Abstract
One of the key brain regions in cognitive processing and executive function is the prefrontal cortex (PFC), which receives cholinergic input from basal forebrain cholinergic neurons. We evaluated the contribution of synaptically released acetylcholine (ACh) to executive function by genetically targeting the vesicular acetylcholine transporter (VAChT) in the mouse forebrain. Executive function was assessed using a pairwise visual discrimination paradigm and the 5-choice serial reaction time task (5-CSRT). In the pairwise test, VAChT-deficient mice were able to learn, but were impaired in reversal learning, suggesting that these mice present cognitive inflexibility. Interestingly, VAChT-targeted mice took longer to reach criteria in the 5-CSRT. Although their performance was indistinguishable from that of control mice during low attentional demand, increased attentional demand revealed striking deficits in VAChT-deleted mice. Galantamine, a cholinesterase inhibitor used in Alzheimer's disease, significantly improved the performance of control mice, but not of VAChT-deficient mice on the 5-CSRT. In vivo magnetic resonance spectroscopy showed altered levels of two neurochemical markers of neuronal function, taurine and lactate, suggesting altered PFC metabolism in VAChT-deficient mice. The PFC of these mice displayed a drastic reduction in the splicing factor heterogeneous nuclear ribonucleoprotein A2/B1 (hnRNPA2/B1), whose cholinergic-mediated reduction was previously demonstrated in Alzheimer's disease. Consequently, several key hnRNPA2/B1 target transcripts involved in neuronal function present changes in alternative splicing in VAChT-deficient mice, including pyruvate kinase M, a key enzyme involved in lactate metabolism. We propose that VAChT-targeted mice can be used to model and to dissect the neurochemical basis of executive abnormalities.
Introduction
The prefrontal cortex (PFC) is essential for the modulation of executive function, which is loosely defined as a set of cognitive tools that allows hierarchical and timely control of actions leading to specific behaviors (Alvarez and Emory, 2006; Robbins and Roberts, 2007; Chudasama, 2011). Disruption in executive function is a key symptom in neurological and neuropsychiatric disorders, including Alzheimer's disease (AD; Perry and Hodges, 1999; Traykov et al., 2007; McGuinness et al., 2010), schizophrenia (Morice, 1990), autism (Hill, 2004; Sala et al., 2011), and drug addiction (Stalnaker et al., 2009). The basic neurochemical underpinnings of executive function are, however, still poorly understood.
Cholinergic deficits are a hallmark of AD (Perry et al., 1977; Whitehouse et al., 1981, 1982). Moreover, amyloid β (Aβ) oligomers, potential toxins in AD, disrupt cholinergic synaptic transmission in the PFC (Chen et al., 2013). Cholinergic deficiency in AD can have widespread effects, including global changes in alternative splicing of genes involved in synaptic plasticity (Berson et al., 2012).
Acetylcholine (ACh) in the PFC has been implicated in controlling attention (Elliott, 2003; Jurado and Rosselli, 2007), one of the components of executive function. Cue detection and top-down modulation of attentive behavior have both been shown to activate PFC cholinergic activity and to be modulated by cholinergic signaling (Sarter et al., 2001; Parikh et al., 2007). Cholinergic transients in the PFC have been linked to cue detection, and both tonic and phasic PFC ACh release seem to regulate attentional demand (Parikh et al., 2007; for review, see Hasselmo and Sarter, 2011), which may depend mainly on nicotinic receptor signaling (McGaughy et al., 1999; Grottick and Higgins, 2000; Parikh et al., 2010; Guillem et al., 2011).
Cognitive flexibility, the ability to alter strategy according to changing environmental cues, is another key component of executive function (Elliott, 2003; Jurado and Rosselli, 2007). The neurochemical basis of cognitive flexibility is not fully understood, but serotonin is thought to play critical roles (Schmitt et al., 2006; Evers et al., 2007; Brigman et al., 2010).
Release of ACh is a tightly regulated process, with the vesicular acetylcholine transporter (VAChT) controlling a limiting key step (de Castro et al., 2009a; Kolisnyk et al., 2013; for review, see Prado et al., 2013). Genetic elimination of VAChT from the forebrain causes deficits in reversal learning assessed using the Morris Water Maze (MWM) (Martyn et al., 2012). Deficits in reversal learning may be related to hippocampal dysfunction, but could also reflect alterations in behavioral flexibility. To examine if decreased levels of VAChT, a change observed in AD (Efange et al., 1997; Chen et al., 2011), affects executive function we have used touchscreen tasks. We report that elimination of forebrain VAChT caused severe deficits in cognitive flexibility and in sustained attention. In addition, we found that these mutant mice have profound changes in RNA processing in the PFC, which correlate with behavioral and metabolic deficits. Our results suggest that elimination of forebrain cholinergic activity in mice provides a model for understanding the neurochemical basis of executive function.
Materials and Methods
Animals.
Generation of VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice was previously described (Martyn et al., 2012). In short, VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice were generated by crossing VAChTflox/flox (mixed C57BL/6J × 129/SvEv background, backcrossed to C57BL/6J for five generations) with the Six3-Cre mouse line (NMRI background, backcrossed to C57BL/6J for five generations). We then intercrossed VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/wt mice to obtain VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox. For the galantamine experiments, the mice used were wild-type C57BL/6J. Mice were housed in groups of three or four per cage without environmental enrichment in a temperature-controlled room with 14/10 h light/dark cycle, and water was provided ad libitum. Only male mice were used in these studies. Mice were restricted to 85% of their free-feed weight and maintained on 85% of their weight for the duration of the study. All procedures were conducted in accordance with guidelines from the Canadian Council of Animal Care at the University of Western Ontario with an approved institutional animal protocol (2008–127).
