Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Brief Communications

A Functionally Guided Approach to the Morphometry of Occipitotemporal Regions in Developmental Dyslexia: Evidence for Differential Effects in Boys and Girls

Irene Altarelli, Karla Monzalvo, Stéphanie Iannuzzi, Joel Fluss, Catherine Billard, Franck Ramus and Ghislaine Dehaene-Lambertz
Journal of Neuroscience 3 July 2013, 33 (27) 11296-11301; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5854-12.2013
Irene Altarelli
1Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et Psycholinguistique, Département d'Etudes Cognitives, Ecole Normale Supérieure, EHESS, CNRS, 75230 Paris Cedex 05, France,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Karla Monzalvo
2INSERM, U992, 91190 Gif-sur-yvette, France,
3Commissariat à l'Energie Atomique, Division of Life Sciences, Institute of BioImaging, Neurospin, 91190 Gif-sur-yvette, France,
4University Paris-Sud, 91400 Orsay, France,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Stéphanie Iannuzzi
1Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et Psycholinguistique, Département d'Etudes Cognitives, Ecole Normale Supérieure, EHESS, CNRS, 75230 Paris Cedex 05, France,
5Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris, Le Kremlin-Bicêtre, 94275 Paris, France, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Joel Fluss
5Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris, Le Kremlin-Bicêtre, 94275 Paris, France, and
6Neurologie pédiatrique, Hôpitaux Universitaires Genève, 41205 Genève, Suisse
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Catherine Billard
5Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris, Le Kremlin-Bicêtre, 94275 Paris, France, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Franck Ramus
1Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et Psycholinguistique, Département d'Etudes Cognitives, Ecole Normale Supérieure, EHESS, CNRS, 75230 Paris Cedex 05, France,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Ghislaine Dehaene-Lambertz
2INSERM, U992, 91190 Gif-sur-yvette, France,
3Commissariat à l'Energie Atomique, Division of Life Sciences, Institute of BioImaging, Neurospin, 91190 Gif-sur-yvette, France,
4University Paris-Sud, 91400 Orsay, France,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Developmental dyslexia is a learning disability that specifically affects reading acquisition. Cortical anomalies and gray matter volume differences in various temporal regions have been reported in dyslexic subjects compared with controls. However, consistency between studies is lacking. In the present experiments, we focused our structural analyses on the ventral occipitotemporal regions, defined by their functional response to visual categories. We applied a subject-by-subject functionally guided approach on a total of 76 participants (31 dyslexic children). Cortical thickness was estimated for each participant around his/her peak of specific functional activation to visual words, faces, or places. Results from two independent datasets showed a reduction in thickness in dyslexic children compared with controls in the region responsive to words, in the left hemisphere. Additionally, a gender-by-diagnosis interaction was observed at the same location, due to differences in girls only. To avoid the potential confound of reading level, we also contrasted dyslexic and control children matched for reading performance, and we observed a similar difference, although in a smaller extent of cortex. The present study thus provides the first account of a focal cortical thickness reduction in dyslexia in the subregion of ventral occipitotemporal cortex specifically responsive to visual words, when age, gender, and reading performance are taken into account.

Introduction

Dyslexia is a neurodevelopmental disorder under genetic influence, affecting 3–7% of school-age population. It is characterized by a severe difficulty in reading acquisition despite normal intelligence, education, and sensory functions. Understanding the links between genetic variations, brain disruptions, and specific cognitive impairments remains an important challenge for research on dyslexia (Giraud and Ramus, 2013). To this end, finely characterizing potential neuroanatomical markers of the disorder appears essential.

From a functional point of view, a recent meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies (Richlan et al., 2011) revealed that the most consistent hypo-activation in dyslexic children is found in the left occipitotemporal region, which is thought to play a key role in visual word form recognition and processing (Dehaene and Cohen, 2011; Price and Devlin, 2011). This finding is congruent with experiments suggesting an early failure to engage this system in children with dyslexia (Maurer et al., 2007), or in kindergartners bearing a genetic predisposition for the disorder (Raschle et al., 2012).

At the structural level, following the seminal postmortem work by Galaburda et al. (1985), many attempts have been made to describe the brain of dyslexic patients in vivo by MRI. Gray matter volume reductions have been reported in several cerebral regions, comprising temporoparietal and left occipitotemporal areas (Richardson and Price, 2009), in dyslexic adults, children, and at-risk prereaders (Raschle et al., 2011). However, complete consistency between studies is lacking, possibly due to small samples of subjects differing in factors such as age, gender, and severity of the disorder.

