Abstract
It remains unclear to what extent retinotopic maps can undergo large-scale plasticity following damage to human visual cortex. The literature has predominately focused on retinotopic changes in patients with retinal pathologies or congenital brain malformations. Yet, damage to the adult visual cortex itself is common in cases such as stroke, tumor, or trauma. To address this issue, we used a unique database of fMRI vision maps in patients with adult-onset (n = 25) and congenital (n = 2) pathology of the visual cortex. We identified atypical retinotopic organization in three patients (two with adult-onset, and one with congenital pathology) consisting of an expanded ipsilateral field representation that was on average 3.2 times greater than healthy controls. The expanded representations were located at the vertical meridian borders between visual areas such as V1/V2. Additionally, two of the three patients had apparently an ectopic (topographically inconsistent) representation of the ipsilateral field within lateral occipital cortex that is normally associated with visual areas V3/V3A (and possibly other areas). Both adult-onset cases had direct damage to early visual cortex itself (rather than to the afferent drive only), resulting in a mostly nonfunctional hemisphere. The congenital case had severe cortical malformation of the visual cortex and was acallosal. Our results are consistent with a competitive model in which unilateral damage to visual cortex or disruption of the transcallosal connections removes interhemispheric suppression from retino-geniculate afferents in intact visual cortex that represent the vertical meridian and ipsilateral visual field.
Introduction
Damage to visual cortex is a common occurrence in cases of stroke, tumor, and trauma. Although there is a long history of studying the spatial organization of visual field maps (e.g., retinotopic maps) after direct damage (Inouye, 1909; Holmes and Lister, 1916; Spalding, 1952; Horton and Hoyt, 1991), early studies did not focus on identifying plastic changes consistent with reorganization or unmasking. The discovery that plastic changes in visual cortex organization might be limited to an early critical period of development (Wiesel and Hubel, 1963; Hubel and Wiesel, 1970, 1977) led to a common assumption that all cortical connections in the adult visual cortex are fixed. More recently, this assumption has been challenged by evidence of plasticity in early visual cortex evoked by changes in normal experience (for review, see Gilbert and Li, 2012) and by damage to peripheral visual pathways, about which there is still considerable debate (for review, see Wandell and Smirnakis, 2009). Despite this interest, comprehensive studies of retinotopic changes following damage to adult central visual pathways are lacking.
Direct damage to visual cortex produces pathology-related deletions of retinotopic maps that can limit the ability to uniquely identify reorganization-induced changes. Consequently, our strategy in this study was to focus on identifying potential “additions” to cortical retinotopic maps. Additions may occur through rerouting or unmasking of visual input. Primary visual cortex of each hemisphere is normally dominated by the representation of the contralateral visual field with a minor representation of the ipsilateral visual field close to the vertical meridian (Tootell et al., 1998). Retinotopic additions of ipsilateral field (IPS) representation have been associated with gross developmental misrouting of retinal input, including the seminal study on albinism (Morland et al., 2001; Hoffmann et al., 2003; Klemen et al., 2012) as well as others in ahemispheric (Muckli et al., 2009) and achiasmatic (Hoffmann et al., 2012) patients, but have not been clearly identified in patients with adult-onset cortical damage.
Here, we capitalized on a unique database of fMRI visual field maps in 45 patients with diverse central brain pathologies (e.g., stroke, tumor, surgically induced pathology) to search for evidence of large-scale plasticity within the visual cortex. Our approach was twofold: (1) to qualitatively characterize the retinotopic organization of the ipsilateral field within visual cortex of each hemisphere; and (2) to quantitatively compare the size of the ipsilateral field representation between patients and healthy subjects. We identified a subset of patients with atypical organization of visual cortex (areas V1–V3/V3A) consisting of expanded ipsilateral visual field representations compared with healthy subjects. We then considered the possibility that such expansion might be consistent with a competitive model in which damage to visual cortex or the callosal connections removes interhemispheric suppression from retino-geniculate afferents in intact visual cortex that represent the vertical meridian and ipsilateral visual fields.
Materials and Methods
Subjects.
Forty-five patients (age range, 19–65 years; 23 females) with diverse brain pathology and six healthy subjects (age range, 25–48 years; 2 females) participated in this study. Patients with brain pathology that was suspected to involve the visual system were recruited over a period of 6 years by physician referral from hospitals within the Milwaukee Regional Medical Center of Wisconsin. Patients were prescreened and excluded if they (1) had a visual deficit caused by a combination of both retinal and central lesions, (2) were taking either α- or β-blockers that have a known effect on BOLD imaging, or (3) were unable to remain alert and attentive during the fMRI or perimetry protocols. Pathologic involvement of the visual cortex was confirmed in 27 (of the 45) patients using anatomical and functional MRI data collected within the study. These 27 patients (Table 1) were included for further analysis. Healthy subjects with normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity were recruited by word of mouth and/or advertising from the student and faculty pool at the Medical College of Wisconsin. Informed consent was obtained from all participants before scanning, in accordance with the procedures and protocols approved by The Medical College of Wisconsin internal review board.
Description of Patient Data Sets
Behavioral data.
Visual field perimetry was acquired for patients within 48 h of fMRI-based visual field mapping. Patients performed either a Humphrey Field Analyzer (HFA) test (10-2 or 30-2) administered by the referring physician or associate or a comparable Video Automated Perimetry (VAP) test (10-2 or 30-2) custom designed for the MRI environment and administered by the experimenter. As a result of the different types of visual field perimetry acquired, patients were tested either binocularly or monocularly. Binocular testing produced one visual sensitivity plot that represents the contribution of both eyes together. Monocular testing produced two separate visual sensitivity plots, each of which represents the contribution from one eye.
Retinal imaging and fixation tracking were performed with two patients having atypical retinotopic organization [patient 1 (P1) and P2] using an OPKO combined scanning laser ophthalmoscope (SLO) with optical coherence tomography (OCT) (Sabates et al., 2011). This follow-up study occurred 5 and 1.5 years after the initial fMRI study of P1 and P2, respectively. For each eye, patients were instructed to fixate a central crosshair and perform an HFA 10-2 (or HFA modified 10-2) equivalent perimetry task while the OPKO tracked fixation on the retina. The OPKO SLO is an internal fixation tracking device that uses a retinal landmark selected from the SLO fundus image (typically, prominent blood vessels), and continuous image capture with the SLO throughout the microperimetry examination to track eye movements and accurately place the stimulus in the same location for each presentation. Such internal fixation tracking devices have been shown to provide high repeatability and agreement in fixation stability assessment during microperimetry (Chen et al., 2011). The resulting data include a plot of fixation points on the SLO retinal image aligned with the OCT retinal images from which the following were calculated: (1) the centroid of fixation excluding points beyond 2° that appeared to be saccadic, (2) the mean distance and (3) SE of fixation points from the centroid, and (4) the distance between the centroid of fixation and the center of the foveal pit identified by OCT.
MRI acquisition and postprocessing.