Western blotting.
Mouse PFC was collected, protein was isolated, and immunoblotting was performed as previously described (Martins-Silva et al., 2011). The antibodies used were anti-VAChT (catalog #139103; Synaptic Systems) at a 1:3000 dilution, anti-Synaptophysin (catalog #S5768; Sigma-Aldrich) at a 1:500 dilution, anti-hnRNP A2/B1 (catalog #sc-10035; Santa Cruz Biotechnology) at a 1:500 dilution, and anti-β-Actin (catalog #ab49900; Abcam), at a 1:15000 dilution. Band intensity was quantified using FluoroChemQ software (Thermo Fisher Scientific).
ACh release.
ACh release from prefrontal cortical brain slices was quantified by labeling slices with [3H] methyl-choline before using KCl to stimulate release of labeled ACh as previously described (Guzman et al., 2011).
qPCR.
To measure mRNA expression, total RNA was extracted from freshly dissected PFC tissue, using the Aurum Total RNA for fatty and fibrous tissue kit (Bio-Rad) according to the manufacturer's instructions. cDNA synthesis and qPCR analysis were performed as previously described (Guzman et al., 2011). Primer sequences used to determine alternative spliced transcripts are found in Table 1. β-Actin was used as a reference transcript for all reactions. For alternative splicing experiments, the alternative exon levels were normalized to a constitutively expressed exon from the same gene.
Magnetic resonance imaging.
Magnetic resonance spectroscopy of the prefrontal region was performed in four VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice and four littermates (VAChTflox/flox). Spectroscopic localization of a 24 μl voxel was achieved by adiabatic selective refocusing (DelaBarre and Garwood, 1998) on a 9.4 tesla horizontal bore small animal Agilent magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scanner. Water-suppressed full spectra (TR/TE = 3250/20 ms, 128 acquisitions), water-suppressed macromolecule spectra (TR/TE = 5000/20 ms, inversion time TI = 873 ms, 128 acquisitions), and water-unsuppressed spectra (TR/TE = 3250/20 ms, 8 acquisitions) were acquired. All animals were anesthetized with 2% isoflurane during the procedure and were maintained at 37° by blowing warm air into the bore of the magnet using a Model 1025 Small Animal Monitoring and Gating System (SA Instruments).
Metabolite analysis.
Magnetic resonance spectra were analyzed using purpose-built software (fitMAN; Bartha et al., 1999) to determine the contribution of each metabolite relative to total creatine as previously described (Bartha, 2007, 2008). Briefly, spectra were lineshape corrected (Bartha et al., 2000b) and the macromolecule and residual water contribution was removed (Kassem and Bartha, 2003). Then, the spectrum was fitted in the time domain to a basis set of 19 metabolite lineshapes (Pfeuffer et al., 1999; Bartha et al., 2000a). Five metabolites (measured relative to creatine) were reliably measured and included in group comparisons: N-acetylaspartate (NAA), myo-inositol (Myo), choline (Cho), taurine (Tau), and lactate (Lac).
Touchscreen behavioral assessment
Apparatus and task.
Mice were trained in the 5-choice serial reaction time task (5-CSRT) and in the pairwise visual discrimination in automated Bussey–Saksida Mouse Touchscreen Systems model 81426 (Campden Instruments). Schedules were designed and data were collected using the ABET II Touch software v.2.15 (Lafayette Instruments).
Pretraining.
Before being trained on the pairwise visual discrimination or the 5-CSRT task, mice were first put through a pretraining program. This consisted of first habituating the mice to the testing chamber with the lights off for 15 min. The next day, mice were left in the chamber with the lights off for 20 min, this time with the reward tray primed with a 150 μl reward (strawberry milkshake; Saputo Dairy Products), and a tone was played whenever the mouse entered the reward tray. Whenever the mouse returned to the reward tray, it received a reward (7 μl) paired with the tone. This training was repeated the next 2 d for 40 min sessions (phase 1).
The next training phase (phase 2) involved pairing the reward with the presentation of the stimulus on the touchscreen. The stimulus appeared randomly in one of the windows and after 30 s, it was removed and a reward (7 μl) was given paired with a tone. If the mouse touched the screen while the image was displayed, it immediately received a reward (7 μl). Once the mouse collected the reward a new trial was initiated. This phase was repeated until the mouse completed 30 trials within 60 min (phase 2).
Phase 3 was used to further shape behavior. It involved displaying the stimulus randomly in one of the windows. The mouse was required to touch the stimulus on the screen to receive a reward (7 μl) paired with a tone. There was no response to the mouse touching anything but the stimulus. Once again, this was repeated until the mouse completed 30 trials within 60 min. Phase 4 was identical to phase 3 except that the mouse was required to initiate each trial by nose poking the reward tray. Criterion was 30 correct trials within 60 min.
