Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Featured ArticleArticles, Development/Plasticity/Repair

Impaired Critical Period Plasticity in Primary Auditory Cortex of Fragile X Model Mice

Heesoo Kim, Robert Gibboni, Colleen Kirkhart and Shaowen Bao
Journal of Neuroscience 2 October 2013, 33 (40) 15686-15692; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3246-12.2013
Heesoo Kim
Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, California 94720
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Robert Gibboni
Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, California 94720
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Colleen Kirkhart
Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, California 94720
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Shaowen Bao
Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, California 94720
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Fragile X syndrome, the most common form of heritable mental retardation, is a developmental disorder with known effects within sensory systems. Altered developmental plasticity has been reported in the visual and somatosensory systems in Fmr1 knock-out (KO) mice. Behavioral studies have revealed maladaptive auditory responses in fragile X syndrome patients and Fmr1 KO mice, suggesting that adaptive plasticity may also be impaired in the auditory system. Here we show that, whereas tonotopic frequency representation develops normally in Fmr1 KO mice, developmental plasticity in primary auditory cortex is grossly impaired. This deficit can be rescued by pharmacological blockade of mGluR5 receptors. These results support the mGluR hypothesis of fragile X mental retardation and suggest that deficient developmental plasticity may contribute to maladaptive auditory processing in fragile X syndrome.

Introduction

Fragile X syndrome is caused by expansion of trinucleotide CGG repeats in the Fmr1 gene resulting in hypermethylation and loss of function of the gene (Jin and Warren, 2003). Fmr1 encodes fragile X mental retardation protein (FMRP), an mRNA-binding protein that suppresses and regulates local mRNA translation. The lack of FMRP exaggerates mGluR-stimulated protein synthesis, which has been hypothesized to cause various fragile X symptoms (Bear et al., 2004; Osterweil et al., 2010). Supporting this mGluR hypothesis of fragile X syndrome, the blockade of mGluR either genetically or pharmacologically reverses certain fragile X phenotypes in animal models (McBride et al., 2005; Yan et al., 2005; Dölen et al., 2007; de Vrij et al., 2008; Meredith et al., 2011; Su et al., 2011; Michalon et al., 2012; Thomas et al., 2012).

Fragile X mental retardation is the most common form of heritable mental retardation. Among its symptoms are maladaptive sensory responses and impaired sensory integration (Miller et al., 1999; Chen and Toth, 2001; Nielsen et al., 2002), which have been hypothesized to contribute to the impaired development of higher cognitive functions (Hanson et al., 1986; LeBlanc and Fagiolini, 2011). Studies in mouse models lacking the Fmr1 gene revealed altered ocular dominance plasticity in visual cortex and a delayed critical period of synaptic plasticity in barrel cortex (Dölen et al., 2007; Harlow et al., 2010). Altered auditory processing in humans with fragile X syndrome and Fmr1 knock-out (KO) animals suggests that development and plasticity in the auditory system may also be affected by fragile X syndrome (Miller et al., 1999; Chen and Toth, 2001; Nielsen et al., 2002).

The development of acoustic representations in primary auditory cortex is profoundly influenced by early experience (Zhang et al., 2001; de Villers-Sidani et al., 2007; Insanally et al., 2009; Popescu and Polley, 2010). Exposure of juvenile animals to patterned sensory input refines the balance of excitation and inhibition (Dorrn et al., 2010; Sun et al., 2010), resulting in receptive field and sensory map reorganization and a long-lasting impact on sound perception (Han et al., 2007). The robust effects of early experience on sound representation and perception make the auditory cortex an ideal system to investigate how genetic mutations may lead to sensory abnormalities, such as those caused by fragile X mental retardation and Fmr1 gene deletion. In addition, cellular and synaptic abnormalities have been well characterized in the Fmr1 KO mouse, making it a valuable model to study the mechanisms of sensory development and plasticity.

In the present study, we investigate the development of cortical sound representations and sound-induced cortical map reorganization in the Fmr1 KO mouse. Our results indicate that the Fmr1 KO mouse develops a normal tonotopic cortical frequency map, but does not show experience-dependent map reorganization. Systemic administration of MPEP, an mGluR5 antagonist, rescues the impairment in cortical map plasticity.

Materials and Methods

Rearing and injection procedures.

All procedures used in this study were approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of California, Berkeley. Wild-type (WT) and Fmr1 KO mice on the FVB background were originally obtained from The Jackson Laboratory (FVB.129P2-Pde6b+ Tyrc-ch/AntJ and FVB.129P2-Pde6b+ Tyrc-ch Fmr1tm1Cgr/J). Only male homozygous WT or KO mice were used in this study. Litters of mouse pups were placed in sound-attenuating chambers with their mothers from postnatal day 9 (P9) through P20 (early window) and exposed to 16 kHz pure tone pips (25 ms duration, 60 dB) presented in trains of 6 pips (each train every 2 s and pips presented at 5 pips per second within a train) through an overhead speaker. Additional juvenile mice were exposed to the same sound stimuli at a later time window (P20–P30). Animals reared in a normal laboratory husbandry setting served as naive controls.