In the present experiments, we aimed at characterizing the structure of ventral occipitotemporal regions in developmental dyslexia with refined anatomical detail. Congruently with the functional literature, our prediction was that, instead of a widespread defect, only the region responsive to written words would structurally differ in dyslexic children compared with controls. We applied a subject-by-subject region-of-interest approach, restricting our analysis to functionally defined territories, where we estimated cortical thickness. We analyzed data from two independent datasets, differing in age and imaging characteristics, to exclusively report replicated results. Finally, a major issue being the potential confound between effects of developmental dyslexia per se versus effects of reading experience, we further tested the obtained results in a comparison between dyslexic participants and control children matched for reading performance.

Materials and Methods

Participants.

Forty-five control (25 boys) and 31 dyslexic children (17 boys) participated in our experiments. No child with a history of neurological or psychiatric disorder, with a hearing or uncorrected visual deficit was included. Nonverbal IQ was in the normal range (standard scores in the block design subtest from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children or WISC III or IV ≥7 or overall nonverbal IQ ≥80; Wechsler, 2005). Children and their parents gave written informed consent; all experimental procedures received approval from the local ethics committee.

In Study 1, dyslexic participants (mean age, 11 years 9 months) were matched with controls for age and gender. In Study 2, dyslexic children (mean age, 9 years 10 months) were paired with two distinct control groups, one matched for age, gender, and parental education (age-matched group), and the other for gender and reading level (reading-matched group; mean age, 6 years 8 months). The dyslexic and age-matched control children of Study 2 were selected from the study of Monzalvo et al. (2012) so as to be paired one-by-one for age and gender. All dyslexic participants were previously diagnosed by a dedicated learning disability center (Dr. Billard, CHU Bicêtre). Parental education was determined through a sociodemographic questionnaire; only maternal education was available in Study 2.

Behavioral tests.

A battery of behavioral tests was administered to determine participants' intellectual, verbal, and reading abilities. Children's verbal skills and working memory were evaluated through WISC similarities and digit span subtests, respectively. Reading level was assessed by the standardized French test “L'alouette” (Lefavrais, 1967) and dyslexic children were expected to present a delay ≥18 months. Phonological skills (using a phoneme deletion task; Sprenger-Charolles et al., 2005) and rapid automatized naming of pictures (Plaza and Robert-Jahier, 2006) were also assessed. All tests are age-standardized, except for phoneme deletion and rapid automatized naming tasks.

MRI procedure.

All children underwent a 3-T MRI exam (Siemens Tim Trio), comprising the acquisition of both functional and structural images. The functional paradigm aimed at describing category-specific visual areas in ventral occipitotemporal cortices. A revolving checkerboard, houses, faces, words, and also tools in the case of Study 1 (30 black and white pictures in each category and 30 frequent regular words) were presented in a block paradigm (Monzalvo et al., 2012). No explicit reading of the words was requested. The child was instructed to press a button as soon as he/she detected a star (two of them being randomly presented in each block), to keep his/her attention focused on the visual stimuli.

Whole-brain images were acquired using a 32-channel head coil in Study 1 (T1 sequence parameters: acquisition matrix = 230 × 230 × 224, TR = 2300 ms, TE = 3.05 ms, flip angle = 9 deg, FOV = 230 mm, voxel size = 0.9 × 0.9 × 0.9 mm) and a 12-channel head coil in Study 2 (T1 sequence parameters: acquisition matrix = 256 × 256 × 176, TR = 2300 ms, TE = 4.18 ms, flip angle = 9 deg, FOV = 256 mm, voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm). For functional imaging, the sequence parameters were the same in both studies (TR = 2400 ms, TE = 30 ms, matrix = 64 × 64 × 40, voxel size = 3 × 3 × 3 mm) with 108 volumes acquired in one run in Study 1, and four runs of 84 volumes each in Study 2.

fMRI analyses.

