Abstract
Expression of dynorphin, an endogenous opioid peptide, increases with age and has been associated with memory impairments in rats. In human, prodynorphin (Pdyn) gene polymorphisms might be linked to cognitive function in the elderly. Moreover, elevated dynorphin levels have been reported in postmortem samples from Alzheimer's disease patients. However, the cellular and molecular processes affected by higher dynorphin levels during aging remain unknown. Using Pdyn−/− mice, we observed significant changes in the function and expression of Group 1 metabotropic glutamate receptor (mGluR). Compared with age-matched wild-type (WT) littermates, we found increased expression of mGluR1α and mGluR5 in the hippocampus and cortex of old, but not young, Pdyn−/− mice. Increased Group 1 mGluR expression in aged Pdyn−/− mice was associated with enhanced mGluR-mediated long-term depression, a form of synaptic plasticity. Notably, whereas aged WT mice developed spatial and recognition memory deficits, aged Pdyn−/− mice performed similarly as young mice. Pharmacological treatments with 3-cyano-N-(1,3-diphenyl-1H-pyrazol-5-yl)benzamide, a positive modulator of mGlu5 receptors, or norbinaltorphimine, an antagonist for dynorphin-targeted κ-opioid receptor, rescued memory in old WT mice. Conversely, mGlu5 receptor antagonist 2-methyl-6-(phenylethynyl)pyridine hydrochloride impaired spatial memory of old Pdyn−/− mice. Intact cognition in aged Pdyn−/− mice paralleled with increased expression of Group 1 mGluR-related genes Homer 1a and Arc. Finally, aged Pdyn−/− mice displayed less anxiety-related behaviors than age-matched WT mice. Together, our results suggest that elevated Pdyn expression during normal aging reduces mGluR expression and signaling, which in turn impairs cognitive functions and increases anxiety.
Introduction
Aging is generally characterized by reduced cognitive abilities and heightened anxiety-related behaviors (Bedrosian et al., 2011). Increasing findings suggest that age-related cognitive and emotional dysfunctions are related to rising dynorphins in the aged brain (Yakovleva et al., 2007; Kolsch et al., 2009). Dynorphins, a class of centrally expressed opioids peptides that are encoded by the prodynorphin (Pdyn) gene, have been related to learning and memory processes, emotional control, and stress response (Schwarzer, 2009). Expression of dynorphins increases with age, and this upregulation has been associated with memory impairments in rats (Jiang et al., 1989; Kotz et al., 2004). In human, Pdyn gene polymorphisms might be linked with episodic memory deficits in the elderly (Kolsch et al., 2009) and elevated dynorphin expression could be involved in the pathogenesis of Alzheimer's disease (Yakovleva et al., 2007).
Maintenance of intact cognition during aging could require an adaptation of synaptic plasticity processes, which underlie memory formation. Acquisition and consolidation of spatial memory depend partly on Group 1 metabotropic glutamate receptor (mGluR) function in aged rats (Lee et al., 2005; Ménard and Quirion, 2012b). These receptors are enriched in the hippocampus, a brain area involved in cognition and stress responsivity (Squire, 1992; McEwen, 1999). Activation of Group 1 mGluR leads to long-term depression (LTD), a form of synaptic plasticity involved in goal directed behavior, novelty detection, and recognition memory formation (Luscher and Huber, 2010). LTD is crucial for developing effective neuronal network connections and facilitating spatial cognitive map generation (Kemp and Manahan-Vaughan, 2007; Malleret et al., 2010). Immediate early gene (IEG) translation is induced by mGluR activation and necessary for memory consolidation (Ménard and Quirion, 2012a). Gene expression has been targeted to explain memory deficits related to aging (Benoit et al., 2011). Increased expression of dynorphins in aged brain could affect cognition and emotional behaviors by impairing glutamatergic transmission. Both exogenously applied and endogenously released dynorphins weaken glutamatergic neurotransmission and long-term potentiation (Wagner et al., 1993). Activation of κ-opioid receptor (KOR), a postsynaptic receptor of dynorphins, suppresses presynaptic glutamate release (Simmons et al., 1994). Furthermore, KOR activation mediates stress-induced memory deficits (Carey et al., 2009). Notably, Group 1 mGluR activation increases Pdyn expression (Mao and Wang, 2001), suggesting a tight interaction between these systems. These findings raise a possibility that alteration of Pdyn could affect mGluR function and cognitive processes in aged brain.
In the current study, we evaluated whether deletion of Pdyn gene affects mGluR expression and function, memory formation, and anxiety during aging using Pdyn knock-out mice (Loacker et al., 2007). We observed an increase in Pdyn expression in the hippocampus and cortex of wild-type (WT) mice with aging. Group 1 mGluR protein level and function and related IEG expression were significantly enhanced in aged Pdyn−/− mice compared with WT. Although old WT mice developed cognitive deficits in the Morris Water Maze (MWM) and novel object recognition (NOR) task, memory processes were intact in old Pdyn−/− mice. Finally, anxiety-related behaviors increased with age in WT but not in Pdyn−/− mice.
Materials and Methods
Mouse breeding.
Heterozygous Pdyn+/− mice (C57BL/6N background; obtained from Dr. Christoph Schwarzer) (Loacker et al., 2007) were bred to generate the F1 generation of Pdyn+/+, Pdyn+/−, and Pdyn−/− mice. Pdyn+/+ littermates were used as WT controls. Animals were housed up to five per cage and maintained on a 12 h light/dark cycle with ad libitum access to food (Purina Lab Chow; Mondou) and water in the Douglas Institute animal facility until 6, 12, or 18–25 months old. Animal care, surgery, and handling procedures were approved by the McGill University Animal Care Committee. Male mice were killed within a month after behavioral training for biochemical and electrophysiological analyses.
Mouse genotyping.
DNA from mice ears was extracted using HotSHOT genomic DNA preparation. The genotype of mice was determined by PCR using Pdyn primers (5′-GGCTTCTCATCTTTTCTCACCC-3′ and 5′-TCACCACCTTGAACTGACGC-3′) situated on exon 4 of the Pdyn gene and Cre primers (5′-CCACGACCAAGTGACAGCAATG-3′ and 5′-AAGTGCCTTCTCTACACCTGCG-3′) situated on the Cre-recombinase sequence, with 38 cycles of 94°C for 45 s, 57°C for 45 s, and 72°C for 60 s. Pdyn−/− will produce a band of 327 bp with Cre primers, whereas a band will be detected at 550 bp for WT with Pdyn primers.