Finally, in the last pretraining phase (phase 5), the previous procedure was repeated but if the mouse touched an incorrect screen, it received a 5 s time-out, during which the light was turned on. The final phase had a stricter criterion, requiring the mouse to perform 30 trials in 60 min with at least 23 correct responses in 2 consecutive days.
Pairwise visual discrimination and reversal.
Pairwise visual discrimination and reversal task were performed as previously described (Romberg et al., 2013). Mice used in this experiment were 6–8 months old. At the beginning of each session, the reward tray was primed with 7 μl of milkshake. Briefly, the mice were first trained to discriminate between two visual stimuli, which were presented simultaneously, with their spatial location randomized over a 30 trial session. If mouse nose poked the correct stimulus (S+), a tone was played and mouse received a reward (7 μl), whereas if the incorrect stimulus (S−) was nose poked, light in the chamber was turned on for 5 s (time-out) followed by a correction trial. During the correction trial, the trial was repeated until the mouse poked the correct stimulus. Criterion was reached when the mouse selected the correct image (S+) on 80% of the trials, excluding correction trials, for 2 consecutive days. Once mice reached criteria, they were given two sessions to assess baseline performance on the task. For reversal learning the rule associated to each stimulus was switched, that is, the rewarded image (S+) during acquisition became the (S−) image in reversal and was punished, while the (S−) image from acquisition became the correct stimulus and was rewarded. Reversal learning was assessed over the course of 10 sessions.
Training in 5-CSRT.
The 5-CSRT task was performed as previously described (Romberg et al., 2011). A distinct cohort of mice (8–10 months old) was trained in the 5-CSRT task. At the beginning of each session, the reward tray was primed with 7 μl of milkshake. Each trial was initiated after the mouse nose poked the magazine. In this phase, the stimulus was delivered after a variable 5–10 s delay (delay period), during which the animal was required to attend to the screen. In case the mouse prematurely touched the screen during this delay, the response was recorded as premature and the mouse was punished with a 10 s time-out. The stimulus duration was initially set to 4 s, followed by a limited holding period of 5 s, during which the stimulus was absent, but the mouse could still respond to the location (holding period). Responses to the stimulus window during stimulus presence or the holding period were recorded as correct and a 7 μl reward was administered, while responses to any other window were recorded as incorrect. A correct choice was rewarded with a tone and food delivery. An incorrect response was punished with a 10 s time-out. A failure to respond to any window either during stimulus display, or during the holding period, was recorded as an omission and punished with a 10 s time-out. Perseverative responses to the screen after premature, correct, and incorrect choices were also recorded. Once the performance of a mouse reached criterion at 4 s stimulus duration (80% accuracy, 20% omissions for 3 consecutive days), the stimulus duration was reduced to 2 s. After reaching criterion with the 2 s stimulus, mice were tested for two more days. The mean measurement of those 2 d was used to assess baseline performance. If the mouse completed <30 trials, it was considered not to have reached criteria, even if it met accuracy and omissions thresholds.
Probe trial.
To probe attention in mice we used a previously described probe trial schedule with reduced stimulus durations (Romberg et al., 2011). Briefly, each mouse was tested for 2 consecutive days at a given stimulus duration (1.5, 1, 0.8, and 0.6 s). After each test, the animal was retested onto the 2 s stimulus duration for 2 d or until criteria were reached (80% accuracy, 20% omission), to assess baseline performance. The order of the probe trial sessions were semirandomized using a Latin square method.
Distraction task.
To further test attentional demand, and assess susceptibility to distraction, we developed a distraction version of the 5-CSRT task. During this task, a 1000 ms distractor tone, different from the reward tone, was played semirandomly at five different time points during the delay period: 0 (corresponding to when the mouse initiates the task), 0.5, 2.5, 4.5, and 5 s (corresponding to when the stimulus is displayed on the touchscreens). Stimulus duration was set to 2 s. For each distractor tone time point mice were tested for five sessions. At the end of the five sessions one baseline task (50 trial, 2 s stimulus duration, no distractor sounds) was performed before moving to the next distractor time point.
5-CSRT task measures.
On all 5-CSRT task sessions, accuracy was defined as the total number of correct responses, divided by the number of correct and incorrect (touches to a wrong window while the correct stimulus was still displayed) responses. The rate of omissions was the proportion of omitted responses to total trials. Response latency was the time for the mouse to touch the correct stimulus after its onset. Reward collection latency was the time for the mouse to return to the reward tray once it had touched the correct stimulus. A premature response was counted when the mouse touched one of the windows before stimulus onset. Finally, a perseverative response was any identical response that occurred following a correct, incorrect, or premature response.
Drug treatment.
Galantamine hydrobromide, a cholinesterase inhibitor, (Sigma-Aldrich) was dissolved in physiological saline (0.9% NaCl) before administration. Sixty minutes before being tested on the 5-CSRT mice received an intraperitoneal injection of drug (100 μl, 1 mg/kg) or saline. The dose for galantamine was selected based on previous studies (Prado et al., 2006; de Castro et al., 2009b; De Jaeger et al., 2013). Previously, we have tested 3 mg/kg galantamine in other tasks, but this dose was no more effective than 1 mg/kg and it produced hypersalivation in mice (de Castro et al., 2009b). Moreover, at 1 mg/kg wild-type mice were able to increase their performance in the 5-CSRT (see below). Mice were tested at the 0.6 s stimulus duration, which represents a high attention demanding task (Romberg et al., 2011). The order of the injections was counterbalanced. Between injections mice had three washout days wherein their performance on the 5CSRT was re-baselined at the 2 s stimulus duration.
