted by cocaine rats, the results of the statistical analysis failed to indicate any significant acute effects of our various intra-vmPFC pretreatments on the behavior of either Sal1h (Table 3) or Coc6h (Fig. 4A) animals when tested for cue-reinforced responding at 3 d withdrawal (intravenous effect, F(1,114) = 128.02, p < 0.0001; intracranial effect and intracranial × intravenous interaction, p values >0.15). In addition, we failed to observe any influence of our intra-vmPFC pretreatments on the number of inactive lever presses emitted during this test session (total inactive lever presses in 30 min: VEH, 5.3 ± 1.7; MPEP, 4.8 ± 1.2; MTEP, 8.4 ± 2.8; JNJ 16259685, 3.6 ± 1.0; one-way ANOVA, p = 0.36).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Comparison of the average number of infusions earned by saline (1 h/d; Sal1h) and cocaine (6 h/d; Coc6h) self-administering rats during the last 3 d of self-administration before studies of the effects of intra-vmPFC manipulations of group 1 mGluRs at 3 d (antagonist studies) or 30 d (agonist study) withdrawal

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3.

Comparison of the average number of lever presses emitted by saline-experienced animals during tests for cue-reinforced behavior conducted at 3 d (antagonist studies) or 30 d (agonist study) after the last saline self-administration session

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Intra-vmPFC pretreatment with mGluR-targeting compounds does not acutely influence cue-reinforced responding in cocaine-experienced animals. A, When assessed at 3 d withdrawal, an infusion of 3.0 μg/side of the mGluR5 antagonists MPEP or MTEP or 30 pg/side of the mGluR1 antagonist JNJ 16259685 (JNJ) failed to significantly influence cue-reinforced lever-pressing behavior exhibited by cocaine-experienced animals during a 30 min test of drug seeking. B, Similarly, an infusion of 27.5 ng/side of the mGluR1/5 agonist DHPG also failed to influence lever-pressing behavior when assessed in rats with a history of cocaine self-administration at 30 d withdrawal. Sample sizes are indicated in parentheses.

Similarly, the data from a study using only cocaine-experienced animals also failed to indicate any significant effect of an intra-vmPFC infusion of the group 1 mGluR agonist DHPG on lever pressing on a test conducted at 30 d withdrawal (Fig. 4B) (t test, p > 0.20).

vmPFC group 1 mGluR blockade impairs extinction learning during short-term withdrawal from cocaine

To determine whether the reduction in vmPFC group 1 mGluR expression observed at 30 d withdrawal in Coc6h animals tested for cue-reinforced behavior relates to impaired extinction learning in these animals, we assayed for residual effects of vmPFC group 1 mGluR blockade either before or after the initial test of cue-reinforced behavior (without additional pharmacological manipulation) 24 h later. As was the case for the initial studies, there were no significant differences regarding the saline/cocaine intake of the animals during the last 3 d of self-administration training between the different intracranial treatment groups before testing (Table 2; all p values >0.05).

As was observed previously, manipulations of vmPFC group 1 mGluRs failed to influence the behavior of saline-experienced animals on either test, although all saline groups exhibited greater lever-pressing behavior on the second test, in the absence of intracranial microinjection (Table 3) (test effect, F(1,27) = 19.8, p < 0.0001; all other p values >0.50). A comparison of the behavior of cocaine-experienced rats revealed that the change in behavior across the two cue tests depended on the intracranial treatment of the animals (Fig. 5A) (intracranial group × test, F(3,37) = 3.98, p = 0.02). Deconstruction of the significant interaction along the intracranial group factor revealed a test-dependent reduction in responding in rats infused intra-vmPFC with VEH before initial testing for cue-reinforced behavior (F(1,10) = 9.17, p = 0.01), indicating extinction learning. Interestingly, cocaine-experienced rats infused intra-vmPFC with either MTEP or JNJ 16259685 before initial testing failed to show this test-dependent reduction in responding (respectively, p = 0.27 and p = 0.61), indicating impaired extinction learning or memory. In support of the latter possibility, cocaine-experienced animals infused intra-vmPFC with JNJ 16259685 immediately after the initial test also failed to exhibit a test-dependent reduction in lever-pressing behavior compared with controls (p = 0.45).

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Intra-vmPFC pretreatment with mGluR-targeting compounds produces residual effects on cue-reinforced responding in cocaine-experienced animals. A, When tested for cue-reinforced behavior 24 h after an intra-vmPFC infusion of 3.0 μg/side MTEP before initial testing for cue-reinforced behavior (MTEP PreRx) or an intra-vmPFC infusion of 30 pg/side JNJ 16259685 either before (JNJ PreRx) or immediately after (JNJ PostRx) the initial test for cue-reinforced behavior in the absence of any additional intracranial manipulation, VEH-pretreated rats exhibited reduced responding (extinction), whereas no behavioral extinction was apparent in animals pretreated or posttreated with mGluR antagonists. B, In contrast to the vmPFC, intra-dmPFC pretreatment with 30 pg/side JNJ 16259685 failed to influence behavioral extinction at 3 d withdrawal. C, Interestingly, when assayed at 30 d withdrawal, VEH-pretreated rats exhibited no sign of behavioral extinction when tested for cue-reinforced behavior, whereas extinction was facilitated in cocaine-experienced animals pretreated with 27 ng/side DHPG. Sample sizes are indicated in parentheses. *p < 0.05 versus test 1. +p < 0.05, main effect of test. WD, Withdrawal.