Anatomical and functional MRI data were acquired on one of two MRI scanners: a General Electric Signa 3 tesla scanner with an in vivo 8-channel head coil (n = 22 patients, 6 healthy subjects), or a General Electric Signa 1.5 tesla scanner with an IGC Medical Advances mirrored bird cage head coil (n = 5 patients). Anatomical, T1-weighted, 3D-spoiled, gradient-recalled-at-steady-state images were acquired for the whole brain in the axial direction at a resolution of 0.96 × 1.07 × 1.00 mm [repetition time (TR) 9.5 ms; echo time (TE), 3.9 ms; flip angle, 12°]. Functional T2*-weighted gradient-recalled, echoplanar images were acquired in the axial plane at a resolution of 3.75 × 3.75 × 4.00 mm (for 3 T: TR, 2000 ms; TE, 30 ms; flip angle, 77°; for 1.5 T: TR, 2000 ms; TE, 40 ms; flip angle, 90°). Each functional scan was either 108 time frames (2 s TR) in duration, of which the first 6 and last 2 time frames (16 s) were discarded, or 84 time frames (2 s) in duration, of which the first 2 and last 2 time frames (8 s) were discarded. A total of six repeated scans (three scans for each of two tasks) were acquired within the same session for each subject.
fMRI postprocessing was performed with the AFNI analysis suite (Cox, 1996). Raw fMRI k-space data were converted to images using a custom reconstruction technique (Jesmanowicz et al., 1998). The resulting images were assembled into volumetric datasets, coregistered to reduce motion artifacts. Data from repeated stimulus tasks were then averaged across scans. For qualitative analyses only (i.e., visual display and visual area boundary identification), spatial smoothing was performed on the task-averaged time series data using a 6 mm spherical function.
Visual stimuli and identification of active voxels.
As described previously (DeYoe et al., 2011), conventional eccentricity and polar angle mapping were accomplished using temporal phase-mapped, counterphase flickering (8 Hz) checkered annuli and wedges (Engel et al., 1994, 1997; Sereno et al., 1995; DeYoe et al., 1996). Briefly, an annulus expanded from the center of gaze to the periphery over a period of 32 or 40 s. The expansion steps, inner and outer diameters, and check size were all scaled in proportion to the mean eccentricity of the annulus. A wedge (quarter or hemifield) rotated about the center of gaze over a period of 32 or 40 s. The annulus expansion or wedge rotation sequences were repeated five times in succession during each fMRI scan. Participants were instructed to fixate on a central green square and not move their eyes or head while viewing the stimulus sequences. Stimuli were viewed using either binocular stalks (∼9° viewing angle depending on optical focus) with an Avotec Silent Vision Visual System or a custom mirror and back-projection screen mounted on the MR head coil (∼21° viewing angle depending on head size/positioning) with images projected by an Avotec SV-6011 Projection Visual System or Brain Logics MR Digital Projection System. Either a Cambridge Research & Instrumentation VSG video board or custom Prism Acquire software (http://www.prismclinical.com) drove the projection systems.
The retinal position of the stimulus that optimally activated a particular cortical locus (voxel) was determined from the time delay of its fMRI response using the Hilbert Delay “plug-in” of the AFNI (Analysis of Functional NeuroImages) software package (Cox, 1996). To identify valid fMRI responses, the algorithm used a modification of a standard temporal cross-correlation method using a sinusoidal reference waveform (Bandettini et al., 1993; Saad et al., 2003), and efficiently identified the optimum temporal phase, the cross-correlation coefficient, and the covariance for each voxel. Active voxels were defined as those with a correlation coefficient ≥0.40. The statistical significance of each active voxel within a volume of interest (VOI) was computed automatically within AFNI and corrected for multiple comparisons using the false discovery rate q value. At a correlation coefficient of 0.40, the mean q value in our subjects was 0.0016 (SEM, ±00034).
Visual cortex volume of interest.
Prism View visualization and analysis software (http://www.prismclinical.com) was used to construct VOIs that generally included visual areas V1–V3. Three-dimensional VOIs were constructed on the task-averaged (nonsmoothed) functional data slice by slice using the anatomical data to define the VOI boundaries (Fig. 1). The VOIs were constrained by the medial, ventral, and posterior edges of occipital cortex, and the lateral ventricles and collateral sulcus. To avoid fMRI noise artifacts typically thought to be due to flow effects in venous structures along the parietal occipital sulcus (POS) and midsagittal sinus, VOIs followed the white/gray matter boundary or up to one to two voxels away from the POS in clear cases of drainage artifacts. To confirm that the VOIs accurately included V1–V3 and excluded higher visual areas, we later overlaid the masked datasets onto cortical surface models for two healthy subjects and five patients, and identified visual area boundaries based on retinotopic features (see Cortical surface model construction and visual area identification). Note that two main factors prevented the construction of VOIs based solely on visual area boundaries: (1) the required cortical surface models could not be constructed in many patient hemispheres due to extensive damage; and (2) some patient hemispheres with cortical surface models had insufficient retinotopy to identify visual area boundaries.
Anatomy and analysis VOIs for C1 and patients with atypical ipsilateral field representation (P1–P3). The most inferior slices are depicted on the left, and progressively more superior slices are depicted on the right. Slice number is indicated in the lower left corner of each image. Cortical tissue included in the VOIs of each hemisphere is outlined in red. L, Left; R, right; A, anterior; P, posterior.
Functional field maps.
We computationally “back-projected” the cortical fMRI activation patterns onto a chart of the observer's visual field. The resulting display, termed a “functional field map” (FFMap) shows which portions of the subject's visual field are capable of evoking a brain response. Individual FFMaps were constructed from all voxels within or intersecting the VOI mask (see above) that were active in both the eccentricity and polar angle tasks. The task-averaged data were used to determine the visual field location (eccentricity and polar angle) of the stimulus that maximally excited each visually responsive voxel within the VOI. For each active voxel, a circle symbol was computationally placed at the corresponding location on a schematic diagram of the visual field. The color of the symbol was selected from a scale representing the polar angle location in the visual field for that voxel. The diameter of each symbol was scaled to a 70% confidence interval for the true visual field location, based on a previous study by Saad et al. (2001) in which the temporal variability of fMRI responses was characterized. In that study, the SD in temporal phase delay across all responsive voxels in a sample from seven subjects was found to be 2.1 s. For a temporal phase-mapping paradigm with a 40 s cycle period, the 70% confidence interval for the estimated phase delay is thus 2.16 s. The diameter of the circle symbol was then adjusted accordingly to reflect the corresponding range in mean eccentricity of the annulus mapping sequence. (Note that the range scales with eccentricity so that the symbols are smaller near the center of gaze. Accuracy in the polar angle dimension also scales with eccentricity, so for simplicity the symbols have been plotted as circles. We also assume that the variance of fMRI phase measurements is approximately uniform throughout visually responsive cortex.)
Histograms and statistical analyses.
Polar histograms were created to show the volume of cortical tissue responding to each polar angle increment within the observer's visual field. Using a sliding boxcar window (width, 45°; step size, 9°), individual polar histograms were constructed for each hemisphere separately from all voxels in the VOI mask that were activated by the polar angle task. Bar histograms were created by binning the total volume of cortex responding to the left and right hemifields. Each mean and its SE were computed. In healthy subjects, two-sided Wilcoxon rank sum tests were used to test the hypothesis of no difference in the distributions of the volume of cortex activated by each hemifield between the left (n = 6) and right (n = 6) hemispheres. No significant difference was found in the ipsilateral hemifield between the left (1568 ± 235 mm3) and right (1428 ± 250 mm3) hemispheres (test statistic, W = 42, p = 0.6905) or in the contralateral hemifield between the left (14,661 ± 1996 mm3) and right (15,447 ± 970 mm3) hemispheres (W = 32, p = 0.3095). Thus, to obtain the best estimate of the normal amount of ipsilateral and contralateral field (CON) representation in any given hemisphere, regardless of side, we averaged the histograms of each healthy subject after “flipping” the right hemisphere histograms 180° across the vertical meridian. The resulting group-averaged histogram displays the ipsilateral field on the left and the contralateral field on the right. For comparison, group-averaged histograms for patient hemispheres with typical and atypical organization were produced in the same manner. To test for between-group differences in the distributions of the volume of cortex activated by each hemifield, we performed two-tailed Wilcoxon rank sum tests using the Matlab statistics toolbox (http://www.mathworks.com/help/stats/ranksum.html).