Statistical analysis.
For the pairwise visual discrimination task response accuracy was calculated as the number of correct trials divided by the total number of trials, excluding correction trials. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. SigmaStat 3.5 software was used for statistical analysis. Comparison between two experimental groups was made by Student's t test or Mann–Whitney rank sum test, when the data did not follow a normal distribution. When several experimental groups or treatments were analyzed, two-way ANOVA or two-way ANOVA with repeated measures was used as required. When appropriate, a Tukey's HSD post hoc comparison test was used. In all comparisons, p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.
Results
VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice have reduced VAChT and ACh release in the PFC
We have previously reported that VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice have the VAChT gene deleted from >90% of their basal forebrain cholinergic neurons (Martyn et al., 2012). To confirm that this deletion affected prefrontal cortical cholinergic signaling, we performed Western blot analysis to assess VAChT expression. VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice have a significant reduction in PFC VAChT protein expression (t(6) = 2.706, p = 0.0353; Fig. 1A). Moreover, this reduction in VAChT protein levels results in a significant decrease in newly synthesized [3H] ACh release in slices of PFC from VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice when compared with control mice (t(4) = 2.899, p = 0.0442; Fig. 1B). We used qPCR to assess the expression of nicotinic receptors (nAChRs), which have been shown previously to be critical for attentional performance. The PFC of VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice shows no significant change in the expression of α7nAChR mRNA (t(10) = 0.9300, p = 0.3743) nor β2nAChR mRNA (t(10) = 0.8359, p = 0.4227). Interestingly, α4nAChR mRNA is slightly upregulated (t(10) = 2.550, p = 0.0289; Fig. 1 C).
Decreased forebrain cholinergic tone specifically disturbs reversal learning in the pairwise visual discrimination task.
We have previously demonstrated that VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice have impairments in reversal learning in the MWM (Martyn et al., 2012), suggesting the possibility that these mice have behavior flexibility deficits. To determine whether VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice present alterations in cognitive flexibility we used a “nonhippocampal” pairwise visual discrimination task (Romberg et al., 2013). This task has been previously shown to depend on the PFC and also on striatal-cortical loops (Graybeal et al., 2011). The performance of VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice did not differ from that of controls (VAChTflox/flox) when they were trained to operate the touchscreen (pretraining phase). The number of sessions the mice took to acquire each training phase did not differ from control (RM-ANOVA, no effect of genotype F(1,54) = 0.3950, p = 0.5398; main effect of training phase F(4,54) = 5.227, p = 0.0012; and no interaction effect F(4,54) = 1.389, p = 0.2495; Figure 2A). Acquisition of the pairwise visual discrimination task (Fig. 2B; for stimuli used) did not differ between genotypes, in terms of sessions to criteria (Fig. 2C; t(14) = 0.2446, p = 0.8117), correction errors made (Fig. 2D; t(14) = 0.2942, p = 0.7746), response latency (Fig. 2E; t(14) = 1.019, p = 0.3256), or reward collection latency (Fig. 2F; t(14) = 0.2606, p = 0.7988). We have previously reported that these mice are hyperactive in novel environments, but they are able to habituate to the environment (Martyn et al., 2012). Hence, due to the extensive training for the performance in the touchscreen tasks the lack of differences in response and reward collection latencies is not surprising. However, VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice showed severe reversal learning impairment (Fig. 3A; for the stimuli used), measured by the percentage of correct responses (Fig. 3B; RM-ANOVA, main effect of genotype F(1,132) = 19.78, p = 0.0008; main effect of session F(11,132) = 23.28, p = 0.0001; and significant interaction effect F(1,12) = 5.035, p = 0.0001) and by its increased correction errors (Fig. 3C; RM-ANOVA, main effect of genotype F(1,132) = 14.72, p = 0.0024; main effect of session F(11,132) = 14.37, p = 0.0001; and significant interaction effect F(1,12) = 2.817, p = 0.0025). Post hoc analysis showed that VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice never improved significantly from the first reversal session and in 10 sessions they performed only at chance level. Importantly, during the reversal trials, the VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice did not differ from controls in terms of response latency (Fig. 3D; RM-ANOVA, no effect of genotype F(1,154) = 0.4233, p = 0.5258; main effect of session F(11,154) = 4.705, p = 0.0001; and no interaction effect F(11,154) = 0.9997, p = 0.4493) or reward collection latency (Fig. 3E; RM-ANOVA, no effect of genotype F(1,154) = 1.107, p = 0.3105; main effect of session F(11,132) = 3.965, p = 0.0001; and no interaction effect F(11,154) = 1.141, p = 0.3333). These results suggest that forebrain cholinergic tone is important for cognitive flexibility.
VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice have impaired acquisition of the 5-CSRT task
Similar to the results for visual discrimination experiments, the performance of the second cohort of VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice did not differ from that of control mice for touchscreen operation training (pretraining phase). The number of sessions necessary to reach criteria across all pretraining phases did not differ between genotypes (RM-ANOVA, no effect of genotype F(1,32) = 1.528, p = 0.2515; no effect of training phase F(4,32) = 1.492, p = 0.2280; Figure 4A). However, when mice were trained to respond to flashes of light displayed in one of the five spatial locations on the touchscreen (training phase), VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox showed a significantly worse performance, needing more sessions to reach criteria (RM-ANOVA, main effect of genotype, F(1,8) = 10.06, p = 0.0132; main effect of stimulus duration, F(1,8) = 5.731, p = 0.0436, no interaction effect, F(1,8) = 4.252, p = 0.0731; Fig. 4B).
Deletion of forebrain VAChT results in inattentive but not impulsive or compulsive behavior
Once mice reached criteria they were tested for attentional performance by decreasing stimulus duration to 1.5, 1, 0.8, and 0.6 s as previously described (Romberg et al., 2011). At 1.5 s, VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice were able to perform identically to control mice in both accuracy and omissions (Fig. 5A,B). In contrast, increasing attentional demand by diminishing the stimuli period revealed an attentional deficit in VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice. Response accuracy did not differ between genotypes (RM-ANOVA, no effect of genotype F(1,24) = 0.0007548, p = 0.9788; main effect of stimulus duration F(3,24) = 10.28, p < 0.001; Figure 5A). However, VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice presented an increased rate of omission (RM-ANOVA, main effect of genotype F(1,24) = 57.99, p < 0.0001; main effect of stimulus duration F(3,24) = 36.44, p < 0.0001; no interaction effect F(3,24) = 2.176, p = 0.1171; Figure 5B). Post hoc analysis revealed that VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice omitted more at 0.6–1 s stimulus durations. Interestingly, VAChT-deficient mice had no alterations in premature responses (RM-ANOVA, no effect of genotype F(1,24) = 0.07440, p = 0.7874; no effect of stimulus duration F(3,24) = 0.9891, p = 0.4146; Figure 5C) or perseverative responses (RM-ANOVA, no effect of genotype F(1,24) = 0.04610, p = 0.9695; no effect of stimulus duration F(3,24) = 1.244, p = 0.3232; Figure 5D). Additionally, mutant mice did not differ from littermate controls in response latency (RM-ANOVA, no effect of genotype F(1,24) = 2.279, p = 0.1818; no effect of stimulus duration F(3,24) = 0.09686, p = 0.7662; Figure 5E) and reward collection latency (RM-ANOVA, no effect of genotype F(1,24) = 2.279, p = 0.1818; no effect of stimulus duration F(3,24) = 1.189, p = 0.3420; Figure 5F).
Deletion of forebrain VAChT impairs sustained attention
To assess sustained attention (vigilance), we analyzed both response accuracy and rate of omissions over blocks of 10 trials (Romberg et al., 2011). Response accuracy of control mice did not significantly vary across blocks, but did reduce significantly with decreases in stimuli duration (RM-ANOVA, main effect of stimulus duration F(3,48) = 5.893, p = 0.0104; no effect of block F(4,48) = 1.214, p = 0.317; Figure 6A). In contrast, response accuracy of VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice reduced significantly across blocks and stimuli duration (RM-ANOVA, main effect of stimulus duration F(3,48) = 4.257, p = 0.0077; main effect of block F(4,48) = 3.933, p = 0.0289; Figure 6B). As with response accuracy, rate of omissions of control mice did not vary across blocks, only with stimuli duration (RM-ANOVA, main effect of stimulus duration F(3,48) = 5.803, p = 0.0228; no effect of block F(4,48) = 0.5352, p = 0.7105; Figure 6C), whereas rate of omissions of VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice increased significantly across stimuli duration and blocks (RM-ANOVA, main effect of stimulus duration F(3,48) = 9.387, p = 0.0018; main effect of block F(4,48) = 2.803, p = 0.0360; Figure 6D). Together the data suggest that VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice have impaired ability to sustain attention.
Deletion of forebrain VAChT increases susceptibility to distractions
To assess distractibility of VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice, we increased attentional demand by testing them on a distraction variation of the 5-CSRT. Stimulus duration was set to 2 s, where performance of both genotypes was identical, and distractor sounds were played at set time points during trials. Response accuracy of VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice tended to be reduced by distractions compared with controls (RM-ANOVA, near significant effect of genotype F(1,32) = 4.825, p = 0.0589; main effect of distractor onset F(4,32) = 3.583, p = 0.0159; Figure 7A). Rate of omission was significantly higher in VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice than controls (RM-ANOVA, significant effect of genotype F(1,32) = 6.809, p = 0.0312; no effect of distractor onset F(4,32) = 2.564, p = 0.0572; Figure 7B). Importantly, neither response latency (RM-ANOVA, no effect of genotype F(1,32) = 1.045, p = 0.3367; main effect of distractor onset F(4,32) = 7.069, p = 0.0003; Figure 7C) nor reward collection latency varied between genotypes (RM-ANOVA, no effect of genotype F(1,32) = 0.7467, p = 0.4127; main effect of distractor onset F(4,32) = 3.226, p = 0.0248; Figure 7D).