All preprocessing and analyses of functional data were performed in SPM5. Images were aligned to the first functional image, coregistered with the individual anatomical image, normalized to the MNI adult brain, and smoothed (5 mm Gaussian kernel). fMRI data modeling used the canonical SPM hemodynamic response function and its time derivative, convolved with the experimental conditions (corresponding to each visual category presented). The 6 movement parameters were entered as regressor of no interest. No group differences in movement were found, in either study (for mean translation and rotation respectively, Study 1: t = −1.3 p > 0.2, t = −1.1 p > 0.2 Study 2: age-matched t = −0.3 p > 0.7, t = −0.6 p > 0.5, reading-matched t = 1.2 p > 0.2, t = 1.5 p > 0.1). For each subject and each visual category, the peak location was determined as the voxel of maximal t value within a sphere of 10 mm centered on a reference peak [in MNI, left: words (−42 −48 −15), faces (−39 −54 −16.5), houses (−27 −48 −9); right: faces(39 −49.5 −18), houses (30 −46.5 −9); Fig. 1a] in the t-map corresponding to the contrast of interest [i.e., (words > others), (faces > others), (houses > others)]. The reference peaks were the peak specific responses for each category reported in Monzalvo et al. (2012), computed across 46 dyslexic and control children. In that study, there was no significant specific response for words in the right hemisphere.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

a, Spherical search space around the group reference peak for each category. b, Each subjects' response peak to words and faces is projected on the left ventral surface of an inflated template brain, for all participants in Study 1.

Cortical thickness estimation.

A surface-based cortical reconstruction was applied to all subjects, using Freesurfer (Dale et al., 1999). This software reconstructs cortical surfaces, registers them to a common surface template and estimates cortical thickness (Fischl and Dale, 2000).

For each subject, each category peak was localized on the reconstructed cortical surface, thanks to the Talairach transformation estimated by Freesurfer (the dispersion of those peaks across subjects in Study 1 is represented in Fig. 1b). By successive dilatations around those peaks, disks of ∼4 and 10 mm radius were built on the cortical surface. Mean cortical thickness was estimated within these disks, in each subject's own native space.

Statistical analyses.

Statistical analyses were conducted in SPSS (version 8, SPSS). Demographic differences between groups were tested through independent sample t tests. Analyses of covariance were first applied to behavioral measures, with diagnosis and gender as between-subject factors and parental education and age (except for the reading-matched comparison in Study 2) as covariates.

Regarding cortical thickness, mixed analyses of covariance were run, with region of interest (left-hemisphere words, faces, houses; right-hemisphere faces, houses) as within-subject factor, diagnosis and gender as between-subject factors and parental education, age, and mean hemispheric thickness as covariates. Significant interactions were further investigated by separate between-subject analyses of covariance for each region, with the same factors and covariates. Effect sizes were calculated using Cohen's d formula.

To test the possibility that the location of peak coordinates or its variability might differ between groups, we ran multivariate analyses of variance and Box's M tests, entering the Talairach x, y, and z coordinates for each category peak as dependent variables and diagnosis and gender as between-subject factors.

Results

Demographic and behavioral results

Demographic characteristics are reported in Table 1. As for behavioral tests (Table 2), no differences were found in nonverbal abilities between dyslexic and control children, in any study. In most other tests, dyslexic participants performed significantly worse than their age-matched peers. When paired with children of the same reading level, only working memory (assessed by the age-standardized test of digit span) remained significantly lower in dyslexic children.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Group composition in Studies 1 and 2

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Behavioral scores for control and dyslexic children

No consistent effect of gender across studies and no diagnosis by gender interaction was identified for any behavioral measure.

Peak response locations

Individual peak locations for each visual category did not differ between dyslexic and control children, as no effect of diagnosis on the combined x, y, z Talairach coordinates was found in either study. Moreover, the peaks were not more widely dispersed among dyslexic participants than among controls, as Box's M tests were not significant. Similarly, gender showed no effect on peak location and dispersion.

Group differences in cortical thickness

No difference in mean hemispheric thickness was observed between dyslexic children and their age-matched controls, in Studies 1 and 2, whereas reading-matched (and therefore younger) controls presented significantly greater left mean hemispheric thickness than dyslexic individuals (t(24) = 2.2, p = 0.04). This result is consistent with previously documented age trends (Sowell et al., 2004). As mentioned earlier, mean hemispheric thickness was included as a covariate in all analyses.

In Study 1, a significant diagnosis*gender*region interaction was found (F(4,112) = 3, p = 0.02). Analyses computed on the individual regions revealed a thicker cortex in controls relative to dyslexic children around the specific peak for words in the left hemisphere (F(1,29) = 4.45, p = 0.04, d = 0.5) and a diagnosis by gender interaction around that same peak (F(1,29) = 6.59, p = 0.02): girls, but not boys, presented significantly thicker cortices in the control than in the dyslexic group (girls F(1,10) = 30.65, p < 0.001, d = 2, boys F(1,15) < 1), as displayed in Figure 2a. Crucially, no main effect of diagnosis was observed around the specific peaks to faces and houses, in either hemisphere. A gender by diagnosis interaction was however significant around the left-hemisphere response peak to houses (F(1,28) = 8.66, p = 0.007) driven by opposite marginal effects in girls and boys (girls, F(1,10) = 3.78, p = 0.08, d = 1; boys, F(1,14) = 2.40, p = 0.14, d = 1.2).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Boxplots for left hemisphere cortical thickness around the response peak to the contrast (words > others). a, Study 1, 11-year-old age-matched groups. b, Study 2, 9-year-old age-matched groups. c, Study 2, reading-matched groups. Plots a and b are based on disks of 10 mm radius, whereas c is based on a 4 mm radius.