Drugs used for behavioral experiments.
Norbinaltorphimine dihydrochloride (norBNI), 3-cyano-N-(1,3-diphenyl-1H-pyrazol-5-yl)benzamide (CDPPB), and 2-methyl-6-(phenylethynyl)pyridine hydrochloride (MPEP) were purchased from Tocris Bioscience. Drugs were dissolved in d-methyl-sulfoxide (DMSO, Sigma-Aldrich). CDPPB, MPEP, norBNI, and DMSO controls were diluted in saline solution (5% final concentration of DMSO) and administered intraperitoneally 20 min before behavioral training. All drugs were given at 10 mg/kg (Christoffersen et al., 2008; Uslaner et al., 2009; Munro et al., 2012; Smith et al., 2012).
Novel object recognition.
This task evaluates episodic and reference memory. To reduce stress associated to a novel environment, mice were first exposed to an empty arena for 5 min (day 0). All the experiments were conducted under red light to facilitate object interaction and reduce stress. On day 1, the mouse was allowed to interact with two identical objects (familiar) for 5 min. Sixty minutes later, the animal was reintroduced in the arena (50 cm × 50 cm) with a familiar object and a novel object for 5 min. The position of the objects was always the same in the arena to remove any spatial memory parameter from the task. On day 2 (24 h later), the mouse was exposed again to the familiar object and another novel object for 5 min. After a 5 min delay, the animal was exposed once more to the familiar object and another novel object for 5 min. Unless otherwise specified, all experiments were digitally recorded for offline analysis using a tracking system Top Scan 2.0 (Clever Systems). Both the distance traveled and interaction time with objects of studied mice were examined.
Elevated plus maze.
On day 3, the animals were tested for 5 min in an elevated plus maze (EPM) apparatus to evaluate anxiety and explorative behaviors. This task opposes the innate fear of rodents for open bright spaces to their desire to explore new environments. Security was provided by the closed arms, whereas the open arms offer exploratory values. Distance traveled of mice in different arms of the maze was measured by Top Scan 2.0. Total distance traveled was used as an indicator of motor function.
Novelty-suppressed feeding (Thatcher-Britton).
After 24 h of food deprivation, time taken by the mice to reach food pellets in the center area of a rat open field (120 cm × 120 cm) was measured on day 4 to evaluate anxious and explorative behaviors (maximum of 10 min). Like EPM, this task approaches the conflict between the innate fear of rodents of the open bright center area of the arena versus their desire to feed. Security was provided by the walls of the arena.
MWM.
In this task, mice were required to locate a fixed position hidden submerged platform (10 cm diameter, 1 cm below water surface) in a water pool (1.2 m diameter) containing water (24°C) that is rendered opaque by nontoxic white paint using distal visuospatial cues. Before the hidden platform training, a cued test, including three trials of 60 s in which the platform was visible, was conducted on day 0 of the second week of behavioral training to assess age-related visual deficits and motivation to escape from water. Swimming speed was used as a control of motor function, a parameter potentially altered by aging. On day 1, mice were trained to find the hidden platform by pseudo-randomly placing the mice in a different position on each of the three 60-s-long trials per day for five consecutive days (days 1–5). The platform was positioned in the middle of the same quadrant in these trials. Animals were guided to the platform if it was not located within 60 s. All the mice remained on the platform for 15 s before removal. At 60 min after the acquisition phase on day 5, mice were given one probe trial of 60 s for which the platform was removed from the pool. The number of times the animal swam over the platform location was evaluated with a tracking system. After 2 d of rest, all animals were submitted to a reverse memory paradigm to assess reversal learning. Briefly, the platform was moved to the opposite quadrant while the position of visual cues stayed the same. Mice were then trained to find the new platform location for four consecutive days (three trials per day). A second probe test was conducted 60 min after the last acquisition trial on day 11. Finally, a third probe test was done 7 d later to reactivate and examine long-term memory processes. Animals were killed by decapitation 2–3 h after the last MWM probe test to allow IEG expression triggered by memory retrieval (day 18). After each trial, mice were immediately placed under a heat lamp to prevent hypothermia. To control for possible effects due to circadian cycles, all trials were performed at approximately the same time of the day between 10 and 15 h. Data derived from the MWM task were recorded on a computer using a video tracking system.
Tissue preparation and biochemical analysis.
The hippocampus and adjacent cortex from one half of the brain were dissected as previously reported (Lauterborn et al., 2000; Ménard and Quirion, 2012b), snap frozen, and stored at −80°C for immunoblotting experiments. The other half-brain was fixed by immersion in 4% paraformaldehyde for 24 h, cryoprotected in 30% sucrose for 48 h, and stored at −80°C for immunocytochemical experiments.
Immunoblotting.
Tissue containing the hippocampus, entorhinal, perirhinal, and portions of adjacent neocortices were homogenized with a Polytron in 2 ml of lysis buffer (50 mm Tris-acetate, pH 7.4, 100 μm EGTA, 5 μm leupeptin, 200 μm phenylmethylsulfonyl fluoride, and 1 μg/ml N-tosyl-l-phenylalanine chloromethyl ketone, Sigma-Aldrich). Total protein concentrations were determined using bicinchoninic acid protein assay kit (Pierce). Western blot analysis was performed on aliquots of homogenates obtained from WT and Pdyn−/− mouse brains. A total of 10 μg of protein was loaded on 4–20% Tris-glycine gels (Invitrogen) and subjected to SDS-PAGE. Proteins were transferred onto Hybond-C nitrocellulose membranes (GE Healthcare). To block nonspecific sites, membranes were first incubated for 1 h at room temperature in PBS containing 2% BSA. Membranes were next incubated with primary antibodies against Group 1 mGluR (mGluR1α and mGluR5, Millipore), Homer 1a (Santa Cruz Biotechnology), or Arc proteins (Cell Signaling) in PBS containing 2% BSA. Bands corresponding to proteins were detected by peroxidase-conjugated secondary antibody (Santa Cruz Biotechnology) and revealed by Western Lightning Chemiluminescence Reagent Plus (PerkinElmer) on Kodak BioMax MS film (GE Healthcare). The same labeling procedure was used for negative controls with the primary antibody omitted. Actin level was used as a loading control. Immunoblots were placed on a Northern light illuminator, and computer-generated images were analyzed semiquantitatively by densitometry with a microcomputer imaging device (Imaging Research, MCID).