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Histological verification of microinjector placements within the PFC. A diagram depicting the results of our histological examination of microinjector placements conducted on the completion of testing for the residual effects of an intra-vmPFC infusion of VEH, MTEP, JNJ 16259685 (JNJ), and DHPG and of an intra-dmPFC infusion of JNJ 16259685, as examples of placements for the animals included in the statistical analyses of the data in this study. As exemplified by the saline (open circles) and cocaine (filled circles) animals in this figure, only animals exhibiting microinjector placement within the ventral prelimbic cortex, within the infralimbic cortex or at their interface, were included in the statistical analyses of the data for vmPFC studies. As exemplified by the VEH (open circles) and JNJ 16259685 (filled circles) animals in the panel for the dmPFC study, only animals exhibiting microinjector placement within the dorsal prelimbic cortex, the anterior cingulate, or their interface were included in the statistical analyses of the data for the dmPFC study.

We also assayed the acute and residual effects of intra-dmPFC pretreatment with JNJ 16259685 on cue-reinforced responding at 3 d withdrawal. As illustrated in Figure 5B, acute pretreatment with the mGluR1 antagonist into the dmPFC failed to influence cue-reinforced behavior, nor did it alter the test-dependent reduction in behavior observed at 3 d withdrawal (test effect, F(1,14) = 5.46, p = 0.04; intracranial effect and interaction, p > 0.90).

vmPFC group 1 mGluR stimulation facilitates extinction learning after protracted withdrawal from cocaine

Here again, no differences were observed for the average number of lever presses emitted for infusions of either saline or cocaine during self-administration training between the groups slated to receive intra-vmPFC VEH versus DHPG (Table 2) (intravenous effect, F(1,45) = 66.15, p < 0.0001; no intracranial effect or interaction, p values >0.20). Similar to the results for the antagonists, intra-vmPFC DHPG pretreatment failed to alter the behavior of saline-experienced animals, and all rats exhibited higher responding on the second test in the absence of any additional intracranial manipulation (Table 3) (test effect, F(1,26) = 9.07, p = 0.006; intracranial effect and interaction, p > 0.17). Replicating the data from our initial study (Fig. 5B), we failed to detect a significant acute effect of intra-vmPFC DHPG in cocaine-experienced animals on cue-reinforced behavior at 30 d withdrawal. In fact, an analysis of the time course (in 5 min intervals) of lever-pressing behavior during the first cue test indicated that within-session extinction did not differ between VEH- and DHPG-pretreated cocaine-experienced rats (time effect, F(5,70) = 11.67, p < 0.0001; time × intracranial interaction, p > 0.05). However, we observed differential effects of intra-vmPFC pretreatment on the change in behavior during subsequent testing (Fig. 5C) (test effect, F(1,21) = 5.55, p = 0.03; intracranial effect, p = 0.62; test × intracranial, F(1,21) = 6.15, p = 002). As illustrated in Figure 5C, VEH-infused cocaine rats exhibited no change in behavior across the two cue tests (p = 0.90), indicating resistance to extinction during protracted withdrawal. In contrast, DHPG-infused cocaine rats exhibited an ∼30% reduction in lever pressing during subsequent testing, and this was statistically significant (F(1,6) = 8.63, p = 0.03).

Discussion

The present report provides validation of an important role for vmPFC group 1 mGluRs in learning to suppress cocaine-seeking behavior during protracted withdrawal from heavy cocaine intake. Specifically, “incubated” drug-seeking and across-session extinction deficits exhibited by cocaine-experienced rats during protracted withdrawal were associated with reduced vmPFC mGluR1/5 levels, which reflected an interaction between cocaine withdrawal and the opportunity to engage in cue-reinforced behavior under extinction conditions. Interestingly, intra-vmPFC manipulations of mGluR1/5 function had no immediate effects on cue-reinforced behavior under extinction conditions but produced residual effects on subsequent behavioral testing 24 h later. Intra-vmPFC mGluR1/5 blockade, either before or after testing for cue-reinforced behavior, impaired, whereas receptor hyperstimulation before testing facilitated, the manifestation of extinction learning in cocaine-experienced rats during short- and long-term withdrawal, respectively. In contrast, intra-dmPFC mGluR1 blockade failed to influence cue-reinforced behavior. Together, these results support the hypothesis that reduced vmPFC group 1 mGluR function is a time-dependent adaptation that manifests when a heavily cocaine-experienced animal is given the opportunity to engage in drug seeking during withdrawal. This reduction in vmPFC group 1 mGluR function impairs the ability to consolidate memories related to new behavior-outcome contingences in the absence of drug, rendering it more difficult to suppress drug craving and drug-seeking behavior when presented with stimuli predictive of drug availability.

Cue-reinforced behavior and the regulation of PFC group 1 mGluR expression

Alterations in PFC mGluR1/5 expression were observed only in cocaine-experienced animals allowed a single (2 h) opportunity to engage in cue-reinforced behavior in the absence of cocaine. As reported previously (Ben-Shahar et al., 2009; Hao et al., 2010; Ghasemzadeh et al., 2011), we failed to detect changes in PFC group 1 mGluR expression in cocaine-experienced animals left undisturbed in the home cage. Using procedures akin to those used in the present study, Ghasemzadeh et al. (2011) also failed to detect changes in either vmPFC or dmPFC mGluR5 expression in cocaine-experienced animals withdrawn for 10 d in the absence of extinction training. However, they also failed to observe receptor changes within the vmPFC of animals with extensive (10 × 6-h sessions) extinction training but observed reduced mGluR5 levels within the dmPFC in behaviorally extinguished animals (Ghasemzadeh et al., 2011). Such data indicate that, akin to reports for mPFC AMPA glutamate receptors in heroin-experienced animals (Van den Oever et al., 2010), mGluR1/5 expression within both the vmPFC and dmPFC appears to be regulated in a complex manner by interactions between cocaine experience, duration of withdrawal, and the opportunity to engage in cue-reinforced behavior in the absence of cocaine. Furthermore, given the discrepancies in biochemical results between the present study (1 × 2-h extinction session) versus the previous study by Ghasemzadeh et al. (2011) (10 × 6-h extinction sessions), it would appear that the effects of cocaine on mGluR1/5 expression within both the vmPFC and dmPFC depends also on the extent to which animals have undergone extinction training and/or manifest drug-seeking behavior, with vmPFC mGluR1/5 levels inversely related to drug seeking and dmPFC receptor levels positively related to drug seeking.