Cortical surface model construction and visual area identification.
Smoothed, task-averaged functional data were projected onto cortical surface models for five patients with retinotopic anomalies (three shown) and two healthy controls (one shown) using public domain software packages SureFit and Caret (http://brainmap.wustl.edu; Van Essen et al., 2001) or Freesurfer (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu). The smoothed surface maps were only used qualitatively for visual display and identification of visual areas to emphasize global patterns of topography. Visual areas were determined by manual identification of sign reversals in the representation of visual field polar angle. All quantitative analyses were performed on the unmodified (nonsmoothed) task-averaged volumetric data.
Results
Retinotopic maps in healthy subjects primarily represent the contralateral field
As a control condition, we performed fMRI-based visual field mapping in six healthy subjects to characterize the normal ipsilateral field representation. Figure 2 summarizes our findings for a representative healthy control subject (C1). Voxels that were activated by a rotating checkered wedge stimulus are color coded to show their preferred stimulus polar angle and displayed as an overlay on spherically inflated cortical surface models of each hemisphere (Fig. 2A,E). Both hemispheres were primarily activated by stimuli in the contralateral (opposite) visual hemifield but also had a small amount of activation extending along the vertical meridian into the ipsilateral (same-side) field. For instance, the left hemisphere (Fig. 2A) contained representations of the right visual field (red-oranges, yellow-greens) and the vertical meridian (white solid lines) with a very small representation of the ipsilateral field (blue-purples and darker greens). Notably, the ipsilateral field representation can be difficult to detect within visual areas V1–V3. Since spatial and temporal “smoothing” during common postprocessing methods may contribute to this effect, smoothed flat maps are only used here for display purposes, and all quantitative analyses (see below) were based on the unsmoothed 3D volumetric data. As an analysis tool, the normally small size of the ipsilateral field representation within healthy subjects enables any additions within patients to be particularly apparent.
Polar angle retinotopy for C1. A, E, Color-coded polar angle maps from the left (A) and right (E) hemispheres displayed on partially inflated (left) and spherical (right) cortical surface models (posterior pole: white star). Visual area boundaries are marked by representations of the horizontal (dotted white) and vertical (solid white) meridian representations. B, F, Functional field maps depicting a back-projection of the retinotopic maps from the left (B) and right (F) hemispheres onto maps of C1's visual field. Each circle in the FFMap is associated with a voxel in the brain that was maximally activated by a stimulus at the symbol's location in the visual field. The color of the symbols is as for A and E. C, G, Polar histograms depicting the amount (in cubic millimeters) of brain tissue (voxels) in the left (C) and right (G) hemispheres representing different angular positions in the visual field of C1 (gray shading) and averaged for all healthy controls (light blue shading, dotted lines show ±SEM). D, H, Bar graphs depicting the total amount of tissue representing each visual hemifield in the left (D) and right (H) hemispheres of C1 (gray shading) and averaged for all healthy controls (light blue shading; error bars indicate ±SEM). L, Left; R, right; RH, right hemisphere; LH, left hemisphere.
Retinotopic maps for this same subject showing voxels activated by the expanding ring stimuli and color coded by visual field eccentricity are depicted as an overlay on spherically inflated cortical surface models in Figure 3. Note the mirror symmetric representations of visual field eccentricity between hemispheres and the large cortical magnification of the center of gaze (reds, oranges) near the occipital pole (white star) compared with more peripheral eccentricities (greens, blues). To visualize relationships between the cortical retinotopic maps and the subject's visual field, we used the polar angle and eccentricity data to back-project the retinotopic maps from each hemisphere onto a map of the subject's visual field, as illustrated in Figure 2, B and F. Each circle symbol has a one-to-one relationship with a voxel in the early visual cortex VOI (Fig. 1, red outline, row C1) that was activated by both the rotating wedge and expanding ring stimuli. In correspondence with Figure 2, A and E, symbols are color coded by visual field polar angle. As expected, the resulting functional field maps showed a full representation of the contralateral visual field (within the tested eccentricities) in each hemisphere. The ipsilateral field locations that maximally activated voxels in visual cortex were also apparent. For instance, the left hemisphere functional field map (Fig. 2B) contained a fairly uniform distribution of symbols mostly restricted to the right side of the map and a small number of symbols extending into the ipsilateral field along the vertical meridian. Note that the diameter of each symbol reflects a 70% confidence interval for the true visual field location and are not an estimate of population receptive field size.
Eccentricity maps for healthy control (C1). Color-coded eccentricity maps from the left (top) and right (bottom) hemispheres of C1 displayed on spherical cortical surface models (posterior pole: white star). Visual area boundaries are as in Figure 2, A and E. RH, Right hemisphere; LH, left hemisphere.
To quantify the ipsilateral representation and its range of variability within healthy subjects, we converted the functional field maps into polar histograms that show the amount (in cubic millimeters) of brain tissue (voxels) representing different angular positions in the subject's visual field (Fig. 2C,G). The majority of early visual cortex in each hemisphere of C1 (Fig. 2C,G, gray shading) and the control group as a whole (Fig. 2C,G, light blue shading) was devoted to representation of the contralateral field, but a small amount of tissue was devoted to the ipsilateral field, particularly near the vertical meridian. Not only was the amount of activation in the contralateral field larger than in the ipsilateral field, but the variability also was larger (Fig. 2C,G, black dashed lines: ±SEM). Note that, to create the average polar histogram for control subjects (Fig. 2C,G, light blue shading), data were pooled from both hemispheres after “flipping” the field representation of one hemisphere. The same average histogram is shown in Figure 2, C and G, but is “mirrored” across the vertical meridian. The total amount of activation for each visual hemifield was summed and displayed as a bar graph (Fig. 2D,H). On average for the control group, 10% of the tissue in the early visual cortex VOIs represented the ipsilateral field (1519 ± 165 mm3) compared with the contralateral field (15,075 ± 1065 mm3).
Patients had diverse central pathologies, ages of onset, and retinotopic maps
From an initial dataset of 45 patients with brain pathology, 27 patients were selected with clear anatomical evidence of pathology involving visual cortex and/or underlying white matter tracts. As detailed in Table 1, 2 patients (P2, P27) had congenital cerebral malformations, and the remaining 25 patients had adult-onset pathologies including the following: arteriovenous malformation (AVM), epilepsy, stroke, tumor, trauma, and surgically induced collateral damage. The pathology resulted in direct damage to visual cortex within the VOI (generally V1–V3) in 15 patients [Table 1, direct damage (DD) = yes]. The involved hemisphere was functionally “disconnected” from the opposite hemisphere (e.g., it was mostly nonfunctional or had a developmental absence of a corpus callosum) in seven patients [Table 1, functional disconnection (Func Dis) = yes]. The mean patient age at the time of study was 39.7 years (SD = 12.6 years). The time between the onset of pathology (for known cases, n = 8) and the time of study ranged from 0.5 to 25 years (mean = 8.3 years, SD = 8.2 years).