In Study 2, a significant diagnosis*gender*region interaction (F(4,76) = 3.7, p = 0.008) for the age-matched comparison was also observed. Controls showed thicker cortices than dyslexic children around the specific peak for words (F(1,19) = 9.68, p = 0.006 d = 1) and the diagnosis by gender interaction (F(1,19) = 6.65, p = 0.018) was again explained by a difference in cortical thickness between control and dyslexic girls (F(1,7) = 14.47, p = 0.007, d = 2), but not boys (F(1,9) < 1), as shown in Figure 2b. However, the diagnosis by gender interaction around the peak of response to (houses > others), reported in Study 1, did not reach significance (F(1,19) = 3.51, p = 0.08). No other main effect of diagnosis or interaction was observed in either hemisphere.

Finally, we compared reading-matched controls to dyslexic children in the region found to be different between age-matched subjects in both Studies 1 and 2, i.e., the response peak to (words > others). The effect of diagnosis did not reach significance (F(1,20) = 1.71, p = 0.2), nor did the diagnosis by gender interaction (F(1,20) = 2.27, p = 0.15). However, when estimating cortical thickness within a disc of smaller radius (∼4 mm), both the effect of diagnosis (F(1,20) = 5.45, p = 0.03, d = 0.5) and the diagnosis by gender interaction became significant (F(1,20) = 6.49, p = 0.02). Control girls, but not boys, showed significantly thicker cortices than dyslexic girls in this specific region (girls, F(1,8) = 7.82, p = 0.02, d = 1.4; boys, F(1,10) < 1), as can be seen in Figure 2c. For consistency, we retested the comparison between the age-matched groups using a 4 mm radius disc, and we found that in Study 1 the main effect of diagnosis (F(1,29) = 2.26, p = 0.1, d = 0.4) and the diagnosis by gender interaction (F(1,29) = 3.4, p = 0.08) were marginally significant, whereas they were significant in Study 2 (respectively, F(1,19) = 9.8, p = 0.006, d = 1; F(1,19) = 6.15, p = 0.02).

Discussion

In the present experiments, we analyzed the cortical thickness of left and right ventral occipitotemporal regions. We refined traditional analyses by applying a subject-by-subject functionally based approach and measured the structural properties of individually defined functional territories. We found a cortical thickness reduction in dyslexic children, specifically located around the response peak to words in the left hemisphere; furthermore, the global group difference was carried exclusively by girls. These results were replicated in two independent datasets of dyslexic and control children, whereas no consistent difference was observed around the functional peaks of other visual categories (houses and faces).

A reduction of gray matter tissue across occipitotemporal regions in dyslexia has been inconsistently reported. Although increased gray matter volume was observed in the left fusiform gyrus of dyslexic subjects by Silani et al. (2005), the opposite effect was subsequently described bilaterally (Kronbichler et al., 2008; Brambati et al., 2004). Recent meta-analyses of voxel-based morphometry (VBM) studies have not managed to resolve these inconsistencies (Linkersdörfer et al., 2012; Richlan et al., 2013). However, methodological approaches like VBM, which are relatively insensitive to individual anatomical characteristics, might miss the type of subtle differences reported in the present study. This might be particularly relevant for ventral occipitotemporal regions, given their complexity in terms of anatomical landmarks (Frost and Goebel, 2012) and cytoarchitecture (Caspers et al., 2013). Moreover, Frost and Goebel (2012) reported that functional regions in the fusiform gyrus can be quite variable across subjects in relation to anatomical landmarks, even after accurate alignment. As can be seen in Figure 1b, the dispersion of our functional peaks was indeed not negligible. The method used in the present study is thus likely to be more effective than previous investigations in detecting subtle differences between populations.

Cortical thickness decreases with maturation across most of the brain, probably due to an increase of the myelin sheet in lower cortical layers (Sowell et al., 2004). Variations in thickness can also be associated with the density of elements in perineuronal space, among which glial cells and mainly dendrites, whose arborization follows synaptogenesis and pruning cycles (Jiang et al., 2009). Either of these aspects could underlie the observed results in dyslexic participants.