Group 1 mGluRs undergo rapid desensitization involving receptor internalization and then degradation, and this develops tolerance on repeated receptor activation (Aronica et al., 1993; Desai et al., 1995, 1996; Gereau and Heinemann, 1998). Thus, an incubation of cue-elicited glutamate release within the vmPFC might contribute not only to downregulated receptor expression observed within this subregion during initial extinction learning (present study) but also the apparent tolerance to this effect on extensive extinction training (Ghasemzadeh et al., 2011). At the present time, it is difficult to envision how a similar mechanism might be in place to regulate mGluR1/5 expression within dmPFC because receptor levels are increased in animals during initial extinction learning (present study) but decreased in behaviorally extinguished animals (Ghasemzadeh et al., 2011). It is not known whether or not cue-elicited glutamate release within PFC subregions varies as a function of cocaine experience/withdrawal. Because group 1 mGluRs are perisynaptic in localization, future studies should focus on elucidating the relationship between subregional differences within PFC glutamate receptor expression and extracellular glutamate and how this relationship varies as a function of drug-related learning to begin to understand more precisely the mechanisms through which mGluR1/5 is differentially regulated within PFC subregions by the opportunity to engage in cue-reinforced behavior in the absence of cocaine.

Neuropharmacological manipulations of vmPFC group 1 mGluRs and cue seeking

Pharmacological manipulations of group 1 mGluR function produced no obvious acute effects on cue-reinforced responding. Thus, neither vmPFC nor dmPFC group 1 mGluRs play a major role in the cognitive, motivational, or motor capacity required to engage in cue-reinforced behavior or in the processes that underpin the expression of incubated cue-reinforced behavior during protracted withdrawal. To our knowledge, this study is the first to address the role for PFC group 1 mGluRs during initial extinction of cocaine seeking. As such, we used doses of MPEP, MTEP, and JNJ 16259685 that are within the range of those demonstrated to maximally inhibit drug reward and/or reinforcement when infused intracranially in other animal models of drug seeking or drug taking, and the dose of DHPG used was at or above maximal for increasing locomotion and extracellular glutamate levels when infused into the brain (see Materials and Methods). The fact that we observed residual behavioral effects of our intra-vmPFC pretreatments argues against inadequate antagonist/agonist dosing as an explanation for our negative behavioral data. To date, all of the studies that have examined a potential role for group 1 mGluRs in cue- or context-reinforced behavior have used animals with extensive extinction training, and typically, antagonist infusion into either the nucleus accumbens or hippocampus reduces cocaine seeking during cue/context reexposure (mGluR1: Dravolina et al., 2007; mGluR5: Bäckström et al., 2004; Bespalov et al., 2005; Schroeder et al., 2005; Gass and Olive, 2009b; Kumaresan et al., 2009; Martin-Fardon et al., 2009; Xie et al., 2010; Martin-Fardon and Weiss, 2011; Moussawi et al., 2011; Xie et al., 2012). Although there is some indication that prelimbic and infralimbic cortical inactivation influences drug-seeking behavior only in animals having undergone extinction training (McFarland and Kalivas, 2001; Capriles et al., 2003; Fuchs et al., 2006; Peters et al., 2008; but see Koya et al., 2009), the role of group 1 mGluRs within PFC subregions in drug seeking under extinction conditions remains to be examined.

Neuropharmacological manipulations of vmPFC group 1 mGluRs and behavioral extinction

More ventral aspects of the PFC are posited to play a critical role in extinction of cocaine-seeking behavior (Schmidt et al., 2005; Peters et al., 2008; Hayton et al., 2010; Van den Oever et al., 2010), and there is an important glutamatergic component in this regard (Gass and Olive, 2009b; Hayton et al., 2010; Van den Oever et al., 2010; Millan et al., 2011). Consistent with this idea, dmPFC mGluR1 antagonist infusion failed to influence the manifestation of extinction learning during early withdrawal, whereas pharmacological manipulations of vmPFC mGluR1/5 function exerted extinction-modulating effects on the residual tests of cue-reinforced behavior. Mimicking the cocaine-induced reduction in vmPFC mGluR1/5 expression observed after protracted withdrawal was sufficient to elicit an extinction impairment akin to that exhibited by cocaine-experienced animals during protracted withdrawal. Interestingly, a similar MPEP-induced impairment of extinction of conditioned fear has been reported recently (Fontanez-Nuin et al., 2011) and is consistent with a general role of mGluR1/5 receptors in consolidation of learning (Riedel et al., 2003). Conversely, “rescuing” vmPFC group 1 mGluR function by high-dose DHPG infusion reversed the extinction deficit and reduced subsequent drug-seeking behavior exhibited by cocaine-experienced rats during protracted withdrawal. Overall, these data are consistent with recent demonstrations that systemic pretreatment with an mGluR5-positive allosteric modulator facilitates behavioral extinction and reduces drug-seeking behavior in cocaine-experienced animals (Cleva et al., 2011) and point to vmPFC group 1 mGluRs as an important neural substrate in mediating these pro-cognitive and anti-addictive effects.