Using the same fMRI-based analyses in patients as those used for the healthy control subjects (see above), the retinotopic organization of visual cortex was examined in all 27 patients for gross evidence of anomalies in the ipsilateral field representation. Initial screening was based on a manual, qualitative inspection of retinotopically encoded fMRI activation within each hemisphere of each patient independently. Briefly, we observed three different retinotopic outcomes in patients: (1) 3 patients had atypical [Table 1, atypical (type A)] retinotopic organization consisting of an expanded ipsilateral field representation compared with healthy controls; (2) another 22 patients had typical (type T) retinotopic organization compared with healthy subjects, except for the presence of retinotopic deletions related to their pathologies; and (3) the 2 remaining patients had evidence for pathology-related hemodynamic artifacts (type X) in their retinotopic maps, defined as BOLD activation that was not reflective of the underlying neural network, and were subsequently removed from further analysis. Both of the latter patients had pathology affecting the normal hemodynamics within visual cortex and, unlike type A patients, had clusters of topographically inconsistent fMRI activation in these regions of cortex that were equally affected in the eccentricity and polar angle task data (i.e., the delay values of voxels in each task were offset by the same amount).
A subset of patients with central damage had an expanded ipsilateral field representation
Patient 1: surgically induced collateral damage
P1 was a 37-year-old female with epileptic foci. She underwent a temporal–occipital resection of the right hemisphere including partial resection of the optic radiations (Fig. 1, void, row P1). Only a portion of medial visual cortex remained intact within the right hemisphere (Fig. 4E, black patch depicts damaged brain region) and was primarily activated by regions of the contralateral field (Fig. 4F, greens, cyans, blues) at peripheral eccentricities (Fig. 5A, bottom row). (Note that the reds and yellows on the cortical surface model in Fig. 4E appear to be a partial volume effect from the opposite hemisphere and were eliminated by the VOI mask used for the FFMaps and quantitative analysis). The FFMap in Figure 4F, a back-projection of the visual activation in the right hemisphere, corresponds to the central 9° of visual field perimetry in Figure 5B (blue dotted outline). The FFMap symbols correspond to regions of the visual field in which the patient's behavioral sensitivity to points of flashed light was degraded or absent (Fig. 5B, black patches in left hemifield). Given the extensive damage to the right hemisphere, visual activation may have been disconnected from behavioral awareness at higher stages in the visual pathway. Quantitatively, the amount of visual activation within this hemisphere was minute (Fig. 4G,H, gray shading) compared with normal controls (Fig. 4G,H, light blue shading), reflecting the effects of direct damage to that hemisphere.
Polar angle retinotopy for patient with surgical ablation of epileptic focus in right hemisphere (P1). A–H, Figure layout is the same as in Figure 2, with the following exceptions. A, Black dashed outlines indicate approximate location of expanded ipsilateral representation. B, Black dashed outlines highlight inset showing expanded ipsilateral representation especially near fovea. B, F, Blue dashed outlines encircle the regions corresponding with the visual field perimetry in Figure 5B. E, Dark shading indicates regions of brain damage. Location of V1 in the right hemisphere was estimated by fundus of calcarine sulcus. RH, Right hemisphere; LH, left hemisphere.
Eccentricity maps and perimetry for P1. A, Figure panel layout is the same as in Figure 3, except that the black dashed outline and visual area boundaries are as in Figure 4A, E. B, VAP 10-2 test (based on the Humphrey 10-2 perimetry) performed binocularly (both eyes together). Gray scale indicates the relative visual sensitivity from 0 to 1. Blue dashed outline encircles the region corresponding with the FFMaps in Figure 4, B and F. L, Left field; R, right field; S, superior field; I, inferior field; RH, right hemisphere; LH, left hemisphere.
This patient's left hemisphere was anatomically intact and contained a retinotopic map of the contralateral field (Fig. 4A, magentas, red-oranges, yellow-greens), but with the addition of an atypical ipsilateral field representation (purples, blues, and greens, highlighted by black dashed outline) involving the central visual field (Fig. 5A, top, reds, oranges, yellows). The atypical representation extended ∼1.5° into the left visual field along the vertical meridian (Fig. 4B) and corresponds to the patient's remaining behavioral sensitivity in the left hemifield near the visual midline (Fig. 5B, white strip). While the right hemisphere would normally support this region, we noted above that this patient's damaged right hemisphere did not contain a significant representation of this portion of the visual field (Fig. 4F). Quantitatively, the left hemisphere had a markedly larger representation of the ipsilateral (left) hemifield compared with healthy controls (Fig. 4C,D, left field, gray vs blue: 5850 vs 1519 ± 165 mm3). Moreover, this larger ipsilateral field representation was accompanied by a smaller contralateral (right) field representation relative to controls (Fig. 4C,D, right field, gray vs blue: 12,488 vs 15,075 ± 1065 mm3), even though this hemisphere was anatomically intact.
Patient 2: congenital cerebral malformation
P2 was a 19-year-old acallosal female with congenital cerebral malformation and chronic epilepsy. Both parietal and occipital lobes were hypergyrated and malformed around a central expanded ventricle/void (Fig. 1, row P2). In the right hemisphere, despite the grossly distorted cortical surface, the topography of the polar angle (Fig. 6E) and eccentricity (Fig. 7A, bottom row) remained largely intact and comparable to that in healthy subjects. Typical sign reversals in the polar angle representation allowed the delineation of dorsal visual areas V1–V3 and ventral visual areas V1–V2 (Fig. 6E), which together contained a representation of the entire contralateral (left) hemifield (Fig. 6F). Quantitatively, the relative distribution of activation across angular locations in the visual field (Fig. 6G) was comparable to controls, though the total amount of visual activation was smaller (Fig. 6H, gray vs blue). This difference in total volume may reflect an overall reduction in occipital cortical tissue as a result of the cerebral malformation.
Polar angle retinotopy for patient with congenital cerebral malformation (P2). A–H, Figure layout is the same as in Figure 2, with the following exceptions. A, Black dashed outlines indicate the approximate location of expanded ipsilateral representation. B, Black dashed outlines highlight inset showing expanded ipsilateral representation especially near fovea. B, F, Blue dashed outlines encircle the regions corresponding with the visual field perimetry in Figure 7B. RH, Right hemisphere; LH, left hemisphere.
Eccentricity maps and microperimetry for P2. A, Figure panel layout is the same as in Figure 3, except that the black dashed outline and visual area boundaries are as in Figure 6A, E. B, Monocular microperimetry plots (LE, left eye; RE, right eye) based on HFA modified 10-2 test presented with an OPKO microperimeter. The color scale for perimetry test points indicates the relative visual sensitivity, from 0 to 1. Perimetry data points are overlaid on SLO retinal images, which are oriented to correspond to visual field locations. Fixation scatter is indicated by group of yellow and blue + symbols. Blue dashed outlines encircle the regions corresponding with the FFMaps in Figure 6, B and F. L, Left field; R, right field; S, superior field; I, inferior field; RH, right hemisphere; LH, left hemisphere.
In contrast to the right hemisphere, the left hemisphere of P2 contained gross distortions of both the cortical surface (Fig. 1, row P2) and the retinotopic organization in the polar angle maps (Fig. 6A, black dashed outline). Only visual areas V1 and ventral V2–V3 could be estimated from sign reversals in the contralateral (right) visual field representation. In healthy individuals, the dorsal V1/V2 boundary is typically associated with a field sign reversal and a progression within V2 back toward the horizontal meridian (Fig. 2A). However, in P2, the map does not change sign but progresses (counterclockwise in Fig. 2) through an expanded representation of the lower right quadrant (red-oranges) with a seamless transition into the left hemifield inferior quadrant (purples, blues) and then into the left superior quadrant (cyans, greens). This atypical, yet coherent progression thus constitutes a complete, ordered transit through both hemifields. However, the ipsilateral representation was primarily associated with central eccentricities (Fig. 7A, black dashed outline). In the FFMap (Fig. 6B, black dashed outline), this appears as symbols populating the left central field except near the vertical meridian, where it expanded more peripherally. Since these visual field regions were also represented in the right hemisphere (Fig. 6, compare B, F), we were not able to determine unequivocally whether the atypical representation was contributing to the patient's visual sensitivity to these regions during microperimetry testing, as shown in Figure 7B by the green dots in the left hemifield that are superimposed onto images of the retina of this patient. (The microperimetry in Fig. 7B corresponds to the central 7° of the FFMaps in Fig. 6 B,F, indicated by the blue dotted outline. Retinal images are shown for the left and right eyes, tested separately, and have been oriented to reflect visual field space, as is typically done for conventional perimetry displays.) Quantitatively, the amount of ipsilateral (left) field activation in the left hemisphere was larger than observed in healthy controls (Fig. 6C,D, left field, gray vs blue: 3881 vs 1519 ± 165 mm3). As in the first patient, the enlarged representation of the ipsilateral field in this hemisphere was accompanied by a smaller representation of the contralateral field (Fig. 6C,D, right field, gray vs blue: 7256 vs 15,075 ± 1065 mm3).