Interestingly, postmortem studies have revealed ectopias, dysplasias, and heterotopias in the brains of dyslexic patients, and susceptibility genes for the disorder appear to be involved in neuronal migration (Poelmans et al., 2011). These lines of evidence have led to the hypothesis of a neuronal migration disruption in dyslexia (Galaburda et al., 1985). In this perspective, the reported cortical thickness differences between dyslexic and control children could be related to a thinner cortical plate in the former; this could either be due to insufficient numbers of cells in place in the cortex, or to a reduction in synaptic connections, because of cells ending up in abnormal positions.

The area surrounding the visual word form area, which develops a specific functional response to words in literates (Dehaene et al., 2010) and whose activity is found reduced in dyslexics (Shaywitz et al., 2002; Monzalvo et al., 2012), is a good candidate for functional and structural plasticity consecutive to reading acquisition. Nevertheless, the fact that Raschle et al. (2011) found gray matter volume reductions in at-risk prereaders in the left hemisphere, in proximity to our own reference peak for words, is an argument against gray matter differences depending exclusively on differential reading experience. Furthermore, hypo-activations in this region have also been observed, even in reading-matched comparisons (Hoeft et al., 2007). In the present study, we replicated cortical thickness differences between control and dyslexic participants when equalizing reading skills (Fig. 2c), although within a smaller patch of cortex (4 vs 10 mm radius) compared with age-matched comparisons. The absence of a clear group difference within a 10 mm sphere weakens the reading age group comparison, and would make it desirable to replicate this result. Nevertheless, the reduction of the area of interest in this second comparison, involving younger controls, is consistent with the idea of an expansion of visual category-specific regions in the course of development and/or expertise acquisition (Golarai et al., 2007; Dehaene et al., 2010), with less expert readers showing a smaller region specialized for visual words and thus a smaller region of increased thickness due to reading expertise. Therefore, although requiring cautious interpretation, our reading-matched comparison suggests that there might be a primary structural defect in dyslexic children precisely in the left occipitotemporal subregion that eventually becomes the visual word form area.

Although a group difference around the response peak to words was expected, the diagnosis by gender interaction was more surprising. A few studies are however consistent with the idea that the biological basis of dyslexia might to some extent differ between boys and girls. Humphreys et al. (1990) described the presence of myelinated glial scars, rather than ectopias, in the brains of three women with dyslexia. Very few structural MRI studies have included balanced numbers of male and female dyslexic participants, partly because of the known sex ratio in dyslexia (Flannery et al., 2000) making it more difficult to recruit affected females. Thus, very little is known about possible patterns of structural variation between genders. Among the few exceptions, Sandu et al. (2008) showed that dyslexic girls differed from control girls in global hemispheric measures, whereas no such difference was observed in boys. The findings of both Sandu et al. (2008) and the present study could be interpreted as the presence of a more severe cerebral phenotype in dyslexic girls compared with boys. This could result either from an entirely distinct etiology in girls and in boys, or from the impact of sex-related variations on a shared primary cause. Studies assessing heritability estimates of reading difficulties in males and females have been inconclusive, finding either higher heritability in males (Harlaar et al., 2005) or no difference between sexes (Hawke et al., 2006), thus providing little evidence for a distinct genetic etiology. Some steroid hormones (McCarthy, 2009), as well as other potential nonhormonal factors (Zhang et al., 2003), might give females greater resilience to brain insult across life (Rosen et al., 1999; Ramus, 2006). Trajectories of brain development appear to peak earlier in females than in males (Lenroot et al., 2007), and brains at different developmental stages are not equally vulnerable. All these factors could conceivably contribute to a greater functional resilience of females to structural brain disruptions, thus requiring a more severe neural phenotype to produce similar cognitive impairments.