Electrophysiological and immunocytochemical evidence indicates that there is hyperactivation of vmPFC neurons by the behavior-contingent presentation of cocaine-paired cues early during extinction training in animals with a history of cocaine self-administration (Sun and Rebec, 2006; Koya et al., 2009). Particularly noteworthy are the parallel results for reduced vmPFC mGluR1/5 expression in the present study and elevated vmPFC levels of phospho-extracellular signal-regulated kinase (ERK) reported by Koya et al. (2009) for cocaine-withdrawn rats assayed under very similar self-administration and cue-reinforced testing conditions to those used in the present study. The inverse relationship between vmPFC group 1 mGluR and phospho-ERK levels is intriguing in light of recent findings that group 1 mGluR stimulation, particularly mGluR1, augments inhibitory currents to a greater degree than excitatory currents within PFC (Sun and Neugebauer, 2011). Thus, although reduced mGluR1/5 expression may very well reflect a compensatory response to cue-elicited increases in vmPFC neuronal activity, such a reduction is predicted to disinhibit glutamatergic output from the vmPFC, a finding supported by a recent in vivo microdialysis study of animals responding for cocaine-paired cues without cocaine reinforcement (Suto et al., 2010). It follows then that the cue-elicited reduction in vmPFC mGluR1/5 expression observed during protracted withdrawal could very well instigate a feedforward mechanism of neuronal hyperexcitability within this structure that might be responsible for maintaining the saliency of drug-associated cues and the capacity of these cues to elicit drug craving even in protracted abstinence (Volkow et al., 1992; Childress et al., 1999; Goldstein and Volkow, 2002). Thus, the present data pose a time-dependent reduction in vmPFC mGluR1/5 expression as a molecular cordon to addiction recovery that best be overcome by receptor agonist, rather than antagonist, strategies.

Footnotes

  • This work was funded by National Institutes of Health Grants DA024038 (K.K.S.) and DA027525 (T.E.K.), as well as by grants from the Academic Senate of the University of California at Santa Barbara (K.K.S. and O.B.S.). We thank Chelsea Simpson for her technical assistance.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Karen K. Szumlinski, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of California at Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA 93106-9660. szumlinski{at}psych.ucsb.edu