Patient 3: brain tumor
Figure 8 summarizes the results for P3, a 52-year-old male with an anaplastic astrocytoma and compromised tissue in the left occipital, parietal, and temporal lobes (Figs. 1, row P3, 8A, white dashed outline). Since the extensive cortical damage prevented construction of a cortical surface model for the left hemisphere, anatomical MR images showing three views are displayed instead. The fMRI overlay is color coded by the visual field polar angle (Fig. 8A). A small region of cortex in the left hemisphere (Fig. 8A, green crosshairs) was activated appropriately by stimuli in the upper right visual quadrant between 12° and 21° in eccentricity (Fig. 8B). But, the patient responded unreliably to stimuli in this quadrant during perimetry testing (Fig. 9B). (The FFMap in Fig. 8B corresponds to the central 21° of the perimetry from Fig. 9B,F, as indicated by the blue dotted outlines. Perimetry is shown for the left and right eyes tested separately.) Quantitatively, the amount of visual activation within the left hemisphere was minute compared with normal controls (Fig. 8C,D, gray vs blue).
Polar angle retinotopy for patient with a brain tumor in left hemisphere (P3). A–H, Figure layout is the same as in Figure 2, with the following exceptions. A, Extensive left hemisphere damage prevented construction of a cortical surface model, so three anatomical views of color-coded polar angle maps are displayed. White dashed outline shows the region of cortical damage. L, Left; R, right; S, superior; I, inferior; A, anterior; P, posterior; RH, right hemisphere; LH, left hemisphere. E, Black dashed outlines indicate the approximate location of expanded ipsilateral representation. F, Black dashed outlines highlight the inset showing expanded ipsilateral representation, especially near the vertical meridian. B, F, Blue dashed outlines encircle the regions corresponding with the visual field perimetry in Figure 9B.
Eccentricity maps and perimetry for P3. A, Figure panel layout is the same as in Figure 3, with the following exceptions. A, Top row, Extensive left hemisphere damage prevented construction of cortical surface model, so three views of anatomical color-coded eccentricity maps are displayed. White dashed outline shows region of cortical damage. L, Left; R, right; S, superior; I, inferior; A, anterior; P, posterior. A, Bottom row, Black dashed outline and visual area boundaries as in Figure 8E. B, Monocular Humphrey 30–2 perimetry plots (LE, left eye; RE, right eye). Gray scale indicates the relative visual sensitivity, from 0 to 1. Blue dashed outlines encircle the regions corresponding with the FFMaps in Figure 8, B and F. L, Left field; R, right field; S, superior field; I, inferior field; RH, right hemisphere; LH, left hemisphere.
The right hemisphere of this patient was anatomically intact and contained a full retinotopic map of the contralateral field (Fig. 8E, purples, blues, greens) but with an apparently expanded representation of the ipsilateral field (Fig. 8E, reds, oranges). In contrast to P1 and P2, the ipsilateral field expansion appeared integrated within the normal vertical meridian representations of the contralateral field, rather than as a topographically displaced (ectopic) zone. This is perhaps more evident in the functional field map (Fig. 8F), which shows the expanded ipsilateral representation extending from the center of gaze to approximately 10° eccentricity in the lower right quadrant (black dashed outline) and clustered between 8° and 21° in eccentricity in the upper right quadrant. As already noted, the patient did not respond reliably to points of light flashed in the right visual field, though some degraded visual sensitivity may have remained (Fig. 9B, white patches in right hemifield). Quantitatively, the contralateral (left) visual field representation in the right hemisphere was comparable to healthy controls (Fig. 8G,H, left field, gray vs blue: 15,694 vs 15,075 ± 1065 mm3), but the ipsilateral (right) field representation was markedly larger than controls (Fig. 8G,H, right field, gray vs blue: 4838 vs 1519 ± 165 mm3).
Group-level analysis
We performed a quantitative, group-level analysis of the ipsilateral field representation to test the significance of the atypical organization in hemispheres from patients P1–P3 compared with healthy subjects (Fig. 10A,B, IPS, red vs light blue shading). On average, the amount of tissue activated by the ipsilateral field in hemispheres with atypical organization was 3.2 times larger than in healthy controls (Fig. 10B, IPS, red vs light blue shading: 4838 ± 572 vs 1519 ± 165 mm3). The distributions of the volume of cortex activated by the ipsilateral hemifield were significantly different between groups (two-tailed Wilcoxon rank sums, nA = 3, nC = 12, W = 42, p = 0.004). Moreover, no hemisphere of any individual healthy control had an ipsilateral representation as large as those in the atypical patients (controls, 788–2363 mm3; atypical patients, 3881–5850 mm3).
Fixation records for patients P1 and P2 with atypical cortical organization. A, B, Fixation samples (small blue + symbols) superimposed on SLO retinal images of P1 (A) and P2 (B). Red + symbol denotes the average fixation location during the 5–7 min sample period. Large white crosshairs denote the center of the foveal pit as determined by OCT images shown below and to the left and right of SLO images. Inner and outer white circles shows the 2°, and 4° radii from the foveal pit. S, Superior; I, inferior; T, temporal; N, nasal.
The amount of gray matter in human visual cortex can vary by at least twofold to threefold (Duncan and Boynton, 2003). We reasoned that hemispheres with a greater volume of visually activated cortex would have a generalized expansion of the polar histogram such that both the ipsilateral and contralateral representations would be expanded. Conceivably, our three patients with retinotopic anomalies might have had larger brains. However, it is important to point out that the polar histograms from hemispheres with retinotopic additions (red shading) did not demonstrate a uniform scaling compared with healthy controls (light blue shading), but rather a differential positive scaling of the ipsilateral field and negative scaling of the contralateral representation. On average, the total amount of visually activated cortex (ipsilateral plus contralateral fields) was 16,651 ± 2837 mm3 in atypical patients and 16,594 ± 1133 mm3 in healthy controls. The distributions of the total volume of activated cortex were not significantly different between groups (two-tailed Wilcoxon rank sums, nA = 3, nC = 12, W = 24.5, p = 0.9626). Thus, there was no single multiplicative scaling factor that could be applied to the histograms from anomalous hemispheres that would make them comparable to those of the healthy controls.