Immunocytochemistry.
Coronal sections (20 μm) of fixed brains (fixed with 4% paraformaldehyde and cryoprotected in 30% sucrose solution) at the level of dorsal hippocampus were washed in PBS, pH 7.4, for 5 min then immersed in methanol containing 0.3% H2O2 for 30 min at room temperature to quench endogenous peroxidases. After three washes of 5 min each in PBS, sections were preincubated with 10% normal goat serum (NGS) at room temperature for 1 h, followed by incubation with polyclonal Pdyn (Millipore, dilution 1/1000) or mGluR5 (dilution 1/5000) primary antibodies (in PBS with 1% NGS) overnight at 4°C. Subsequently, sections were washed 3 times in PBS and incubated in biotinylated anti-guinea pig IgG for Pdyn or anti-rabbit for mGluR5 (1:1000, Vector Laboratories) in 1% NGS for 30 min. After washing in PBS, sections were finally incubated with avidin biotinylated enzyme complex (ABC reagent, Vector Laboratories) diluted in PBS for 30 min. The peroxidase reaction was performed with 0.02% hydrogen peroxide and 3,3′-diaminobenzidine tetrahydrochloride (0.1% in 100 mm Tris-HCl buffer, pH 7.4). Sections were then washed in tap water, cleaned, and mounted on Superfrost Plus slides (Thermo Scientific). Immunohistochemical staining was visualized using bright-field microscopy at 4× and 20× magnification. Controls were prepared using the same labeling procedure with either primary or secondary antibody omitted.
Electrophysiology.
Brains were rapidly removed from mice that were anesthetized by isoflurane, and coronal brain slices (350 μm thick) were cut in hyperosmotic, ice-cold, and carbogenated slice cutting solution (in mm: 252 sucrose, 2.5 KCl, 4 MgCl2, 0.1 CaCl2, 1.25 KH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3 and 10 glucose, ∼360 mOsmol/L) using a Vibratome. Freshly cut slices were incubated with carbogenated artificial CSF (aCSF in mm: 125 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 1 MgCl2, 2 CaCl2, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, and 25 glucose, ∼310 mOsmol/L) at 32°C for 1 h and subsequently maintained at room temperature. Bicuculline methobromide (5 μm; Sigma-Aldrich) was used to block GABAA receptor-mediated inhibitory synaptic transmission in all recordings. Postsynaptic responses were evoked by stimulating the Schaffer collateral-commissural pathway (constant current pulses, 0.08 ms) through a tungsten bipolar electrode (FHC) at 0.05 Hz. Field EPSPs (fEPSPs) in hippocampal CA1 stratum radiatum were detected by aCSF-filled glass electrodes. Synaptic responses were amplified and digitized by Multiclamp 700B and Digidata 1400, respectively (Molecular Devices) and stored in a PC for offline analysis using Clampfit (Molecular Devices). All recordings were performed at room temperature. To induce mGluR-dependent LTD, dihydroxyphenylglycine (DHPG) (50 μm, 10 min; Tocris Bioscience) was applied in aCSF. mGluR-LTD was measured at 60 min after the end of DHPG application.
Immunofluorescence.
Coronal 20 μm sections of fixed brains at the level of the dorsal hippocampus were washed in PBS, pH 7.4, for 5 min. Sections were permeabilized with 0.2% Tween 20 in PBS for 10 min at room temperature and then processed for immunofluorescence labeling. In brief, sections were first incubated in 10% NGS (or normal horse serum for Homer 1a) diluted in 0.1 m PBS with 0.05% Tween 20 (PBST) and 1% BSA for 60 min at room temperature, followed by overnight incubation with primary antibodies (1:100 for Homer 1a and Arc and 1:500 for the neuronal marker microtubule-associated protein 2 [MAP2], Abcam) at 4°C in PBST containing 1% serum and 1% BSA. After 3 washes in PBS, sections were incubated with corresponding secondary antibodies (1:500, Invitrogen) conjugated with AlexaFluor-488 or AlexaFluor-568 in 1% BSA in PBST for 2 h at room temperature in the dark. Sections were washed three times with PBS for 5 min each in the dark. Nuclei were stained with Hoechst solution (2 μg/ml, Invitrogen) for 5 min; subsequently, the sections were washed and coverslipped with Fluoromount-G (Southern Biotechnology). Pictures were taken at 40× magnification with an Axio Observer microscope with Apotome (Carl Zeiss).
Statistical analysis.
All data are presented as mean ± SEM. Data of protein levels, MWM parameters, NOR interaction time, distance traveled, and time to reach food were analyzed by two-way ANOVA, followed by Bonferroni post hoc analysis (GraphPad Prism software). Percentage depression of fEPSP during LTD was analyzed using two-tailed Student's t test. IEG protein levels and behavioral data correlations were evaluated with a two-tailed Pearson's test. Significance level was set at p < 0.05.
Results
Group 1 metabotropic glutamate receptor expression and function increases with age in Pdyn−/− mice
We first compared Pdyn expression in young (Y, 7 months old), middle-aged (M-A, 13 months old), and old (O, 23 months old) C57BL/6 WT mice. Consistent with previously published findings (Schwarzer, 2009), Pdyn immunostaining was enriched in the CA3 and dentate gyrus of the adult hippocampus (Fig. 1). Interestingly, positive staining was observed in the CA1 region only for old WT mice. Pdyn expression increased with age in both the hippocampus and adjacent cortex. No Pdyn staining was observed in old Pdyn−/− mice (KO-O, 24 months old).