References

  1. ↵
    1. Aronica E,
    2. Nicoletti F,
    3. Condorelli DF,
    4. Balázs R
    (1993) Pharmacological characterization of metabotropic glutamate receptors in cultured cerebellar granule cells. Neurochem Res 18:605–612.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Bäckström P,
    2. Hyyti ä P
    (2006) Ionotropic and metabotropic glutamate receptor antagonism attenuates cue-induced cocaine seeking. Neuropsychopharmacology 31:778–786.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Bäckström P,
    2. Hyyti ä P
    (2007) Involvement of AMPA/kainate, NMDA, and mGlu5 receptors in the nucleus accumbens core in cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking in rats. Psychopharmacology 192:571–580.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Bäckström P,
    2. Bachteler D,
    3. Koch S,
    4. Hyytiä P,
    5. Spanagel R
    (2004) mGluR5 antagonist MPEP reduces ethanol-seeking and relapse behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology 29:921–928.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Ben-Shahar O,
    2. Ettenberg A
    (1998) Amphetamine infusions into the prefrontal cortex attenuate the sensitization to amphetamine. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 22:763–773.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Ben-Shahar O,
    2. Ahmed SH,
    3. Koob GF,
    4. Ettenberg A
    (2004) The transition from controlled to compulsive drug use is associated with a loss of sensitization. Brain Res 995:46–54.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Ben-Shahar O,
    2. Posthumus EJ,
    3. Waldroup SA,
    4. Ettenberg A
    (2008) Heightened drug-seeking motivation following extended daily access to self-administered cocaine. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 32:863–869.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Ben-Shahar O,
    2. Obara I,
    3. Ary AW,
    4. Ma N,
    5. Mangiardi MA,
    6. Medina RL,
    7. Szumlinski KK
    (2009) Extended daily access to cocaine results in distinct alterations in Homer 1b/c and NMDA receptor subunit expression within the medial prefrontal cortex. Synapse 63:598–609.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Ben-Shahar OM,
    2. Szumlinski KK,
    3. Lominac KD,
    4. Cohen A,
    5. Gordon E,
    6. Ploense KL,
    7. Demartini J,
    8. Bernstein N,
    9. Rudy NM,
    10. Nabhan AN,
    11. Sacramento A,
    12. Pagano K,
    13. Carosso GA,
    14. Woodward N
    (2012) Extended access to cocaine self-administration results in reduced glutamate function within the medial prefrontal cortex. Addict Biol 17:746–757.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Bespalov AY,
    2. Dravolina OA,
    3. Sukhanov I,
    4. Zakharova E,
    5. Blokhina E,
    6. Zvartau E,
    7. Danysz W,
    8. van Heeke G,
    9. Markou A
    (2005) Metabotropic glutamate receptor (mGluR5) antagonist MPEP attenuated cue- and schedule-induced reinstatement of nicotine self-administration behavior in rats. Neuropharmacology 49(Suppl 1):167–178.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Bonson KR,
    2. Grant SJ,
    3. Contoreggi CS,
    4. Links JM,
    5. Metcalfe J,
    6. Weyl HL,
    7. Kurian V,
    8. Ernst M,
    9. London ED
    (2002) Neural systems and cue-induced cocaine craving. Neuropsychopharmacology 26:376–386.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Capriles N,
    2. Rodaros D,
    3. Sorge RE,
    4. Stewart J
    (2003) A role for the prefrontal cortex in stress- and cocaine-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking in rats. Psychopharmacology 168:66–74.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Chiamulera C,
    2. Epping-Jordan MP,
    3. Zocchi A,
    4. Marcon C,
    5. Cottiny C,
    6. Tacconi S,
    7. Corsi M,
    8. Orzi F,
    9. Conquet F
    (2001) Reinforcing and locomotor stimulant effects of cocaine are absent in mGluR5 null mutant mice. Nat Neurosci 4:873–874.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Childress AR,
    2. Mozley PD,
    3. McElgin W,
    4. Fitzgerald J,
    5. Reivich M,
    6. O'Brien CP
    (1999) Limbic activation during cue-induced cocaine craving. Am J Psychiatry 156:11–18.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Childress AR,
    2. Ehrman RN,
    3. Wang Z,
    4. Li Y,
    5. Sciortino N,
    6. Hakun J,
    7. Jens W,
    8. Suh J,
    9. Listerud J,
    10. Marquez K,
    11. Franklin T,
    12. Langleben D,
    13. Detre J,
    14. O'Brien CP
    (2008) Prelude to passion: limbic activation by “unseen” drug and sexual cues. PLoS One 3:e1506.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Ciccocioppo R,
    2. Sanna PP,
    3. Weiss F
    (2001) Cocaine-predictive stimulus induces drug-seeking behavior and neural activation in limbic brain regions after multiple months of abstinence: reversal by D(1) antagonists. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98:1976–1981.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    1. Clark L,
    2. Cools R,
    3. Robbins TW
    (2004) The neuropsychology of ventral prefrontal cortex: decision-making and reversal learning. Brain Cogn 55:41–53.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Cleva RM,
    2. Hicks MP,
    3. Gass JT,
    4. Wischerath KC,
    5. Plasters ET,
    6. Widholm JJ,
    7. Olive MF
    (2011) mGluR5 positive allosteric modulation enhances extinction learning following cocaine self-administration. Behav Neurosci 125:10–19.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Cozzoli DK,
    2. Goulding SP,
    3. Zhang PW,
    4. Xiao B,
    5. Hu JH,
    6. Ary AW,
    7. Obara I,
    8. Rahn A,
    9. Abou-Ziab H,
    10. Tyrrel B,
    11. Marini C,
    12. Yoneyama N,
    13. Metten P,
    14. Snelling C,
    15. Dehoff MH,
    16. Crabbe JC,
    17. Finn DA,
    18. Klugmann M,
    19. Worley PF,
    20. Szumlinski KK
    (2009) Binge drinking upregulates accumbens mGluR5-Homer2-PI3K signaling: functional implications for alcoholism. J Neurosci 29:8655–8668.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. ↵
    1. Cozzoli DK,
    2. Courson J,
    3. Caruana AL,
    4. Miller BW,
    5. Greentree DI,
    6. Thomspon AB,
    7. Wroten MG,
    8. Zhang PW,
    9. Xiao B,
    10. Hu JH,
    11. Klugmann M,
    12. Metten P,
    13. Worley PF,
    14. Crabbe JC,
    15. Szumlinski KK
    (2012) Nucleus accumbens mGluR5-associated signaling regulates binge alcohol drinking under drinking-in-the-dark procedures. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 36:1623–1633.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Damasio AR
    (1996) The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 351:1413–1420.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    1. Desai MA,
    2. Burnett JP,
    3. Mayne NG,
    4. Schoepp DD
    (1995) Cloning and expression of a human metabotropic glutamate receptor 1 alpha: enhanced coupling on co-transfection with a glutamate transporter. Mol Pharmacol 48:648–657.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  23. ↵
    1. Desai MA,
    2. Burnett JP,
    3. Mayne NG,
    4. Schoepp DD
    (1996) Pharmacological characterization of desensitization in a human mGlu1 alpha-expressing non-neuronal cell line co-transfected with a glutamate transporter. Br J Pharmacol 118:1558–1564.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Dravolina OA,
    2. Zakharova ES,
    3. Shekunova EV,
    4. Zvartau EE,
    5. Danysz W,
    6. Bespalov AY
    (2007) mGlu1 receptor blockade attenuates cue- and nicotine-induced reinstatement of extinguished nicotine self-administration behavior in rats. Neuropharmacology 52:263–269.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Fontanez-Nuin DE,
    2. Santini E,
    3. Quirk GJ,
    4. Porter JT
    (2011) Memory for fear extinction requires mGluR5-mediated activation of infralimbic neurons. Cereb Cortex 21:727–735.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    1. Franklin TR,
    2. Acton PD,
    3. Maldjian JA,
    4. Gray JD,
    5. Croft JR,
    6. Dackis CA,
    7. O'Brien CP,
    8. Childress AR
    (2002) Decreased gray matter concentration in the insular, orbitofrontal, cingulate, and temporal cortices of cocaine patients. Biol Psychiatry 51:134–142.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Fuchs RA,
    2. Evans KA,
    3. Ledford CC,
    4. Parker MP,
    5. Case JM,
    6. Mehta RH,
    7. See RE
    (2005) The role of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, basolateral amygdala, and dorsal hippocampus in contextual reinstatement of cocaine seeking in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 30:296–309.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Fuchs RA,
    2. Branham RK,
    3. See RE
    (2006) Different neural substrates mediate cocaine seeking after abstinence versus extinction training: a critical role for the dorsolateral caudate-putamen. J Neurosci 26:3584–3588.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Garavan H,
    2. Pankiewicz J,
    3. Bloom A,
    4. Cho JK,
    5. Sperry L,
    6. Ross TJ,
    7. Salmeron BJ,
    8. Risinger R,
    9. Kelley D,
    10. Stein EA