Additionally, our results were not dependent upon minor differences that might have occurred in the VOI boundaries between atypical patients and healthy controls. To confirm that the VOIs in healthy controls were not “missing” tissue activated by the ipsilateral field (e.g., in a higher visual area) that might have been included in atypical patients, the same set of analyses described previously were repeated in healthy controls except using expanded VOIs that included all of visual cortex. We observed a scaling of the total amount of visual activation in both the ipsilateral and contralateral visual field, rather than a selective increase of the ipsilateral field. Particularly, the amount of tissue activated by the ipsilateral field in controls with expanded VOIs was 2081 ± 285 mm3 compared with the 4838 ± 572 mm3 in atypical patients (Fig. 10A,B, IPS, dark blue vs red). The distributions of the volume of cortex activated by the ipsilateral field remained significantly different between groups (two-tailed Wilcoxon rank sums, nA = 3, nC = 12, W = 41, p = 0.009).
Majority of patients with central damage had typical retinotopic organization
The remaining 22 patients had typical retinotopic organization compared with healthy subjects except for the presence of retinotopic deletions caused by their pathologies. While hemispheres with direct damage did contain “deletions” within their retinotopic maps, the topography for the remainder of the map was comparable to healthy controls. As an alternate control group, an averaged polar histogram was computed from the hemispheres of patients with typical retinotopic organization that did not contain direct pathology (Fig. 10C,D, typical patients shaded light yellow). Compared with healthy controls, patient hemispheres with typical organization had a generally smaller amount of tissue activated by both the ipsilateral and contralateral fields (Fig. 10D, light yellow vs blue; IPS: 1242 ± 173 vs 1519 ± 165 mm3; CON: 10,164 ± 757 vs 15,075 ± 1065 mm3). While the distributions of the volume of cortex activated by the ipsilateral field were not significantly different between groups (two-tailed Wilcoxon rank sums, nC = 12, nT = 22, W = 247, z = 1.318, p = 0.187), the distributions of the volume of cortex activated by the contralateral field differed significantly between groups (two-tailed Wilcoxon rank sums, nC = 12, nT = 22, W = 303, z = 3.334, p = 0.001).
We predicted that visual cortex in typical patients may tend to be less responsive during the fMRI task, regardless of visual hemifield. We hypothesized that, if this were the case, the differences between typical patients and healthy controls could be removed by applying a single scaling factor to both the ipsilateral and contralateral field representations of typical patients. To test this hypothesis, we normalized the contralateral field histogram of typical patients to that of healthy controls. This required a scaling factor of ∼1.5, which was then applied to the ipsilateral field histogram of typical patients. After this step, no significant differences remained between the typical patients (Fig. 10C,D, normalized typical patients, dark yellow; IPS: 1842 ± 257 mm3; CON: 15,075 ± 1123 mm3) and healthy controls in the distributions of the volume of cortex activated by each hemifield (two-tailed Wilcoxon rank sums, nC = 12, nTN = 22; IPS: W = 198, z = −0.433, p = 0.665; CON: W = 213, z = −0.090, p = 0.928). This suggests that most patients with pathology of one hemisphere tended to have topographically normal retinotopic organization in the unaffected hemisphere, though with uniformly less fMRI activation overall.
Last, almost none of the typical patient cases were anatomically and functionally comparable to those of atypical patients. To review, all three atypical patients had evidence for a substantial functional “disconnection” of the vertical meridian representations between hemispheres, due to either (1) a mostly nonfunctional visual cortex (evidenced by the lack of BOLD-fMRI activation and visual field perimetry response) and direct damage within the visual cortex VOI (P1 and P3), or (2) the developmental absence of a corpus callosum (P1). While five patients (P4–P9) with typical retinotopic organization contained a hemisphere that was mostly nonfunctional (Table 1, Func Dis = yes), the pathology in three of the five patients (P6–P9) did not result in direct damage within the visual cortex VOI (Table 1, DD = no). Rather, these patients had damage to the LGN and/or optic radiations that removed the afferent drive to visual cortex. In the other two of five patients (P4 and P5), the pathology resulted in direct damage within the visual cortex VOI (Table 1, DD = yes). Interestingly, though, both of these patients had ipsilateral field representations (P4, 3263 mm3; P5, 3431 mm3) that trended toward atypical: they were substantially larger than that of any healthy subject (788–2363 mm3), though not as large as that of any patient hemisphere in our atypical group (3881–5850 mm3). These findings suggest that simply removing the retino-geniculate input may not be sufficient to acquire an expanded ipsilateral field representation. In such cases, the remaining intrinsic activation within the intact cortex may be sufficient to prevent initial unmasking of the ipsilateral field. Therefore, although two hemispheres can appear to be nonfunctional because they lack fMRI response and the patients have a hemianopia, their potential for expansion of the ipsilateral field representation may be quite different given the underlying pathology/anatomical involvement.
Expanded ipsilateral field representation in atypical patients was not accounted for by eccentric fixation
An important concern is that abnormal fixation in patients with visual pathology could cause artifactual distortions in visual field maps measured with fMRI. Use of a nonfoveal region for fixation, termed an eccentric preferred retinal locus (PRL), is a common adaptive strategy of patients with central visual field deficits. The PRL allows them to place more of the scene of interest within the intact portion of their visual field (for review, see Crossland et al., 2011). While a normal subject will use their central fovea to fixate the center of a visual display, a patient that uses a PRL to the right of their fovea will rotate their eyes to the left to place their PRL at the center of the display. This creates an asymmetrical representation of the visual display on the retina and, consequently, could create an apparent asymmetry in the measured cortical representation (Baseler et al., 2002).
The use of an eccentric PRL, however, is not consistent with the results of our study. Patients can be classified as having predominately normal central fixation if >50% of their preferred fixation points fall within 2° of the fovea (Fujii et al., 2003). We used an OPKO combined SLO with OCT and microperimetry to track fixation on the retina of two patients with atypical organization (the third patient was no longer available) and found that both had predominately normal central fixation (70–81% of fixation points fell within a 2° diameter of the fovea). More specifically, for both eyes of P1, the centroid of fixation (Fig. 11A, red + symbol) was 0.64° to the left of the central foveal pit (white crosshair) identified in OCT. The average distance of fixation points (blue + symbols) from the centroid was 0.38 ± 0.06° in the left eye and 0.46 ± 0.11° in the right eye. For P2, the centroid of fixation (Fig. 11B, red + symbol) was 0.33° to the left of the central foveal pit (white crosshair) in the left eye and 0.27° to the right in the right eye. The average distance of fixation points (blue + symbols) from the centroid was 0.66 ± 0.05° in the left eye and 0.75 ± 0.05° in the right eye. Note that the direction of fixation in the right eye of P2 is in the opposite direction to that which, if large enough, could theoretically create an artifactual asymmetry involving an expanded representation of the left hemifield in the left hemisphere (as observed in this patient).
Patients with atypical cortical organization have larger ipsilateral field representations. A, B, Group-averaged polar histograms and bar graphs for hemispheres from patients with atypical retinotopic organization (red shading, n = 3), healthy controls (light blue shading, n = 12), and healthy controls with expanded VOIs including all of visual cortex (dark blue shading, n = 12). Black dashed lines in the inset in A and error bars in B show ±SEM. C, D, Group-averaged polar histograms and bar graphs for hemispheres from healthy controls (light blue shading, n = 12), patients with typical retinotopic organization (yellow, n = 22), and patients with typical retinotopic organization that have been normalized to match controls in the total volume of activation (dark yellow, n = 22). Note: different y-axis scales in B and D for the ipsilateral versus contralateral visual fields.
In sum, our data suggest that the ipsilateral field expansions were not the result of eccentric PRLs. While we cannot exclude the possibility that P1 and P2 used a fixation locus during the fMRI experiments different to what was measured here, we have used high-accuracy retinal imaging/tracking to provide the best estimate of their average locus of fixation while fixating a central marker, as they would within the fMRI scanner environment. We found that the center of mass of fixation in both patients was located within the confines of the foveal pit.