Galantamine improves attention in wild-type mice on a demanding task
Detection of pro-attentive effects of pharmacological manipulations on the 5-CSRT is hampered by possible ceiling effects (Robbins, 2002). To detect differences in mice it is important to test the drug on challenging conditions, which can be achieved by shortening the duration of the stimulus presentation (Romberg et al., 2011). To find a suitably challenging task in which we could observe increased attention, we trained a group (n = 8) of wild-type mice on the 5-CSRT, and ran them through the probe trial series, serially reducing stimulus duration (1.5, 1, 0.8, and 0.6 s). Choice accuracy (F(4,31) = 13.77, p < 0.0001; Fig. 8A) and rate of omissions (F(4,31) = 6.716, p = 0.0024; Fig. 8B) were significantly affected by reducing stimulus duration. Post hoc analysis revealed that at 0.6 s stimulus duration the rate of accuracy was significantly reduced, and omissions significantly increased from the 1.5 s stimulus duration. We therefore chose both, 0.8 s stimulus duration and the more challenging 0.6 s stimulus duration, for pharmacological testing. Administration of galantamine (1 mg/kg, i.p.) 1 h before 5-CSRT testing at 0.8 s did not improve accuracy (paired t test, t(7) = 1.287, p = 0.2544; Fig. 8C) or omissions (paired t test, t(7) = 0.4581, p = 0.6661; Fig. 8D). However, when administered before a 0.6 s stimulus duration session, galantamine (1 mg/kg) significantly improved accuracy compared with saline (paired t test, t(7) = 2.405, p = 0.0471; Fig. 8E) and significantly reduced the rate of omission (paired t test, t(7) = 2.379, p = 0.0489;Fig. 8F). Response latency (paired t test, t(7) = 1.296, p = 0.2360), and reward collection latency (paired t test, t(7) = 0.390, p = 0.7080) were not changed by galantamine injections (data not shown). These results suggest that increased cholinergic tone can increase attentional performance when the probe trial is sufficiently demanding to avoid a potential ceiling effect.
Galantamine does not improve attention deficits in VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice
To investigate if the deficits observed in VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox are exclusively related to decreased levels of synaptic ACh, we injected mice with galantamine (1 mg/Kg IP), a dose that effectively improved performance of wild-type mice (Fig. 8), and tested both VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox and VAChTflox/flox mice at 0.6 s (50 trials). Galantamine tended to improve choice accuracy, albeit this effect did not reach statistical significance (paired t test, t(5) = 1.954, p = 0.1224; Fig. 8G). However, the drug was able to significantly reduce rate of omissions (paired t test, t(5) = 3.383, p = 0.0277; Fig. 8H) in VAChTflox/flox mice. Interestingly, galantamine had no effect on the performance of VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice, neither improving accuracy (paired t test, t(5) = 0.162, p = 0.880; Fig. 8G) nor rate of omission (paired t test, t(5) = 0.868, p = 0.434; Fig. 8H).
Deletion of forebrain VAChT results in metabolic abnormalities in the PFC
The lack of effect of galantamine in VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox suggests that diminished ACh release in mutant mice may not be sufficiently increased by galantamine to reverse the attentional deficits in these mice. In addition to that, a chronic decrease in cholinergic tone may cause circuitry or metabolic changes that could affect how neuronal circuitries are recruited for specific cognitive tasks. We used in vivo magnetic resonance spectroscopy to assess if metabolic parameters were affected in the prefrontal region of VAChT-deficient mice (Fig. 9A; representative spectra). This analysis revealed that VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice had significantly less Lac (t(6) = 2.600, p = 0.0428; Fig. 9B) and Tau (t(6) = 2.522, p = 0.0452; Fig. 9C) than controls. Levels of NAA (t(6) = 0.0907, p = 0.9307; Fig. 9D), Myo (t(6) = 0.9598, p = 0.3742; Fig. 9E), and Cho (t(6) = 0.1461, p = 0.8886; Fig. 9F) remained unchanged.
Deletion of forebrain VAChT results in altered RNA metabolism in the PFC
Cholinergic deficits in AD have been proposed to cause major transcriptome changes via aberrant hnRNPA2/B1 expression (Berson et al., 2012). hnRNPA2/B1 refers to a family of proteins that functions as splicing factors and mRNA chaperones (Hoek et al., 1998; Kamma et al., 1999). This gene family is critical for regulating alternative splicing in numerous genes involved with synaptic plasticity and cognition (Berson et al., 2012). Its expression is severely reduced in Alzheimer brain and appears enhanced in primary mouse neurons under carbachol induction (Berson et al., 2012), suggesting cholinergic regulation of hnRNPA2/B1 proteins. To determine whether the PFC of VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox shows parallel suppression of the hnRNPA2/B1 proteins, their levels were assessed. Compared with controls, the PFC of VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice showed a significant reduction in hnRNPA2/B1 expression (75% decrease, t(6) = 4.941, p = 0.0026; Fig. 10A). We then used qPCR to assess whether the decrease in hnRNPA2/B1 could have a functional impact on RNA processing in the PFC. We evaluated the alternative splicing of the key genes, SIPA1L1(SIPA), REELIN(RELN), DRAM2, CD55, DYSTONIN (DST), and ENAH, as manifested by increased exon inclusion tested both in Alzheimer's brain and in mouse brain depleted of its cholinergic neurons by saporin-mediated treatment (Berson et al., 2012). This analysis revealed that VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice had significant changes in the splicing of these genes in the PFC, with significantly increased inclusion of exon 8 of CD55 (t(10) = 2.550, p = 0.0289), exon 41 of DST (t(10) = 7.436, p = 0.0001), exon 18 of RELN (t(10) = 3.230, p = 0.0090), and exon 2 of DRAM2 (t(10) = 3.990, p = 0.0260). In addition the inv isoform of the ENAH gene was also significantly increased (t(10) = 2.522, p = 0.0303). In contrast, intron 5 of SIPA (t(10) = 0.5449, p = 0.5978) and exon 3 of RELN (t(10) = 2.215, p = 0.0511), which have also been shown to be affected in AD, were unchanged, although we detected a trend for the latter (Fig. 10B).