    (2000) Cue-induced cocaine craving: neuroanatomical specificity for drug users and drug stimuli. Am J Psychiatry 157:1789–1798.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Gass JT,
    2. Olive MF
    (2009a) Role of protein kinase C epsilon (PKCvarepsilon) in the reduction of ethanol reinforcement due to mGluR5 antagonism in the nucleus accumbens shell. Psychopharmacology 204:587–597.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  31. ↵
    1. Gass JT,
    2. Olive MF
    (2009b) Positive allosteric modulation of mGluR5 receptors facilitates extinction of a cocaine contextual memory. Biol Psychiatry 65:717–720.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Gereau RW 4th.,
    2. Heinemann SF
    (1998) Role of protein kinase C phosphorylation in rapid desensitization of metabotropic glutamate receptor 5. Neuron 20:143–151.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Ghasemzadeh MB,
    2. Vasudevan P,
    3. Mueller C
    (2009) Locomotor sensitization to cocaine is associated with distinct pattern of glutamate receptor trafficking to the postsynaptic density in prefrontal cortex: early versus late withdrawal effects. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 92:383–392.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Ghasemzadeh MB,
    2. Vasudevan P,
    3. Giles C,
    4. Purgianto A,
    5. Seubert C,
    6. Mantsch JR
    (2011) Glutamatergic plasticity in medial prefrontal cortex and ventral tegmental area following extended-access cocaine self-administration. Brain Res 1413:60–71.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Goldstein RZ,
    2. Volkow ND
    (2002) Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry 159:1642–1652.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Goldstein RZ,
    2. Alia-Klein N,
    3. Tomasi D,
    4. Carrillo JH,
    5. Maloney T,
    6. Woicik PA,
    7. Wang R,
    8. Telang F,
    9. Volkow ND
    (2009a) Anterior cingulate cortex hypoactivations to an emotionally salient task in cocaine addiction. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:9453–9458.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    1. Goldstein RZ,
    2. Craig AD,
    3. Bechara A,
    4. Garavan H,
    5. Childress AR,
    6. Paulus MP,
    7. Volkow ND
    (2009b) The neurocircuitry of impaired insight in drug addiction. Trends Cogn Sci 13:372–380.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Goulding SP,
    2. Obara I,
    3. Lominac KD,
    4. Gould AT,
    5. Miller BW,
    6. Klugmann M,
    7. Szumlinski KK
    (2011) Accumbens Homer2-mediated signaling: a factor contributing to mouse strain differences in alcohol drinking? Genes Brain Behav 10:111–126.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Grant S,
    2. London ED,
    3. Newlin DB,
    4. Villemagne VL,
    5. Liu X,
    6. Contoreggi C,
    7. Phillips RL,
    8. Kimes AS,
    9. Margolin A
    (1996) Activation of memory circuits during cue-elicited cocaine craving. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 93:12040–12045.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  40. ↵
    1. Grimm JW,
    2. Hope BT,
    3. Wise RA,
    4. Shaham Y
    (2001) Neuroadaptation. Incubation of cocaine craving after withdrawal. Nature 412:141–142.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Grimm JW,
    2. Shaham Y,
    3. Hope BT
    (2002) Effect of cocaine and sucrose withdrawal period on extinction behavior, cue-induced reinstatement, and protein levels of the dopamine transporter and tyrosine hydroxylase in limbic and cortical areas in rats. Behav Pharmacol 13:379–388.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Hamlin AS,
    2. Clemens KJ,
    3. McNally GP
    (2008) Renewal of extinguished cocaine-seeking. Neuroscience 151:659–670.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Hao Y,
    2. Martin-Fardon R,
    3. Weiss F
    (2010) Behavioral and functional evidence of metabotropic glutamate receptor 2/3 and metabotropic glutamate receptor 5 dysregulation in cocaine-escalated rats: factor in the transition to dependence. Biol Psychiatry 68:240–248.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Hayton SJ,
    2. Lovett-Barron M,
    3. Dumont EC,
    4. Olmstead MC
    (2010) Target-specific encoding of response inhibition: increased contribution of AMPA to NMDA receptors at excitatory synapses in the prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 30:11493–11500.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  45. ↵
    1. Hearing MC,
    2. See RE,
    3. McGinty JF
    (2008) Relapse to cocaine-seeking increases activity-regulated gene expression differentially in the striatum and cerebral cortex of rats following short or long periods of abstinence. Brain Struct Funct 213:215–227.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Iso Y,
    2. Grajkowska E,
    3. Wroblewski JT,
    4. Davis J,
    5. Goeders NE,
    6. Johnson KM,
    7. Sanker S,
    8. Roth BL,
    9. Tueckmantel W,
    10. Kozikowski AP
    (2006) Synthesis and structure–activity relationships of 3-[(2-methyl-1,3-thiazol-4-yl)ethynyl]pyridine analogues as potent, noncompetitive metabotropic glutamate receptor subtype 5 antagonists; search for cocaine medications. J Med Chem 49:1080–1100.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Kalivas PW,
    2. O'Brien C
    (2008) Drug addiction as a pathology of staged neuroplasticity. Neuropsychopharmacology 33:166–180.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Kenny PJ,
    2. Markou A
    (2004) The ups and downs of addiction: role of metabotropic glutamate receptors. Trends Pharmacol Sci 25:265–272.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Kerstetter KA,
    2. Aguilar VR,
    3. Parrish AB,
    4. Kippin TE
    (2008) Protracted time-dependent increases in cocaine-seeking behavior during cocaine withdrawal in female relative to male rats. Psychopharmacology 198:63–75.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Konishi S,
    2. Nakajima K,
    3. Uchida I,
    4. Sekihara K,
    5. Miyashita Y
    (1998) No-go dominant brain activity in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Eur J Neurosci 10:1209–1213.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Koya E,
    2. Uejima JL,
    3. Wihbey KA,
    4. Bossert JM,
    5. Hope BT,
    6. Shaham Y
    (2009) Role of ventral medial prefrontal cortex in incubation of cocaine craving. Neuropharmacology 56(Suppl 1):177–185.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Kumaresan V,
    2. Yuan M,
    3. Yee J,
    4. Famous KR,
    5. Anderson SM,
    6. Schmidt HD,
    7. Pierce RC
    (2009) Metabotropic glutamate receptor 5 (mGluR5) antagonists attenuate cocaine priming- and cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking. Behav Brain Res 202:238–244.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. London ED,
    2. Ernst M,
    3. Grant S,
    4. Bonson K,
    5. Weinstein A
    (2000) Orbitofrontal cortex and human drug abuse: functional imaging. Cereb Cortex 10:334–342.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  54. ↵
    1. Martin-Fardon R,
    2. Weiss F
    (2011) (−)-2-oxa-4-aminobicylco[3.1.0]hexane-4,6-dicarboxylic acid (LY379268) and 3-[(2-methyl-1,3-thiazol-4-yl)ethynyl]piperidine (MTEP) similarly attenuate stress-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking. Addict Biol doi:10.1111/j.1369–1600.2011.00345.x, Advance online publication. Retrieved November 19, 2012.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  55. ↵
    1. Martin-Fardon R,
    2. Baptista MA,
    3. Dayas CV,
    4. Weiss F
    (2009) Dissociation of the effects of MTEP [3-[(2-methyl-1,3-thiazol-4-yl)ethynyl]piperidine] on conditioned reinstatement and reinforcement: comparison between cocaine and a conventional reinforcer. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 329:1084–1090.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  56. ↵
    1. Matochik JA,
    2. London ED,
    3. Eldreth DA,
    4. Cadet JL,
    5. Bolla KI
    (2003) Frontal cortical tissue composition in abstinent cocaine abusers: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroimage 19:1095–1102.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. McFarland K,
    2. Kalivas PW
    (2001) The circuitry mediating cocaine-induced reinstatement of drug-seeking behavior. J Neurosci 21:8655–8663.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  58. ↵
    1. McLaughlin J,
    2. See RE
    (2003) Selective inactivation of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex and the basolateral amygdala attenuates conditioned-cued reinstatement of extinguished cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. Psychopharmacology 168:57–65.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Melendez RI,
    2. Vuthiganon J,
    3. Kalivas PW
    (2005) Regulation of extracellular glutamate in the prefrontal cortex: focus on the cystine-glutamate exchanger and group I metabotropic glutamate receptors. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 314:139–147.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  60. ↵
    1. Millan EZ,
    2. Marchant NJ,
    3. McNally GP
    (2011) Extinction of drug seeking. Behav Brain Res 217:454–462.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Moussawi K,
    2. Zhou W,
    3. Shen H,