Future research should aim to clarify the interaction between predispositions to dyslexia and gender on the associated brain phenotypes. At the very least it now seems crucial for any brain imaging study of dyslexia to take gender differences into account.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by Ecole des Neurosciences de Paris, Agence Nationale de la Recherche (Grants Nos. ANR-06-NEURO-019-01, ANR-11-BSV4-014-01, ANR-11-0001-02 PSL*, and ANR-10-LABX-0087), European Commission (Grant No. LSHM-CT-2005-018696), University Paris-Sud (BQR), CNRS, INSERM, and the Bettencourt-Schueller Foundation. We thank Rémi Lebigre for his help with image preprocessing. We also like to thank Nadège Villiermet, Camille Chabernaud, Laure Bricout, and the clinical and technical staff at Bicêtre Hospital and at Neurospin for their contribution to children testing in a welcoming environment, and all children and families for their participation.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Irene Altarelli, Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et Psycholinguistique, Département d'Etudes Cognitives, Ecole Normale Supérieure, 29 Rue d'Ulm, 75230 Paris Cedex 05, France. irene.altarelli{at}ens.fr

References

  1. ↵
    1. Brambati SM,
    2. Termine C,
    3. Ruffino M,
    4. Stella G,
    5. Fazio F,
    6. Cappa SF,
    7. Perani D
    (2004) Regional reductions of grey matter volume in familial dyslexia. Neurology 63:742–745, pmid:15326259.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    1. Caspers J,
    2. Zilles K,
    3. Eickhoff SB,
    4. Schleicher A,
    5. Mohlberg H,
    6. Amunts K
    (2013) Cytoarchitectonical analysis and probabilistic mapping of two extrastriate areas of the human posterior fusiform gyrus. Brain Struct Funct 218:511–526, doi:10.1007/s00429-012-0411-8, pmid:22488096.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Dale AM,
    2. Fischl B,
    3. Sereno MI
    (1999) Cortical surface-based analysis: I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 9:179–194, doi:10.1006/nimg.1998.0395, pmid:9931268.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Dehaene S,
    2. Cohen L
    (2011) The unique role of the visual word form area in reading. Trends Cogn Sci 15:254–262, doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.003, pmid:21592844.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Dehaene S,
    2. Pegado F,
    3. Braga LW,
    4. Ventura P,
    5. Nunes Filho G,
    6. Jobert A,
    7. Dehaene-Lambertz G,
    8. Kolinsky R,
    9. Morais J,
    10. Cohen L
    (2010) How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science 330:1359–1364, doi:10.1126/science.1194140, pmid:21071632.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    1. Fischl B,
    2. Dale AM
    (2000) Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97:11050–11055, doi:10.1073/pnas.200033797, pmid:10984517.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    1. Flannery KA,
    2. Liederman J,
    3. Daly L,
    4. Schultz J
    (2000) Male prevalence for reading disability is found in a large sample of black and white children free from ascertainment bias. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 6:433–442, pmid:10902412.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Frost MA,
    2. Goebel R
    (2012) Measuring structural-functional correspondence: spatial variability of specialised brain regions after macro-anatomical alignment. Neuroimage 59:1369–1381, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.035, pmid:21875671.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Galaburda AM,
    2. Sherman GF,
    3. Rosen GD,
    4. Aboitiz F,
    5. Geschwind N
    (1985) Developmental dyslexia: four consecutive patients with cortical anomalies. Ann Neurol 18:222–233, doi:10.1002/ana.410180210, pmid:4037763.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Giraud AL,
    2. Ramus F
    (2013) Neurogenetics and auditory processing in developmental dyslexia. Curr Opin Neurobiol 23:37–42, doi:10.1016/j.conb.2012.09.003, pmid:23040541.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Golarai G,
    2. Ghahremani DG,
    3. Whitfield-Gabrieli S,
    4. Reiss A,
    5. Eberhardt JL,
    6. Gabrieli JD,
    7. Grill-Spector K
    (2007) Differential development of high-level visual cortex correlates with category-specific recognition memory. Nat Neurosci 10:512–522, doi:10.1038/nn1865, pmid:17351637.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Harlaar N,
    2. Spinath FM,
    3. Dale PS,
    4. Plomin R
    (2005) Genetic influences on early word recognition abilities and disabilities: a study of 7-year-old twins. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 46:373–384, doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00358.x, pmid:15819646.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Hawke JL,
    2. Wadsworth SJ,
    3. DeFries JC
    (2006) Genetic influences on reading difficulties in boys and girls: the Colorado twin study. Dyslexia 12:21–29, doi:10.1002/dys.301, pmid:16512171.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Hoeft F,