Interhemispheric competition: a mechanism for atypical organization?
As outlined in Figure 12, we can begin to understand our results within the framework of a competitive model that is based on the following four factors. (1) Retino-geniculate afferents to V1 have a region of overlap between hemispheres resulting in a dual representation (one in each hemisphere) of the vertical meridian plus a strip of the ipsilateral field. (2) Normally, interhemispheric connections passing through the corpus callosum simply continue the effects of connectivity that are present throughout the rest of visual cortex. Overall, the net effects of horizontal intracortical connectivity are generally suppressive. (3) Unilateral damage to visual cortex, or the transcallosal connections themselves, relieves the suppression at the “edge” of the visual field representation in the intact hemisphere. Since for V1 this edge represents the vertical meridian and adjacent ipsilateral field, the loss of tonic suppression allows it to expand. In the following section, each of these factors is considered in more detail.
Conceptual model of interhemispheric suppression and effects of unilateral damage on apparent retinotopy. A, Schematic of the retino-geniculate afferent pattern into V1, including a naso-temporal overlap zone (highlighted by red lines) providing a source of ipsilateral visual field input to V1 in each hemisphere. B, Tonic suppression along the edge of each hemisphere (left V1 only shown for clarity) reduces the apparent number of retino-geniculate afferents (black dashed curve) along the naso-temporal overlap zone. Inset: the effect of a loss of suppression is an increase in the apparent number of afferents. C, Schematic model of flattened visual cortex highlighting the V1/V2 border at the representation of the ipsilateral field (thin contrasting colored strip). Compared with healthy visual cortex (top), unilateral damage (bottom) to the right hemisphere causes expansion of the visual field representation near the vertical meridian, including the ipsilateral field.
The retino-geniculate afferent pattern (Fig. 12A) provides a small source of ipsilateral visual field representation within V1. This arises from a retinal zone along the vertical meridian (Fig. 12A, top, red lines) that contains a mixture of ganglion cells projecting either ipsilaterally (more temporally located cells) or contralaterally (more nasally located cells) through the optic chiasm (Stone et al., 1973; Bunt and Minckler, 1977; Bunt et al., 1977; Fukuda et al., 1989). The overlap zone (black dashed outline) spans ∼0.5° in radius near the fovea, extending to 1.5° more peripherally. Due to the naso-temporal overlap and the topographic nature of the retino–geniculo–striate pathway, the afferent pattern within V1 (Fig. 12A, bottom, red lines) should maintain a bilateral representation of the visual field vertical meridian. Accordingly, single-unit recording in the baboon identified an ipsilateral representation out to 2° within the first few millimeters of both V1 and V2 along the vertical meridian border (Kennedy et al., 1985). In Figure 12A, we modeled the afferent pattern to V1 as a smoothly decreasing, spatial gradient representing the contralateral visual field and adjacent ipsilateral field.
Within visual cortex, bilaterally represented regions of the visual field are interconnected through interhemispheric projections (Choudhury et al., 1965; Hubel and Wiesel, 1965; Zeki, 1970; Berlucchi, 1972; Sanides, 1978; Essen and Zeki, 1978; Newsome and Allman, 1980). Functionally, these interhemispheric connections have been hypothesized to reiterate the effects of horizontal connections and/or lateral spread of extrastriate feedback normally present throughout cortex (Van Essen et al., 1982). This characterization reflects the concept that interhemispheric connections are functionally comparable to intrahemispheric connections in that both are cortico–cortico pathways that have similar laminar distributions in primates (largely originating from layer III and terminating in layer IV and above) and a remarkable degree of fine structure in spatial distribution parallel to the cortical surface. Like interhemispheric horizontal connections, feedback projections also spread laterally and can cross via the corpus callosum to terminate in the opposite hemisphere. Therefore, we consider the cortical tissue in each hemisphere as effectively forming a single functional unit that is fully interconnected by a uniform network involving intrahemispheric and interhemispheric connections in a similar fashion.
Overall, the net effects of intrahemispheric connections are generally suppressive. It is typically thought that receptive fields of visual cortex neurons are organized into two or more spatially concentric zones (not necessarily circular) often referred to as the center, “near” surround, and “far” (or nonclassical) surround (Allman et al., 1985; Angelucci and Bressloff, 2006). Though interactions among these regions can be complex, it is common for the surrounds to exert an antagonistic (e.g., suppressive) effect on the center response, especially for stimulus patterns that extend uniformly over both center and surround regions. Indeed, recent recordings of synaptic conductances in awake (but not anesthetized) animals suggest that cortical responses to visual stimulation are typically dominated by synaptic inhibition that has “extremely broad spatial selectivity” (Haider et al., 2013). Such “suppressive” interactions can be mediated by a variety of quite general neurophysiological mechanisms, including lateral and feedback inhibition (Hartline, 1949; Kuffler, 1953; Angelucci and Bressloff, 2006), divisive normalization (Carandini and Heeger, 1994, 2012), or other nonlinear processes (Priebe and Ferster, 2012). A key feature of these mechanisms is often that the suppressive effects appear to be driven from surround distances that extend at least as far (Stettler et al., 2002) or farther than the distances covered by the center excitatory interactions. For instance, divisive normalization is based on the notion that each neuron's center response is “divided” by the activity of neighboring neurons pooled over an extended “surround.” Similarly, “biased competition” models of attention posit ubiquitous tonic suppression among arrays of multiple stimuli that is relieved only when one stimulus gains a competitive edge through attention (Desimone and Duncan, 1995; Luck et al., 1997). To the extent that such spatial interactions must be mediated in some fashion by laterally spreading connections either within or between cortical visual areas, it follows from above that these interactions also generally spread between the hemispheres.
Of particular interest here is the zone of interhemispheric suppression along the edge of the visual field representation in each hemisphere (the bilaterally represented regions). In a healthy intact brain, the resulting effects of a tonic suppression along the edge of each hemisphere would be to decrease the apparent number of retino-geniculate afferents along the vertical meridian (Fig. 12B, black dashed curve) compared with that of the retino-geniculate afferents alone (Fig. 12B, solid black curve).
Based on this mechanism, the third and final factor posits that unilateral damage to visual cortex, or pathology of the corpus callosum, would disrupt the tonic suppression and cause retinotopic unmasking (increases in activation) along the vertical meridian representation (Fig. 12B, inset). Topographically, this would appear as an expansion of the ipsilateral visual field representation (yellow) plus an expansion of an adjacent zone of the contralateral field (blue). Figure 12C shows this effect in a schematic model of flattened visual cortex (Polimeni et al., 2006; Schira et al., 2007) that highlights the V1/V2 border at the representation of the vertical meridian. This competitive model is consistent with the retinotopic expansion of the existing vertical meridian representations observed in V1/V2 of P1–P3 as well as V3/V3A of P1 and P2. Additionally, patients P1 and P2 had an apparently ectopic (topographically displaced) representation of the ipsilateral visual field within lateral occipital cortex that is normally associated with visual areas V3/V3A (and possibly other areas). Interhemispheric competition within extrastriate cortical areas including crossed feedback projections from hierarchically higher to lower cortical levels may provide a functionally similar mechanism, but such speculations are reserved for the Discussion.