We next evaluated Group 1 mGluR levels in the hippocampal formation of young, middle-aged, and old WT and Pdyn−/− mice. mGluR1α (Fig. 2A; genotype effect F(1,30) = 4.73, p = 0.0376, 93.8 ± 12.0% for WT vs 154.6 ± 26.7% for Pdyn−/−, n = 6) and mGluR5 protein levels (Fig. 2B; genotype effect F(1,30) = 6.78, p = 0.0142, 121.1 ± 13.5% for WT vs 210.3 ± 24.6% for Pdyn−/−, n = 6) were significantly increased only in old Pdyn−/− mice hippocampus and adjacent cortex. mGluR5 immunoreactivity revealed intensely stained pyramidal neurons in the hippocampal CA1 region (Fig. 2C). Specificity of the antibody was controlled with mGluR5 peptide preadsorption (data not shown). Although immunostaining slightly decreased during aging for WT mice, higher immunoreactivity was observed in the CA1 region of old Pdyn−/− (Fig. 2C).
To test whether Group 1 mGluR function is affected in Pdyn−/− mice, we studied the effect of pharmacological activation of Group 1 mGluRs with a specific agonist DHPG on synaptic function. DHPG has been shown to induce LTD of glutamatergic transmission (Palmer et al., 1997). We examined changes in the slope of fEPSPs in the hippocampal CA1 region of both old WT and Pdyn−/− mice after DHPG application (50 μm, 10 min; Fig. 3A). We found that the percentage depression at 60 min after DHPG application in old Pdyn−/− slices was significantly higher than age-matched WT controls (Fig. 3B; −30.4 ± 5.7% in Pdyn−/− vs −4.2 ± 6.4% in WT, unpaired two-tailed t test p = 0.0123, n = 6 slices/genotype with 3–4 mice per genotype). To examine whether the impact of Pdyn knockdown on mGluR function occurs in aged mice only, we performed LTD recordings in young WT and Pdyn−/− mice (8–9 months old) but found no differences (Fig. 3C,D). Knockdown of Pdyn therefore increases Group 1 mGluR expression and function in the aged hippocampus.
Spatial memory is intact in Pdyn−/− mice despite aging
Synaptic plasticity is crucial to maintain cognitive abilities, and LTD induced by Group 1 mGluR activation has been linked to memory in the aging brain (Lee et al., 2005). We predicted that the maintenance of mGluR expression and LTD would protect old mice from displaying cognitive impairment. We trained 6-, 10–14-, and 18–25-month-old WT and Pdyn−/− mice in a hippocampal-dependent MWM task to assess their spatial memory. No differences were observed between the groups in the visible platform trials that assess swimming ability, visual function, and motivation (Fig. 4A). In the hidden platform trials, old WT mice became memory-impaired and exhibited longer escape latencies than younger mice (Fig. 4B; age effect: one-way ANOVA, F(2,46) = 9.35, p = 0.0004, escape latency on day 5: young: 18.2 ± 2.2 s; middle-aged: 24.3 ± 3.6 s; old: 32.4 ± 2.2 s, n = 15–16). Genetic deletion of Pdyn prevented this deficit with latencies to escape to the platform comparable to young and middle-aged animals (age effect: one-way ANOVA, F(2,33) = 0.888, p = 0.421, escape latency on day 5: young: 12.6 ± 1.4 s; middle-aged: 17.7 ± 2.4 s; old: 19.5 ± 3.8 s, n = 10–18). Average escape latencies of old Pdyn−/− mice were significantly lower than WT mice on hidden platform training days 3, 4, and 5, and two-way ANOVA analysis revealed significant difference in learning acquisition for Pdyn−/− compared with WT (F(1,96) = 13.4, p = 0.0012).
Inhibitory learning and reversal memory are highly dependent on hippocampal LTD (Dong et al., 2013). mGluR5 knock-out mice perform poorly in reversal learning (Xu et al., 2009), suggesting that the intact mGluR-LTD in aged Pdyn−/− animals can rescue reversal learning of these mice. We found that old WT mice were unable to learn the new platform position in a reversal learning paradigm (Fig. 4C; time effect: one-way ANOVA, F(3.60) = 1.318, p = 0.277, escape latency on day 8: 43.3 ± 2.7 s, day 9: 38.8 ± 3.9 s; day 10: 35.5 ± 3.2 s; day 11: 35.0 ± 3.5 s, n = 16). On the other hand, old Pdyn−/− exhibited successful reversal learning (time effect: one-way ANOVA, F(3,36) = 2.96, p = 0.0459, escape latency on day 8: 25.8 ± 4.2 s, day 9: 18.8 ± 3.0 s, day 10: 17.4 ± 1.6 s, day 11: 15.7 ± 2.7 s, n = 10). Moreover, the average escape latency of old Pdyn−/− mice on day 11 was similar to young and middle-aged Pdyn−/− mice (age effect: one-way ANOVA, F(2,27) = 1.20, p = 0.317, young: 10.8 ± 2.2 s; middle-aged: 15.8 ± 3.2 s; old: 15.7 ± 2.7 s, n = 10–18), suggesting intact memory processes. Reversal learning curves were significantly different between the old mice groups (genotype effect: two-way ANOVA, F(1,72) = 26.2, p < 0.0001).
Unimpaired spatial memory in old Pdyn−/− mice was also suggested by probe trials (Fig. 5A). Probe trials were performed 60 min after the last hidden platform training on day 5 of learning and day 11 of reverse acquisition tasks. The number of platform crossings decreased with age for the WT mice after regular learning (age effect: one-way ANOVA, F(2,46) = 5.21, p = 0.0092, day 5: young: 3.9 ± 0.5; middle-aged: 3.4 ± 0.5; old: 1.9 ± 0.3) and reversal learning probe tests (age effect: one-way ANOVA, F(2.46) = 4.56, p = 0.0156, day 11: young: 4.2 ± 0.6; middle-aged: 3.3 ± 0.6; old: 1.9 ± 0.4, n = 15–16). Conversely, we observed no age differences in Pdyn−/− mice platform crossings during probe tests after regular learning (age effect: one-way ANOVA, F(2,28) = 3.28, p = 0.0526, day 5: young: 6.2 ± 0.8; middle-aged: 3.2 ± 0.8; old: 4.1 ± 0.9) and reversal learning tasks (age effect: one-way ANOVA, F(2,28) = 0.735, p = 0.489, day 11: young: 5.5 ± 0.5; middle-aged: 4.0 ± 0.9; old: 4.6 ± 0.8, n = 10–18). Old Pdyn−/− mice crossed the hidden platform positions significantly more times than old WT mice in both probe tests (genotype effect: two-way ANOVA, F(1,24) = 10.9, p = 0.003, n = 10–18). Old Pdyn−/− mice swimming speed was not significantly different from WT mice during probe tests (Fig. 5B; aging and genotype effect: two-way ANOVA, F(1,24) = 3.18, p = 0.0872, n = 10–16). Together, these results support our hypothesis that the increase in Group 1 mGluR expression and function in old Pdyn−/− mice associates with improved spatial learning in these animals.