hnRNPA2/B1 has been shown to regulate the splicing of pyruvate kinase M (PKM) enzyme, which in turn dictates lactate metabolism (David et al., 2010). Knockdown of hnRNPA2/B1 expression in vitro favors the PKM1 isoform, which leads to reduced levels of Lac, whereas upregulation hnRNPA2/B1 favors the PKM2 isoform and increased Lac levels (Clower et al., 2010; for review, see Chen et al., 2010). To test whether alternative splicing of PKM occurs in VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice, we performed qPCR to determine the expression of the PKM1 and PKM2 splice variants. Interestingly the VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice showed a significant upregulation of the PKM1 variant (t(10) = 4.277, p = 0.0016), and a significant reduction in the PKM2 variant (t(10) = 3.073, p = 0.0110; Fig. 10C), effectively changing the ratio between these two enzymes.
Discussion
In this report we show that forebrain VAChT knock-out mice present deficits in two domains of executive function, cognitive flexibility and attention. Additionally, VAChT-deficient mice exhibit metabolic deficits in the PFC suggestive of changes in PFC circuitry. Furthermore, these mice have reduced expression of a key splicing factor, hnRNPA2/B1, which has been previously implicated in synaptic plasticity deficits in AD (Berson et al., 2012) and in mutations that were recently discovered in rare proteinopathies (Kim et al., 2013). These changes have a functional impact on RNA metabolism in the PFC of VAChT-deficient mice. These results suggest that VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice may represent a powerful tool to dissect the molecular and neurochemical basis of executive dysfunction.
Cognitive flexibility in VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice
VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice were able to associate an image with a reward, and another with a punishment, in the pairwise visual discrimination task. Acquisition of this task has been shown to be dependent on glutamatergic signaling, with mice lacking the GLAST glutamate transporter being unable to acquire the task (Karlsson et al., 2009). Evidence suggests that the NMDA receptor is an important molecular switch for the acquisition of the task (Brigman et al., 2008; Barkus et al., 2012). Our results indicate that cholinergic signaling is not required for such learning. In contrast, when contingencies of the pairwise task were reversed, forebrain VAChT knock-out mice were unable to adapt and learn the new rule. Reversal learning in a visual discrimination task has been proposed to serve as a measure of cognitive flexibility in rodents (Izquierdo et al., 2006; Brigman et al., 2008). Neurochemical modulation of reversal learning is complex, as it can be enhanced by targeting multiple neurotransmitter signaling systems including serotonergic (Brigman et al., 2010), dopaminergic (Izquierdo et al., 2006), and glutamatergic (Balschun et al., 2010). The role of cholinergic signaling has been focused predominantly on muscarinic receptors (Ridley et al., 1984, 1985). Determining the exact mechanism has been difficult, as M1 receptor agonists facilitate cognitive flexibility (McCool et al., 2008; Shirey et al., 2009), but no effects on cognitive flexibility were observed in M1-null mice (Bartko et al., 2011). There is evidence suggesting that M2 and M4 receptors may be involved in cognitive flexibility (Nieves-Martinez et al., 2012). Our results indicate that cholinergic signaling is essential for modulation of cognitive flexibility.
Attention deficits in VAChT-deficient mice
To further understand the role of forebrain ACh in executive function we evaluated attention on forebrain VAChT knock-out mice using the 5-CSRT task, a test suggested to be dependent on PFC cholinergic signaling (Guillem et al., 2011; for review, see Robbins, 2000; Dalley et al., 2004a). In line with our previous finding with the VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mouse line having learning and memory deficits (Martyn et al., 2012), these mice showed impairments in acquisition of the 5-CSRT, taking nearly twice as long as controls to reach criteria at the 4 and 2 s phase of the training process. Interestingly, they were not impaired during the pretraining phase, wherein mice are taught to respond to the touchscreen. These results suggest that simple operant conditioning is not dependent on forebrain cholinergic signaling, but rather that forebrain ACh is responsible for the use of such information to perform higher order cognitive tasks. Attentional demand deficits were probed by reducing stimulus duration. Choice accuracy, perseverative, and premature responses of VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice were unaffected. However, the rate of omission for VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox was significantly increased. Additionally, in the presence of a noise distractor VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice were significantly less attentive than controls, showing a much higher rate of omissions. This test was performed under a condition in which the performance of VAChT-deficient mice was on par with controls without the auditory distraction. The auditory distraction task gives a clear indication that the attention deficits observed in VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice were not due to visual abnormalities. These findings are aligned with previous work, given that cue detection has been shown to involve transient rises in ACh release (Himmelheber et al., 2000; Parikh et al., 2007). Our experiments agree with previous work indicating a key role for cholinergic activity in improving response to distractors in attentional tasks (Gill et al., 2000; Terry et al., 2002; Newman and McGaughy, 2008; Broussard et al., 2009; Howe et al., 2010; St Peters et al., 2011).