    4. Reichel CM,
    5. See RE,
    6. Carr DB,
    7. Kalivas PW
    (2011) Reversing cocaine-induced synaptic potentiation provides enduring protection from relapse. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:385–390.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  62. ↵
    1. Neisewander JL,
    2. Baker DA,
    3. Fuchs RA,
    4. Tran-Nguyen LT,
    5. Palmer A,
    6. Marshall JF
    (2000) Fos protein expression and cocaine-seeking behavior in rats after exposure to a cocaine self- administration environment. J Neurosci 20:798–805.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  63. ↵
    1. O'Brien CP
    (2009) Neuroplasticity in addictive disorders. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 11:350–353.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Olive MF
    (2010) Cognitive effects of Group I metabotropic glutamate receptor ligands in the context of drug addiction. Eur J Pharmacol 639:47–58.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Paxinos G,
    2. Watson C
    (2007) The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates (Academic, London), Ed 6.
  66. ↵
    1. Peters J,
    2. LaLumiere RT,
    3. Kalivas PW
    (2008) Infralimbic prefrontal cortex is responsible for inhibiting cocaine seeking in extinguished rats. J Neurosci 28:6046–6053.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  67. ↵
    1. Riedel G,
    2. Platt B,
    3. Micheau J
    (2003) Glutamate receptor function in learning and memory. Behav Brain Res 140:1–47.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. Robinson TE,
    2. Gorny G,
    3. Mitton E,
    4. Kolb B
    (2001) Cocaine self-administration alters the morphology of dendrites and dendritic spines in the nucleus accumbens and neocortex. Synapse 39:257–266.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    1. Rolls ET,
    2. Hornak J,
    3. Wade D,
    4. McGrath J
    (1994) Emotion-related learning in patients with social and emotional changes associated with frontal lobe damage. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 57:1518–1524.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  70. ↵
    1. Schmidt ED,
    2. Voorn P,
    3. Binnekade R,
    4. Schoffelmeer AN,
    5. De Vries TJ
    (2005) Differential involvement of the prelimbic cortex and striatum in conditioned heroin and sucrose seeking following long-term extinction. Eur J Neurosci 22:2347–2356.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Schroeder JP,
    2. Overstreet DH,
    3. Hodge CW
    (2005) The mGluR5 antagonist MPEP decreases operant ethanol self-administration during maintenance and after repeated alcohol deprivations in alcohol-preferring (P) rats. Psychopharmacology 179:262–270.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  72. ↵
    1. Sun H,
    2. Neugebauer V
    (2011) mGluR1, but not mGluR5, activates feed-forward inhibition in the medial prefrontal cortex to impair decision making. J Neurophysiol 106:960–973.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  73. ↵
    1. Sun W,
    2. Rebec GV
    (2006) Repeated cocaine self-administration alters processing of cocaine-related information in rat prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 26:8004–8008.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  74. ↵
    1. Suto N,
    2. Ecke LE,
    3. You ZB,
    4. Wise RA
    (2010) Extracellular fluctuations of dopamine and glutamate in the nucleus accumbens core and shell associated with lever-pressing during cocaine self-administration, extinction, and yoked cocaine administration. Psychopharmacology 211:267–275.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  75. ↵
    1. Swanson CJ,
    2. Kalivas PW
    (2000) Regulation of locomotor activity by metabotropic glutamate receptors in the nucleus accumbens and ventral tegmental area. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 292:406–414.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  76. ↵
    1. Swanson CJ,
    2. Baker DA,
    3. Carson D,
    4. Worley PF,
    5. Kalivas PW
    (2001) Repeated cocaine administration attenuates group I metabotropic glutamate receptor-mediated glutamate release and behavioral activation: a potential role for Homer. J Neurosci 21:9043–9052.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  77. ↵
    1. Szumlinski KK,
    2. Dehoff MH,
    3. Kang SH,
    4. Frys KA,
    5. Lominac KD,
    6. Klugmann M,
    7. Rohrer J,
    8. Griffin W 3rd.,
    9. Toda S,
    10. Champtiaux NP,
    11. Berry T,
    12. Tu JC,
    13. Shealy SE,
    14. During MJ,
    15. Middaugh LD,
    16. Worley PF,
    17. Kalivas PW
    (2004) Homer proteins regulate sensitivity to cocaine. Neuron 43:401–413.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  78. ↵
    1. Tang W,
    2. Wesley M,
    3. Freeman WM,
    4. Liang B,
    5. Hemby SE
    (2004) Alterations in ionotropic glutamate receptor subunits during binge cocaine self-administration and withdrawal in rats. J Neurochem 89:1021–1033.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  79. ↵
    1. Tran-Nguyen LT,
    2. Fuchs RA,
    3. Coffey GP,
    4. Baker DA,
    5. O'Dell LE,
    6. Neisewander JL
    (1998) Time-dependent changes in cocaine-seeking behavior and extracellular dopamine levels in the amygdala during cocaine withdrawal. Neuropsychopharmacology 19:48–59.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    1. Uys JD,
    2. LaLumiere RT
    (2008) Glutamate: the new frontier in pharmacotherapy for cocaine addiction. CNS Neurol Disord Drug Targets 7:482–491.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  81. ↵
    1. Van den Oever MC,
    2. Spijker S,
    3. Smit AB,
    4. De Vries TJ
    (2010) Prefrontal cortex plasticity mechanisms in drug seeking and relapse. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 35:276–284.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  82. ↵
    1. Volkow ND,
    2. Hitzemann R,
    3. Wang GJ,
    4. Fowler JS,
    5. Wolf AP,
    6. Dewey SL,
    7. Handlesman L
    (1992) Long-term frontal brain metabolic changes in cocaine abusers. Synapse 11:184–190.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  83. ↵
    1. Volkow ND,
    2. Fowler JS,
    3. Wang GJ,
    4. Hitzemann R,
    5. Logan J,
    6. Schlyer DJ,
    7. Dewey SL,
    8. Wolf AP
    (1993) Decreased dopamine D2 receptor availability is associated with reduced frontal metabolism in cocaine abusers. Synapse 14:169–177.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  84. ↵
    1. Volkow ND,
    2. Wang GJ,
    3. Fowler JS,
    4. Hitzemann R,
    5. Angrist B,
    6. Gatley SJ,
    7. Logan J,
    8. Ding YS,
    9. Pappas N
    (1999) Association of methylphenidate-induced craving with changes in right striato-orbitofrontal metabolism in cocaine abusers: implications in addiction. Am J Psychiatry 156:19–26.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  85. ↵
    1. Xie X,
    2. Ramirez DR,
    3. Lasseter HC,
    4. Fuchs RA
    (2010) Effects of mGluR1 antagonism in the dorsal hippocampus on drug context-induced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. Psychopharmacology 208:1–11.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  86. ↵
    1. Xie X,
    2. Lasseter HC,
    3. Ramirez DR,
    4. Ponds KL,
    5. Wells AM,
    6. Fuchs RA
    (2012) Subregion-specific role of glutamate receptors in the nucleus accumbens on drug context-induced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. Addict Biol 17:287–299.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  87. ↵
    1. Zavala AR,
    2. Osredkar T,
    3. Joyce JN,
    4. Neisewander JL
    (2008) Upregulation of Arc mRNA expression in the prefrontal cortex following cue-induced reinstatement of extinguished cocaine-seeking behavior. Synapse 62:421–431.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  88. ↵
    1. Zayara AE,
    2. McIver G,
    3. Valdivia PN,
    4. Lominac KD,
    5. McCreary AC,
    6. Szumlinski KK
    (2011) Blockade of nucleus accumbens 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptors prevents the expression of cocaine-induced behavioral and neurochemical sensitization in rats. Psychopharmacology 213:321–335.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 33 (2)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 33, Issue 2
9 Jan 2013
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Deficits in Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Group 1 Metabotropic Glutamate Receptor Function Mediate Resistance to Extinction during Protracted Withdrawal from an Extensive History of Cocaine Self-Administration
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Deficits in Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Group 1 Metabotropic Glutamate Receptor Function Mediate Resistance to Extinction during Protracted Withdrawal from an Extensive History of Cocaine Self-Administration
Osnat Ben-Shahar, Arianne D. Sacramento, Bailey W. Miller, Sierra M. Webb, Melissa G. Wroten, Hannah E. Silva, Amanda L. Caruana, Evan J. Gordon, Kyle L. Ploense, Jennifer Ditzhazy, Tod E. Kippin, Karen K. Szumlinski
Journal of Neuroscience 9 January 2013, 33 (2) 495-506a; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3710-12.2013