Spatial memory is rescued in old WT mice by mGluR5-positive modulator CDPPB or KOR antagonist norBNI
If the impact of aging on spatial memory performance is related to an increase and a decrease in Pdyn and mGluR5 function, respectively, we predicted that old WT mice spatial memory deficits could be improved by either enhancing Group 1 mGluR function or blocking KOR activation. To test this hypothesis, old WT mice were injected with DMSO (vehicle), mGluR5 allosteric modulator CDPPB or norBNI, an antagonist of KOR, 20 min before the start of training on each training day and spatial memory was assessed with the MWM task. Old WT and Pdyn−/− mice were first trained to find a visible platform. Neither genotype (Fig. 6A) nor pharmacological treatment (Fig. 6B) affected motivation to escape water, visual function, or motor function. Treatments with CDPPB or norBNI significantly improved old WT mice acquisition of the task in the first 5 training days (Fig. 6C, two-way ANOVA compared with untreated old WT: F(1,80) = 7.14, p = 0.0146 for CDPPB, Bonferroni post tests < 0.05 on days 4 and 5; F(1,88) = 19.65, p = 0.0002 for norBNI, Bonferroni post tests < 0.05 on days 2, 3, and 4 and F(1,76) = 0.16, p = 0.694 for vehicle, n = 5–16). Similar results were obtained for acquisition of reversal learning with shorter latencies for old WT mice treated with CDPPB or norBNI compared with vehicle or untreated mice (Fig. 6D; two-way ANOVA compared with untreated old WT: CDPPB, F(1,60) = 2.23, p = 0.151, Bonferroni post tests < 0.05 on day 11; norBNI: F(1,66) = 13.7, p = 0.0013, Bonferroni post tests < 0.05 on days 9, 10, and 11; vehicle: F(1,57) = 0.20, p = 0.663, n = 5–16). Finally, CDPPB and norBNI enhanced old WT mice platform crossings for both normal learning and reversal learning probe tests compared with untreated mice (Fig. 6E; two-way ANOVA compared with untreated old WT, CDPPB: F(1,20) = 21.73, p = 0.0002; norBNI: F(1,22) = 14.13, p = 0.0011; vehicle: F(1,19) = 0.67, p = 0.4242, n = 5–16).
Protective effect of Pdyn−/− on spatial memory in old mice is abolished by the mGluR5 inhibitor MPEP
If the protective effect of knocking down Pdyn on spatial memory formation in old Pdyn−/− mice is related to the enhancement of mGluR5 function, we expected that intact spatial memory of these mice will be affected by blocking mGluR5. To test that, we injected old Pdyn−/− mice with the mGluR5 antagonist MPEP or DMSO (vehicle) 20 min before the start of training on each training day. As expected, MPEP impaired the acquisition of the MWM task of old Pdyn−/− mice by increasing the latencies for finding the hidden platform on training days 4 and 5 (Fig. 6C; two-way ANOVA compared with untreated old Pdyn−/− mice: F(1,60) = 3.11, p = 0.0983 for MPEP, Bonferroni post tests < 0.05 on days 4 and 5 and F(1,52) = 0.01, p = 0.941 for vehicle, n = 5–10). In addition, MPEP impaired reversal MWM learning of old Pdyn−/− mice on training days 8, 9, and 10 (Fig. 6D; two-way ANOVA compared with untreated old Pdyn−/− mice: MPEP F(1,45) = 11.37, p = 0.0042, Bonferroni post tests < 0.05 on days 8, 9, and 10; vehicle: F(1,39) = 0.47, p = 0.5059, n = 5–10). Finally, MPEP-treated old Pdyn−/− mice displayed probe trials performances that were significantly poorer than untreated and vehicle-treated old Pdyn−/− mice (Fig. 6E; two-way ANOVA compared with untreated old Pdyn−/− mice: F(1,15) = 4.81, p = 0.0444 for MPEP and F(1,13) = 0.08, p = 0.7804 for vehicle, n = 5–10).
Group 1 mGluR-related immediate early gene Homer 1a and Arc expression is high in old Pdyn−/− mice and correlated with MWM performances
Formation and consolidation of memories involve the expression of various IEGs, including Homer 1a and Arc (Ménard and Quirion, 2012b). If the increase of mGluR expression and function in Pdyn−/− mice is responsible for rescuing age-related cognitive deficits in these mice, we expect that the enhanced cognitive performance of Pdyn−/− mice associates with increases in the expression of downstream mGluR signals, such as Homer 1a and Activity-regulated cytoskeleton associated protein (Arc also known as Arg 3.1). Homer 1a protein level increased with age in Pdyn−/− mice after MWM training (Fig. 7A; young: 93.7 ± 12.5%, middle-aged: 115.7 ± 15.6%, old: 154.5 ± 18.9%, n = 6), whereas it remained stable in WT mice (young: 100%, middle-aged: 115.2 ± 20.1%, old: 100.9 ± 8.5%, n = 6). The effect of age and genotype is significant between the old groups. Homer 1a expression is positively correlated with old mice MWM probe test performance (average of regular and reversal learning platform crossings) (Fig. 7B; Pearson correlation: p = 0.0041). More Homer 1a-positive cells were observed in the CA1 hippocampal region of old Pdyn−/− mice compared with WT mice at the same age (Fig. 7C).