Regulation of attentional performance by endogenous ACh has been investigated in rats using immunolesion with IgG-192 saporin (Walsh et al., 1996; Risbrough et al., 2002; Lehmann et al., 2003; Chudasama et al., 2004; Dalley et al., 2004b). Specifically, deficits in choice accuracy and increases in perseverative responses were observed in rats following 192 IgG-saporin-induced lesions (McGaughy et al., 2002; Dalley et al., 2004b). In these studies no effects were observed on rates of omissions. Our experiments show a somewhat distinct feature, demonstrating increased rates of omission, without changes in perseverative response. Interestingly, selective elimination of the β2 nAChR in mice also revealed an increase in rate of omissions (Guillem et al., 2011). The differences between genetically modified and lesioned animals could therefore be species related. Alternatively, this difference may reflect the capacity of cholinergic neurons to secrete more than one neurotransmitter (El Mestikawy et al., 2011; Prado et al., 2013). Basal forebrain cholinergic neurons can release glutamate in vitro (Allen et al., 2006). There is also evidence that these neurons possess the machinery to release GABA (Henny and Jones, 2008). Recent experiments targeting striatum cholinergic neurons revealed striking behavioral differences between mice that had cholinergic elimination, using immunolesion, or mice that were genetically targeted to eliminate striatal ACh release (Kitabatake et al., 2003; Guzman et al., 2011; for review, see Prado et al., 2013). Immunolesion of cholinergic neurons can therefore have effects beyond impairing ACh release and could affect cotransmission. Whether cotransmission has a role in the small phenotypic differences between our experiments and previous work using rats injected with IgG-192-saporin remains to be determined.
Alterations in PFC function in VAChT-deficient mice
Galantamine was unable to rescue the attention deficits in VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice. We have previously shown, using a mouse line with decreased VAChT expression, that galantamine could reverse social memory deficits (Prado et al., 2006) and object recognition memory deficits (de Castro et al., 2009b; De Jaeger et al., 2013). Hence, the fact that galantamine can improve performance of control mice, but not of VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice, suggests that the latter may not release sufficient synaptic ACh to be enhanced by galantamine. Alternatively, these sophisticated tasks may be more affected by changes in neuronal circuitry. The in vivo 1H spectroscopy data suggest that critical metabolic changes occur in VAChT-deficient mice; namely, reduced levels of both Tau and Lac in the prefrontal region. Lac has been proposed as the preferred metabolic substrate for neurons (for review, see Pellerin et al., 2007), suggesting that decreased cholinergic tone may lead to a general decrease in neuronal activity.
The reduction in PFC hnRNPA2/B1 observed in VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice is in line with previous work indicating cholinergic-mediated regulation of its expression in AD (Berson et al., 2012). Furthermore, we found that alternative splicing alteration pattern in the PFC of these mice is similar to the pattern found in both AD patient samples and hypocholinergic animal models (Berson et al., 2012). Overall, these changes suggest alterations in the PFC circuitry. Furthermore, the splicing change found in the PKM gene indicates a potential mechanism for cholinergic regulation of Lac metabolism. Of note, nearly significant differential expression of the PKM variants was observed in exon array datasets from the entorhinal cortices of three AD patients and three matched controls studied previously, despite the small number of samples (Berson et al., 2012; p < 0.051; raw data deposited in the GenBank).
Decreased Lac levels have been observed in mouse models of AD (Du Yan et al., 2000; Marjanska et al., 2005). Moreover, in the CRND8 transgenic mouse model of AD, lower levels of Tau were observed in vivo by H1-NMR (Salek et al., 2010). Interestingly, high levels of Lac in cultured neurons decrease susceptibility to Aβ-derived peptides and oxidative stress in vitro (Newington et al., 2012). Together, these data suggest that VAChTSix3-Cre-flox/flox mice may be useful for understanding metabolic abnormalities that occur in dementia.
Conclusion
In summary, by eliminating VAChT from the forebrain we determined that cholinergic signaling regulates executive function, affects metabolism, and also RNA processing in the PFC. The PFC has been shown to mediate salient cue detection (Himmelheber et al., 2000; Parikh et al., 2007), and it serves as a hub that regulates numerous neurotransmitter interactions (Dalley et al., 2004a; Carr et al., 2007; Tait and Brown, 2008). Our work helps to define the specific role played by ACh in behaviors related to cortical functioning, and its potential underlying mechanisms. Decreased levels of VAChT in the brain have been reported in AD (Efange et al., 1997; Chen et al., 2011), therefore the executive dysfunction and mRNA processing abnormalities we observed in VAChT-deficient mice may be of relevance to model this specific deficiency in humans. Therefore, this work provides novel insights into the basic neurochemical contributors governing executive function.
Footnotes
This work was supported by Canadian Institutes of Health Research (MOP 89919 and 126000, M.A.M.P. and V.F.P.), Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (V.F.P.), Canadian Foundation for Innovation, and Ontario Research Fund (M.A.M.P., V.F.P.).
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to either Marco Prado or Vania Prado, Robarts Research Institute, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada, N61 5K8. mprado{at}robarts.ca or vprado{at}robarts.ca