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Deficits in Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Group 1 Metabotropic Glutamate Receptor Function Mediate Resistance to Extinction during Protracted Withdrawal from an Extensive History of Cocaine Self-Administration
Osnat Ben-Shahar, Arianne D. Sacramento, Bailey W. Miller, Sierra M. Webb, Melissa G. Wroten, Hannah E. Silva, Amanda L. Caruana, Evan J. Gordon, Kyle L. Ploense, Jennifer Ditzhazy, Tod E. Kippin, Karen K. Szumlinski
Journal of Neuroscience 9 January 2013, 33 (2) 495-506a; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3710-12.2013
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Articles

  • Choice Behavior Guided by Learned, But Not Innate, Taste Aversion Recruits the Orbitofrontal Cortex
  • Maturation of Spontaneous Firing Properties after Hearing Onset in Rat Auditory Nerve Fibers: Spontaneous Rates, Refractoriness, and Interfiber Correlations
  • Insulin Treatment Prevents Neuroinflammation and Neuronal Injury with Restored Neurobehavioral Function in Models of HIV/AIDS Neurodegeneration
Show more Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Disentangling Object Category Representations Driven by Dynamic and Static Visual Input
  • Irrelevant Threats Linger and Affect Behavior in High Anxiety
  • Multisession Anodal Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Enhances Adult Hippocampal Neurogenesis and Context Discrimination in Mice
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.