In line with the age-related cognitive decline, Arc protein level decreased gradually with aging in WT mice hippocampal formation (Fig. 7D; age effect: one-way ANOVA, F(2,15) = 7.159, p = 0.0066, young: 100%, middle-aged: 85.4 ± 7.4%; old: 73.29 ± 6.2%, n = 6). Conversely, Arc expression was maintained in old Pdyn−/− mice (age effect: one-way ANOVA, F(2,15) = 0.9815, p = 0.3976, young: 89.2 ± 9.2%; middle-aged: 109.5 ± 13.5%; old: 105.4 ± 4.1%, n = 6). Again, the effect of genotype on Arc expression was significant between old mice groups. Similar to Homer 1a, Arc expression was positively correlated with old mice MWM probe test performance (Fig. 7E; Pearson correlation: p = 0.0014). The number of Arc-positive-cells was higher in old Pdyn−/− CA1 hippocampal area compared with WT (Fig. 7F). The correlation between the levels of mGluR downstream signals and cognitive performance of Pdyn−/− mice suggests an important role of maintaining mGluR expression and function for the intact spatial learning of these mice.
Recognition memory is intact in old Pdyn−/− mice
To find out whether age-related impairments of nonspatial memory (Rowe et al., 1998; Burke et al., 2011) are also protected in old Pdyn−/− mice, we compared the performance of WT and Pdyn−/− mice in both a long-term recognition memory task and a novel object recognition task. Mice were first exposed to two identical objects for 5 min. Recognition memory was tested by comparing time of interaction with a new versus familiar object after 5 min, 60 min, and 24 h delays. Old WT mice displayed memory deficits by spending close to 50% time (chance level) with the novel object (Fig. 8A). On the other hand, old Pdyn−/− mice spent consistently more time with the novel object than old WT mice (ratio for Pdyn−/− vs WT for 5 min: 65.3 ± 3.9% vs 49.4 ± 6.7%; 60 min: 75.2 ± 3.9% vs 51.2 ± 5.9%; 24 h: 62.6 ± 3.0% vs 47.4 ± 6.5%, n = 8–17), suggesting intact recognition memory. Genotype effect was significant between old WT and Pdyn−/− mice (two-way ANOVA, F(1,69) = 13.17, p = 0.0014) but not between young and middle-aged mice.
Apart from cognitive deficits, aging associates also with altered emotional behaviors, such as increased anxiety (Lenze et al., 2001). The implicated role of dynorphin in anxiety-related behaviors (Schwarzer, 2009) suggests that Pdyn−/− could protect aged mice from increased anxiety. Indeed, in the novel object recognition task, Pdyn−/− mice exhibit higher exploratory activity. Pdyn−/− mice consistently traveled a longer distance in 5 min than WT mice of the same age (Fig. 8B; Pdyn−/− vs WT: young: 8.5 ± 2.2 m vs 4.2 ± 0.4 m; middle-aged: 10.4 ± 1.6 m vs 6.8 ± 1.0 m; old: 5.8 ± 0.6 m vs 3.6 ± 0.4 m, n = 7–17). Representative paths are shown in Figure 8C.
To find out whether age-related changes in recognition memory and exploratory behaviors in old WT mice are related to the enhanced Pdyn and reduced mGluR5 expression, we evaluated the impact of CDPPB and norBNI treatments on old WT mice recognition memory and found that these pharmacological manipulations improved old WT mice performances in these tasks (Fig. 8D; two-way ANOVA compared with untreated old WT: F(1,42) = 4.85, p = 0.0448 for CDPPB, Bonferroni post tests < 0.05 at 5 min delay and for average values; F(1,42) = 9.46, p = 0.0082 for norBNI, Bonferroni post tests < 0.05 at 24 h delay and for average values and F(1,33) = 1.65, p = 0.2253 for vehicle, n = 5–8). We also examined whether the protective effects of Pdyn−/− on old mice performance in these recognition memory tasks were related to the enhancement of mGluR5 function by studying the impact of MPEP on recognition memory formation in old Pdyn−/−. Surprisingly, we observed no effect of MPEP on old Pdyn−/− mice performances (Fig. 8D; two-way ANOVA and comparison with untreated old Pdyn−/− mice: MPEP, F(1,69) = 3.85, p = 0.0620; vehicle: F(1,60) = 0.02, p = 0.8827, n = 5–17). Finally, these drugs did not significantly alter locomotion or exploratory activity (Fig. 8E; WT: 3.6 ± 0.4 m vs Pdyn−/−: 5.8 ± 0.6 m vs norBNI: 5.3 ± 0.2 m, n = 5–17).
Anxiety levels remain low in Pdyn−/− mice despite aging
Higher locomotor and exploration activities observed in Pdyn−/− mice compared with WT (genotype effect: two-way ANOVA, F(1,64) = 11.70, p = 0.0011) suggest that anxiety-related behaviors could be reduced in these mice. We thus investigated further the anxiety state of mice using the EPM task. Mice have to overcome the fear of bright open space to explore a novel environment while security was provided by the closed arms. No difference was observed for distance traveled in the EPM open arms for young animals (Fig. 9A). However, aging affected anxiety-related behaviors as middle-aged and old WT mice traveled a shorter distance in the EPM open arms than young mice (age effect: one-way ANOVA, F(2,27) = 4.30, p = 0.0274, young: 116.4 ± 53.8 mm; middle-aged: 29.8 ± 12.9 mm; old: 44.1 ± 15.8 mm, n = 8–15). In contrast, Pdyn−/− mice at all ages traveled a similar distance in the open arms (age effect: one-way ANOVA, F(2,28) = 0.0952, p = 0.910, young: 221.6 ± 77.2 mm; middle-aged: 179.6 ± 50.2 mm; old: 248.5 ± 107.2 mm, n = 7–15), indicating no effect of aging on anxiety in these mice. Aging and the genotype effect (two-way ANOVA, F(1,61) = 5.80, p = 0.0191) were significant for both middle-aged and old mice groups. Locomotion activity was higher for Pdyn−/− young and old groups (Fig. 9B) (genotype effect: two-way ANOVA, F(1,63) = 11.08, p = 0.0015, n = 7–15). Representative paths are shown on Figure 9C.