    2. Meyler A,
    3. Hernandez A,
    4. Juel C,
    5. Taylor-Hill H,
    6. Martindale JL,
    7. McMillon G,
    8. Kolchugina G,
    9. Black JM,
    10. Faizi A,
    11. Deutsch GK,
    12. Siok WT,
    13. Reiss AL,
    14. Whitfield-Gabrieli S,
    15. Gabrieli JD
    (2007) Functional and morphometric brain dissociation between dyslexia and reading ability. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104:4234–4239, doi:10.1073/pnas.0609399104, pmid:17360506.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Humphreys P,
    2. Kaufmann WE,
    3. Galaburda AM
    (1990) Developmental dyslexia in women: neuropathological findings in three patients. Ann Neurol 28:727–738, doi:10.1002/ana.410280602, pmid:2285260.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Jiang J,
    2. Zhu W,
    3. Shi F,
    4. Liu Y,
    5. Li J,
    6. Qin W,
    7. Li K,
    8. Yu C,
    9. Jiang T
    (2009) Thick visual cortex in the early blind. J Neurosci 29:2205–2211, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5451-08.2009, pmid:19228973.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    1. Kronbichler M,
    2. Wimmer H,
    3. Staffen W,
    4. Hutzler F,
    5. Mair A,
    6. Ladurner G
    (2008) Developmental dyslexia: gray matter abnormalities in the occipitotemporal cortex. Hum Brain Mapp 29:613–625, doi:10.1002/hbm.20425, pmid:17636558.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Lefavrais P
    (1967) Test de l'alouette: manuel (les éditions du centre de psychologie appliquée, Paris).
  19. ↵
    1. Lenroot RK,
    2. Gogtay N,
    3. Greenstein DK,
    4. Molloy Wells EM,
    5. Wallace GL,
    6. Clasen LS,
    7. Blumenthal JD,
    8. Lerch J,
    9. Zijdenbos AP,
    10. Evans AC,
    11. Thompson PM,
    12. Giedd JN
    (2007) Sexual dimorphism on brain developmental trajectories during childhood and adolescence. Neuroimage 36:1065–1073, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.053, pmid:17513132.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Linkersdörfer J,
    2. Lonnemann J,
    3. Lindberg S,
    4. Hasselhorn M,
    5. Fiebach CJ
    (2012) Grey matter alterations colocalize with functional abnormalities in developmental dyslexia: an ALE meta-analysis. PloS one 7:e43122, doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0043122, pmid:22916214.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Maurer U,
    2. Brem S,
    3. Bucher K,
    4. Kranz F,
    5. Benz R,
    6. Steinhausen HC,
    7. Brandeis D
    (2007) Impaired tuning of a fast occipitotemporal response for print in dyslexic children learning to read. Brain 130:3200–3210, doi:10.1093/brain/awm193, pmid:17728359.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    1. McCarthy MM
    (2009) The two faces of estradiol: effects on the developing brain. Neuroscientist 15:599–610, doi:10.1177/1073858409340924, pmid:19700741.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  23. ↵
    1. Monzalvo K,
    2. Fluss J,
    3. Billard C,
    4. Dehaene S,
    5. Dehaene-Lambertz G
    (2012) Cortical networks for vision and language in dyslexic and normal children of variable socio-economic status. Neuroimage 61:258–274, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.035, pmid:22387166.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Plaza M,
    2. Robert-Jahier AM
    (2006) DRA enfants test de dénomination rapide automatisé (Adeprio, Paris).
  25. ↵
    1. Poelmans G,
    2. Buitelaar JK,
    3. Pauls DL,
    4. Franke B
    (2011) A theoretical molecular network for dyslexia: integrating available genetic findings. Mol Psychiatry 16:365–382, doi:10.1038/mp.2010.105, pmid:20956978.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Price CJ,
    2. Devlin JT
    (2011) The interactive account of ventral occipitotemporal contributions to reading. Trends Cogn Sci 15:246–253, doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.001, pmid:21549634.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Ramus F
    (2006) in The dyslexic brain: new pathways in neuroscience discovery, A neurological model of dyslexia and other domain-specific developmental disorders with an associated sensorimotor syndrome, ed Rosen GD (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ), pp 75–101.
  28. ↵
    1. Raschle NM,
    2. Chang M,
    3. Gaab N
    (2011) Structural brain alterations associated with dyslexia predate reading onset. Neuroimage 57:742–749, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.055, pmid:20884362.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Raschle NM,
    2. Zuk J,
    3. Gaab N
    (2012) Functional characteristics of developmental dyslexia in left-hemispheric posterior brain regions predate reading onset. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109:2156–2161, doi:10.1073/pnas.1107721109, pmid:22308323.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  30. ↵
    1. Richardson FM,
    2. Price CJ