While the elimination of interhemispheric competition might constitute one plausible mechanism for the ipsilateral expansion observed here, other mechanisms might also contribute. Ample evidence suggests that the receptive fields of cells within a zone of deafferented visual cortex can undergo activity-dependent plastic reorganization (axonal sprouting and synaptogenesis), resulting in atypical cortical retinotopic patterns in which the receptive fields of initially deafferented neurons become responsive to stimuli presented near the margins of the scotoma. Such reorganization can be associated with the strengthening of long-range excitatory connections that normally support perceptual effects such as contour integration but after reorganization may support perceptual “filling-in” of contours crossing a scotoma (Gilbert and Li, 2012). In the present context, though, neurons representing the midline in the intact hemisphere are not deprived of their afferent input; they have lost only their “lateral” and feedback connections from the opposite hemisphere. It is not clear whether this would be sufficient to trigger strengthening of the remaining horizontal excitatory connections within the intact hemisphere. If it did, the anticipated effect would be to “pull” the receptive fields toward the intact contralateral hemifield, not the disconnected/lesioned ipsilateral field.
Discussion
We used a unique database of fMRI vision maps in patients with central brain pathology to identify anomalies in visual cortex organization elicited by adult (n = 27) as well as congenital pathology (n = 2). Of these patients, we identified three with atypical retinotopic organization consisting of an expanded ipsilateral visual field representation when compared with healthy controls. This pattern was observed in patients with adult-onset damage (n = 2) as well as congenital cerebral malformation (n = 1). In two of these cases (one adult-onset, one congenital), an ectopic or topographically displaced representation of the ipsilateral field was apparent. Our results suggest that visual cortex can undergo large-scale retinotopic changes elicited by adult and congenital pathologies. Our large sample of patients having a variety of different pathologies allows us to estimate the rate of incidence for this phenomenon and suggests that it is relatively rare (∼10%) among patients with central visual pathology in general.
To our knowledge, this is the largest investigation to date of fMRI-based retinotopic mapping in human patients with central damage to the visual cortex. Our results add to previous claims of retinotopic reorganization within a local zone of deafferented cortex, as can occur within primary visual cortex after peripheral binocular retinal pathologies (Baseler et al., 2002; Baker et al., 2005, 2008) or damage to the optic radiations (Dilks et al., 2007), and within extrastriate cortex after selective damage to primary visual cortex (Baseler et al., 1999). In such cases, the deafferented region of cortex [termed the lesion projection zone (LPZ)] becomes responsive to visual input from surrounding portions of the visual field. This form of reorganization is associated with axonal sprouting and synaptogenesis of long-range horizontal connections within cortex adjacent to the LPZ (Darian-Smith and Gilbert, 1994; Yamahachi et al., 2009). However, patients in the present study differ from such patients with peripheral pathologies in three ways. (1) Many patients in our study have direct damage to visual cortex. In contrast, patients with peripheral pathologies have an intact visual cortex that has been partially deafferented. (2) Our patients with atypical organization have responses to the ipsilateral visual field within cortex that would normally represent the contralateral visual field. (3) We observed local expansions of the visual field that were consistent with normal retinotopy and, in two patients, additional ipsilateral field representations that appeared to be ectopic. Patients with peripheral pathologies typically gain responses within the LPZ to retinotopic locations that are normally represented in cortex surrounding the LPZ. In other words, the expansion is topographically consistent with the normal pattern.
Ectopic representation of the ipsilateral visual field has been previously observed in cases of developmental misrouting of the optic nerve/tract in patients that were albinotic (Morland et al., 2001; Hoffmann et al., 2003; Klemen et al., 2012), ahemispheric (Muckli et al., 2009), and achiasmic (Hoffmann et al., 2012). In contrast to patients in our study, patients with peripheral misrouting have an ipsilateral field representation that is coextensive with the normal contralateral field representation within a hemisphere. Thus, voxel (population) receptive fields have bilateral responses, perhaps from two different populations of neurons—one responding to the normal contralateral input and the other responding to the atypical ipsilateral input. There have also been reports of activation in the ipsilateral extrastriate cortex in patients with hemispherectomy (Bittar et al., 1999) and stroke (Nelles et al., 2007) as well as along the V1/V2 border in a patient who underwent visual restoration training (Henriksson et al., 2007). However, these studies did not map the topographic organization, if any, of the ipsilateral representation or, in the latter case, was a control group included to assist in the interpretation of the findings.
As detailed above, our results can be understood within the framework of a competitive model in which unilateral damage (or pathology of the corpus callosum) acts to remove interhemispheric suppression from the intact visual cortex and thereby unmask retino-geniculate afferents representing the vertical meridian and ipsilateral visual field (Fig. 12). Similar types of interactions may account for the displaced ipsilateral field representations that we observed in lateral occipital cortex normally associated with visual areas V3/V3A (and possibly other areas). In healthy individuals, it has been shown that under appropriate conditions these regions respond to isolated stimulation of the ipsilateral field even though conventional visual field mapping shows a dominant contralateral field representation (Tootell et al., 1998). Though such ipsilateral influences might be conveyed by callosal input, extrastriate visual areas (particularly dorsal stream) in primates, and probably in humans, also receive retinal input via a colliculo–pulvinar route that may be either modulatory or driving (for review, see Kaas and Lyon, 2007). For instance, evidence exists for activation in V2 and V3A that does not depend on V1 (Girard and Bullier, 1989; Girard et al., 1991). While little is known about the degree of midline overlap within the pulvinar, receptive fields within a region of lateral pulvinar were found to frequently overlap the ipsilateral hemifield by up to 4.8° among receptive fields centered within the central 7° of the fovea (Bender, 1981; Abbott, 2009). It is conceivable that the degree of midline overlap within the pulvinar is larger than for the geniculostriate input but is normally suppressed in the cortex by the more extensive and dominant contralateral representation. Moreover, there are multiple intrahemispheric pathways that could distribute such alternative sources of ipsilateral input including feedback circuits at each cortical stage, which, if “unmasked,” might appear as an apparently ectopic ipsilateral field representation.
Finally, we cannot rule out the possibility that unique patterns of cortical damage might enable experience-dependent reorganization in ways that differ from those associated with peripheral damage. For instance, perceptual learning is associated with changes in receptive field properties within early visual cortex (e.g., orientation tuning: Leidig et al., 1992; Darian-Smith and Gilbert, 1994; Schoups et al., 2001) and increases in activation (e.g., contrast discrimination: Girard and Bullier, 1989; texture discrimination: Galea and Darian-Smith, 1994). Similar processes may occur in patients with cortical damage that engage in visual rehabilitative therapies, which attempt to expand the size of the residual visual field. Claims have been made for success in some, but not all, patients (for review, see Pouget et al., 2012). Definitive cortical mapping studies with such patients have been lacking, so it is unclear whether there are distinctive patterns of cortical damage that differentiate cases of successful versus unsuccessful therapy. It is possible that the success or failure of rehabilitation for each particular patient may depend on the specific pattern of cortical damage and its interaction with mechanisms such as interhemispheric competition that, when disrupted, may mediate visual field expansions. Though the expanded size of the ipsilateral field measured in this study may seem relatively small, an increase of only a few degrees near the fovea could make a major difference in critical visual functions that are predominately foveal, such as reading. Understanding the conditions under which visual field expansions can occur, and careful consideration of the neural mechanisms that might be involved, could lead to greater insight into the potential for effective visual rehabilitation.
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health (NIH) Grants RO1EB000843 and R42CA113186 to E.A.D. and R01EB007827 to J.H. We thank Joseph Carroll and Adam Dubis for help with retinal imaging experiments, which were supported by the Thomas M. Aaberg, Sr, Retina Research Fund and NIH Grant P30EY001931. We also thank Bernd Remler and Lofti Hacien-Bey for additional patient referrals.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Danielle C. Reitsma, The Medical College of Wisconsin, 8701 Watertown Plank Road, Milwaukee, WI 53226. dreitsma{at}mcw.edu