Next, we tested the impact of mGluR5-related drugs on EPM performances in old WT and Pdyn−/− mice. Positive modulation of mGluR5 had no impact on old WT mice behavior, whereas treatment with MPEP significantly reduced distance traveled in the open arms for Pdyn−/− mice (Fig. 9D; MPEP: 60.6 ± 13.4 mm; vehicle: 218.2 ± 91.9 mm; Pdyn−/−: 248.5 ± 107.2 mm, n = 5–15). To find out whether the improved EPM performances in old Pdyn−/− mice are related to a reduction of dynorphin receptor function, we examined the impact of norBNI on the EPM performances of old WT mice and found that norBNI enhanced locomotion and exploratory activity of old WT mice compared with untreated controls (Fig. 9E; WT: 3.4 ± 0.2 m vs Pdyn−/−: 5.3 ± 0.6 m vs norBNI: 5.1 ± 0.3 m, n = 5–15), suggesting that blocking activation of these receptors decreases age-related anxious behaviors.
Finally, we examined the anxiety state of Pdyn−/− and WT mice using the novelty-suppressed feeding (Thatcher-Britton) task. In this test, we measured the time mice use to reach the food in the middle of a light exposed open field arena. As expected, time to reach food increased with age in WT mice (Fig. 10A; one-way ANOVA, F(2,33) = 4.90, p = 0.0137, young: 282.1 ± 37.4 s; middle-aged: 361.2 ± 50.8 s; old: 494.0 ± 40.8 s, n = 8–15). Aging had a similar effect on Pdyn−/− mice (one-way ANOVA, F(2,30) = 6.328, p = 0.0051, young: 144.4 ± 31.8 s; middle-aged: 266.6 ± 63.6 s; old: 378.4 ± 51.0 s, n = 7–15). Nonetheless, the average food-seeking time is consistently lower for Pdyn−/− compared with WT at all ages (genotype effect: two-way ANOVA, F(1.63) = 8.30, p = 0.0054). Positive modulation of mGluR5 by CDPPB significantly reduced the time to reach food of old WT mice compared with untreated controls (Fig. 10B; CDPPB: 214.5 ± 41.0 s; WT: 494.0 ± 40.8 s, n = 5–15). Together, our results suggest that high Pdyn and low mGluR5 expression in the aged brain could increase anxiety-related behaviors and impair memory formation.
Discussion
In the present study, we found that knocking down Pdyn gene expression ameliorated several cognitive, synaptic, and molecular changes that are associated with aging. First, knocking down Pdyn rescued aged mice from deficits of spatial and recognition memory. In addition, the aging-associated increase in anxiety-related behaviors and decrease in exploratory activity are suppressed in Pdyn−/− mice. At the synaptic level, we found that knocking down Pdyn increased Group 1 mGluR expression and, importantly, rescued the weakening of hippocampal mGluR-LTD in aging. In line with these results, blocking mGluR5 activity impaired old Pdyn−/− mice spatial memory and enhanced anxious behaviors in the EPM task. In contrast, blocking KOR function or enhancing mGluR5 activation by a positive modulator before behavioral testing rescued MWM and recognition memory of old WT mice. In addition, KOR inhibitor norBNI reduced anxiety-related behaviors in EPM task of old WT mice. Finally, at the molecular level, aging-related decrease in Arc expression, a downstream mGluR signal that has been highly implicated in spatial learning, is completely rescued in Pdyn−/− mice. In addition, spatial learning-related Homer 1a expression in old Pdyn−/− mice is significantly higher than WT mice. Together, our findings suggest that an increase in expression of Pdyn during normal aging plays substantial roles in aging-related cognitive and psychopathological changes. In addition, these changes could be related to a reduction in Group 1 mGluR signaling and function.
Although Pdyn has been shown to impair spatial memory formation in MWM, our findings suggest that increased Pdyn expression in aging is related to deficits in both regular and reversal spatial learning in MWM. A decline in spatial memory formation has been associated with increased Pdyn expression in aged rodents (Gallagher and Nicolle, 1993; Svensson et al., 2006). Indeed, direct hippocampal injection of dynorphin (McDaniel et al., 1990) or KOR agonist (Sandin et al., 1998) in adult rats impairs MWM learning. Although most of these studies focused on the impact of dynorphin on MWM acquisition during training, we compared both performances of mice during training and probe tests (long-term memory retrieval) and found that knocking down Pdyn protected aged Pdyn−/− mice from impairments in the acquisition and retention of spatial memory. In addition, we observed no aging-related decline in reversal MWM training and retention in Pdyn−/− mice. Finally, blocking KOR by norBNI alone improved spatial memory of old WT mice. These findings strongly support the hypothesis that increased expression of Pdyn during aging is an important molecular substrate for spatial memory impairment. Although a previous study using another Pdyn KO mouse line did not reveal better probe test performance than WT mice (Nguyen et al., 2005), this seeming discrepancy could result from the use of younger mice (13–17 months vs 18–25 months in the current study) and shorter training paradigm in that study.
We also discovered a previously unknown role of Pdyn in the decline of recognition memory in aging. Aging-related cognitive impairment in recognition memory is highly common in animal models and human (Burke et al., 2012). Although Pdyn has been implicated in stress-induced impairment of NOR, manipulating KOR activation and Pdyn expression has little effect on normal NOR in adult mice (Carey et al., 2009). Our finding that the aging-related decline in recognition memory can be rescued in Pdyn−/− mice, together with the rescue effect of norBNI on recognition memory in old WT mice, provided, to our knowledge, the first evidence suggesting an involvement of Pdyn in aging-related NOR deficits.