    (2009) Structural MRI studies of language function in the undamaged brain. Brain Struct Funct 213:511–523, doi:10.1007/s00429-009-0211-y, pmid:19618210.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Richlan F,
    2. Kronbichler M,
    3. Wimmer H
    (2011) Meta-analyzing brain dysfunctions in dyslexic children and adults. Neuroimage 56:1735–1742, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.040, pmid:21338695.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Richlan F,
    2. Kronbichler M,
    3. Wimmer H
    (2013) Structural abnormalities in the dyslexic brain: a meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. Hum Brain Mapp doi:10.1002/hbm.22127, pmid:22711189, in press.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Rosen GD,
    2. Herman AE,
    3. Galaburda AM
    (1999) Sex differences in the effects of early neocortical injury on neuronal size distribution of the medial geniculate nucleus in the rat are mediated by perinatal gonadal steroids. Cereb Cortex 9:27–34, doi:10.1093/cercor/9.1.27, pmid:10022493.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    1. Sandu AL,
    2. Specht K,
    3. Beneventi H,
    4. Lundervold A,
    5. Hugdahl K
    (2008) Sex-differences in grey-white matter structure in normal-reading and dyslexic adolescents. Neurosci Lett 438:80–84, doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2008.04.022, pmid:18456405.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Shaywitz BA,
    2. Shaywitz SE,
    3. Pugh KR,
    4. Mencl WE,
    5. Fulbright RK,
    6. Skudlarski P,
    7. Constable RT,
    8. Marchione KE,
    9. Fletcher JM,
    10. Lyon GR,
    11. Gore JC
    (2002) Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia. Biol psychiatry 52:101–110, doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01365-3, pmid:12114001.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Silani G,
    2. Frith U,
    3. Demonet JF,
    4. Fazio F,
    5. Perani D,
    6. Price C,
    7. Frith CD,
    8. Paulesu E
    (2005) Brain abnormalities underlying altered activation in dyslexia: a voxel based morphometry study. Brain 128:2453–2461, doi:10.1093/brain/awh579, pmid:15975942.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    1. Sowell ER,
    2. Thompson PM,
    3. Leonard CM,
    4. Welcome SE,
    5. Kan E,
    6. Toga AW
    (2004) Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. J Neurosci 24:8223–8231, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1798-04.2004, pmid:15385605.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  38. ↵
    1. Sprenger-Charolles L,
    2. Colé P,
    3. Béchennec D,
    4. Kipffer-Piquard A
    (2005) French normative data on reading and related skills from EVALEC, a new computerized battery of tests. Eur Rev Appl Psychol 55:157–186, doi:10.1016/j.erap.2004.11.002.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  39. ↵
    1. Wechsler D
    (2005) WISC-IV: echelle d'Intelligence de Wechsler pour enfants-quatrième edition (Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée, Paris).
  40. ↵
    1. Zhang L,
    2. Li PP,
    3. Feng X,
    4. Barker JL,
    5. Smith SV,
    6. Rubinow DR
    (2003) Sex-related differences in neuronal cell survival and signaling in rats. Neurosci Lett 337:65–68, doi:10.1016/s0304-3940(02)01179-5, pmid:12527389.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 33 (27)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 33, Issue 27
3 Jul 2013
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
A Functionally Guided Approach to the Morphometry of Occipitotemporal Regions in Developmental Dyslexia: Evidence for Differential Effects in Boys and Girls
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
A Functionally Guided Approach to the Morphometry of Occipitotemporal Regions in Developmental Dyslexia: Evidence for Differential Effects in Boys and Girls
Irene Altarelli, Karla Monzalvo, Stéphanie Iannuzzi, Joel Fluss, Catherine Billard, Franck Ramus, Ghislaine Dehaene-Lambertz
Journal of Neuroscience 3 July 2013, 33 (27) 11296-11301; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5854-12.2013

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
A Functionally Guided Approach to the Morphometry of Occipitotemporal Regions in Developmental Dyslexia: Evidence for Differential Effects in Boys and Girls
Irene Altarelli, Karla Monzalvo, Stéphanie Iannuzzi, Joel Fluss, Catherine Billard, Franck Ramus, Ghislaine Dehaene-Lambertz
Journal of Neuroscience 3 July 2013, 33 (27) 11296-11301; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5854-12.2013
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Heteromodal Cortical Areas Encode Sensory-Motor Features of Word Meaning
  • Pharmacologically Counteracting a Phenotypic Difference in Cerebellar GABAA Receptor Response to Alcohol Prevents Excessive Alcohol Consumption in a High Alcohol-Consuming Rodent Genotype
  • Neuromuscular NMDA Receptors Modulate Developmental Synapse Elimination
Show more Brief Communications
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.