Despite the seemingly well-known anxiogenic role of dynorphin, we revealed for the first time that dynorphin could be responsible for the heightened anxiety in aging. An anxiogenic role of opioid was revealed initially by the effect of an opioid receptor partial agonist naloxone pretreatment to abolish the anticonflict effect of chlordiazepoxide (Billingsley and Kubena, 1978), suggesting that the anxiolytic effect of benzodiazepine is related to blockade of opioid receptor. The anticonflict effect of benzodiazepine can be blocked by the pretreatment of the KOR antagonist norBNI (Tsuda et al., 1996), supporting a crucial role of dynorphin signaling in anxiety. Indeed, both pharmacological (norBNI) and genetic ablation (Pdyn KO) of dynorphin signals is sufficient to increase exploratory activities in the open field and the open arm of EPM (Knoll et al., 2007; Wittmann et al., 2009). In our study, we observed for the first time an anxiolytic effect of norBNI in old mice by increasing the distance traveled in the EPM open arms and overall exploratory activity of these mice (Fig. 9). Alternatively, dynorphin peptide or KOR agonists are anxiogenic (Tsuda et al., 1996; Wittmann et al., 2009; Smith et al., 2012). Although anxiety and depression in late life are risk factors for disability (Lenze et al., 2001), little is known of the underlying biological mechanisms. Using EPM and novelty-suppression of feeding, we confirmed an increase in anxiety in aged mice. Importantly, we revealed anxiolytic effects of knocking down Pdyn in both tests, suggesting an important contribution of Pdyn in aging-related increase in anxiety. Pdyn−/− mice displayed a decrease and increase in mRNA expression of corticotrophin releasing hormone and neuropeptide Y, respectively (Wittmann et al., 2009); both changes are likely anxiolytic (Arborelius et al., 1999; Carvajal et al., 2004). Thus, the anxiogenic effect of dynorphin in aging is likely related to the interaction between these neuropeptides that regulate emotions.
mGluR5-related signals have been linked to successful cognitive aging in rodents (Lee et al., 2005; Ménard and Quirion, 2012b). Our findings that aging-related reduction of Group 1 mGluR signal and function is rescued in Pdyn−/− mice suggest an intricate interaction between Pdyn and mGluR in aging. Indeed, the restoration of mGluR signal and function in Pdyn−/− mice seem to be responsible for the improvement of cognitive function in these mice because blocking mGluR5 using MPEP abolished the protective effect of knocking down Pdyn on spatial memory formation and EPM performance in old Pdyn−/− mice, and enhancing mGluR5 function using CDPPB alone improved spatial and recognition memory performance and reduced anxiety of old WT mice. Group 1 mGluR activation plays crucial roles in the formation of spatial (Lu et al., 1997; Balschun et al., 1999) and recognition memory (Barker et al., 2006; Christoffersen et al., 2008) that were rescued in old Pdyn−/− mice. Indeed, positive allosteric modulator of mGluR5 alone is sufficient to promote spatial (Balschun et al., 1999) and recognition memory formation (Reichel et al., 2011). In addition, we found that expression of downstream IEG signals of Group 1 mGluR, including Arc and Homer 1a, is increased in Pdyn−/− mice. Changes in MWM performance correlate with both Homer 1a and Arc expression, confirming the importance of these IEGs in spatial learning (Marrone et al., 2008). Homer 1a is an IEG expressed after neuronal activity (Ménard and Quirion, 2012a). It acts as a dominant-negative modulator by reducing mGluR5 coupling with its signaling effectors (Kammermeier and Worley, 2007). Exposure to novelty induces the transcription of Arc in the hippocampus (Luscher and Huber, 2010). Finally, increasing findings suggest that LTD mechanisms play positive roles in spatial (Ge et al., 2010) and recognition memory formation (Griffiths et al., 2008), suggesting imperative contribution of the rescue of mGluR-LTD in the cognitive performance of old Pdyn−/− mice. Notably, our findings do not rule out contribution of other forms of synaptic plasticity that are induced by nonmetabotropic glutamate receptors, such as NMDA receptors. Indirect enhancement of NMDA receptor function by allosteric potentiation of mGluR5 can enhance synaptic plasticity and performance on learning and memory tasks (Ayala et al., 2009; Rosenbrock et al., 2010). Potentiation of mGluR5 activation also reverses cognitive and motivational deficits induced by NMDA receptor antagonists or drugs of abuse (Cleva and Olive, 2011). CDPPB, for example, has been shown to rescue behaviors altered by the NMDA receptor antagonist MK-801 (Darrah et al., 2008). Interestingly, in our study, treatment with CDPPB improved novelty suppression of feeding in old WT mice. Moreover, in line with the inhibitory effect of MPEP on old Pdyn−/− distance traveled in the EPM open arms, mGluR5 KO mice showed increased anxiety with aging (Inta et al., 2013).
Although it remains unclear why aging-related decline in mGluR function is rescued in Pdyn−/− mice, interaction between dynorphinergic and glutamatergic transmission has been supported by previous findings. Dynorphin modulates glutamatergic transmission in a regional dependent manner, so that it inhibits postsynaptic glutamate responses of the perforant path in the dentate gyrus (Wagner et al., 1993) but enhances glutamate release in the CA2/CA3 region of the hippocampus (Faden, 1992). Alternatively, glutamate from perforant path afferents reduces dynorphin release in the dentate gyrus (Xie et al., 1991). On the receptor level, dynorphin has been shown to decrease NMDA receptor function (Chen et al., 1995). Interestingly, Group 1 mGluR activation enhances mRNA expression of preprodynorphin (Mao and Wang, 2001). Notably, these studies were performed almost exclusively in young and adult tissue. Further studies of the interaction between dynorphinergic and glutamatergic transmission could reveal mechanisms underlying the opposing changes of these two neurotransmitter systems in aging.
In conclusion, our findings suggest that reducing Pdyn expression protects aged mice from impairment of cognition and augmentation of anxiety-related behaviors. More importantly, these protective effects of reducing Pdyn expression could be mediated by the upregulation of Group 1 mGluR expression. These results identify Pdyn and Group 1 mGluR signaling as promising targets for drug discovery to prevent age-associated cognitive and behavioral deficits in normal and pathological aging.
Footnotes
This work was supported by Canadian Institutes of Health Research grants to R.Q. and T.P.W., C.M. is the recipient of a Fellowship from Canadian Institutes of Health Research. We thank Eve-Marie Charbonneau, Dr. Joseph Rochford, and Dr. Salah El Mestikawy for technical assistance and advice, and Dr. John Breitner for his scientific input.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to either Dr. Rémi Quirion or Dr. Tak Pan Wong, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Perry Pavilion, 6875 LaSalle Boulevard, Montréal, Quebec, Canada H4H 1R3, remi.quirion{at}frq.gouv.qc.ca or takpan.wong{at}mcgill.ca