Additional litters of animals, exposed to the same sound stimuli during either the early (P9–P20) or the late window (P20–P30), were also given daily injections of 2-methyl-6-(phenylethynyl)-pyrydine (MPEP; reconstituted to 2 mg/ml, injected at 30 mg/kg, i.p.; Sigma-Aldrich) or vehicle saline (15 ml/kg, i.p.). The weights of these animals were monitored daily to ensure proper development. Consistent with previous reports (Moy et al., 2009), KO animals generally had larger body mass than WT animals. Sound exposure tended to reduce weight gain of KO animals and increase weight gain of WT animals (Table 1).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Response properties of auditory cortex neurons

After sound exposure, experimental animals were returned to a normal laboratory husbandry setting until the electrophysiological mapping of primary auditory cortex (typically P35–P45). A subset of early window exposure animals were recorded immediately after removal from the sound exposure box (P20–P25). Care was taken to ensure that all conditions (genotype, exposure condition, injection schedule) were age-matched during mapping, except the subset of animals that were mapped immediately after removal from the sound exposure box. Across the 16 conditions, a total of 76 animals were used for cortical electrophysiological experiments (Table 1). An additional 16 animals were used for auditory brainstem responses (ABRs).

Electrophysiological recording procedure.

The primary auditory cortex (AI) of mice was mapped as previously described (Kim and Bao, 2009), except as follows. Mice were anesthetized with ketamine (100 mg/kg, i.p.) and xylazine (10 mg/kg, i.p.), and supplemented as needed (ketamine 50 mg/kg, xylazine 5 mg/kg, i.p. generally once an hour). The cortex was maintained under a layer of silicone oil, reapplied as necessary. Multiunit responses to 25 ms pure tone pips (2–74 or 4–74 kHz, 0.1 octave increments at 0–70 dB, 10 dB increments) were recorded using tungsten microelectrodes (FHC) in the thalamorecipient layer of AI (350–450 micrometers below the cortical surface). References to neurons in this text refer to multiunit responses recorded extracellularly. Tones were presented to the left ear through an electrostatic speaker (Tucker Davis Technologies) at 3 pips per second and each frequency-intensity combination was repeated three times. Cortical penetration locations were recorded on a high-resolution image.

ABRs were recorded under identical anesthetic conditions with the active electrode at the vertex, the reference electrode caudomedial to the left ear pinna, and the ground electrode at the dorsosacrum (O'Connor et al., 1998; Popescu and Polley, 2010). Tone pips (4, 8, 16, and 32 kHz at 0–70 dB SPL in 5 dB increments) were presented to the left ear and ABRs were recorded ipsilaterally. Responses to 500 pips were averaged and high-pass filtered with a cutoff frequency of 200 Hz.

Data analysis.

Receptive fields and response properties were isolated using custom-made programs in MATLAB as previously described (Insanally et al., 2010), except as follows. The peak of the peristimulus time histogram (PSTH) within a window from 7 to 50 ms after the stimulus onset was defined as the response latency. The response window was defined as a period encompassing the PSTH peak, in which the firing rate was higher than the baseline firing rate. The spikes in the response window were counted to reconstruct the receptive field. The characteristic frequency (CF) and threshold of the receptive fields were identified by hand by a blind experimenter. Bandwidth at 50 dB was automatically calculated as previously described (Insanally et al., 2010).

ABR thresholds at each frequency were determined as the lowest intensity that produced a visibly discernible response. Wave latency times were manually labeled for two frequency (8 and 16 kHz) and two intensity levels (70 and 45 dB). Wave amplitudes were calculated as the amplitude from a peak to an ensuing trough.

All error bars indicate SEM. Statistical tests are indicated in the text.

Results

Fmr1 KO mice exhibit impaired critical period sensory map plasticity

Litters of WT and KO animals were exposed to 16 kHz tones from P9 to P20. This window encompasses the critical period for frequency representation in rats (de Villers-Sidani et al., 2007; Insanally et al., 2009) and has been shown to elicit map plasticity in mice (Barkat et al., 2011). Naive WT and KO animals had very similar cortical frequency representation (Fig. 1A,B), suggesting normal auditory cortex development in KO mice. WT animals exposed to 16 kHz tone showed a substantial increase in representation of 16 kHz, whereas KO animals exposed to the same tone did not (Fig. 1A,B). A 2 × 2 × 10 ANOVA (genotype × exposure conditions × frequency bin) revealed a significant three-way interaction (p = 0.0009), suggesting a differential representation to specific frequency bins between genotype and rearing condition. A further two-way ANOVA (genotype × exposure condition) on individual frequency bins found significant effects at 16 kHz (exposure condition, p = 0.00029; genotype, p = 0.055; interaction p = 0.024; Fig. 1B), indicating representation of the exposed frequency increased in the WT mice only. The sound exposure had opposite effects on the representation of 21.11 kHz for the two genotypes (two-way ANOVA interaction, p = 0.0002), reducing 21.11 kHz representation in the WT but not KO mice. Similar reduction of representations for frequencies near the exposure frequency has been previously reported (Han et al., 2007).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Fmr1 KO mice exhibit impaired critical period plasticity in the auditory cortex. A, Example AI frequency maps of two genotypes (WT and Fmr1 KO) and three conditions (naive, early tone exposure, late tone exposure). Areas outlined in black indicate sites with CFs near the exposure frequency of 16 kHz (±0.2 octaves). Example receptive fields are shown for each map, with the location indicated by white roman numerals (x-axis is frequency from 2–74 kHz; y-axis is sound level from 0 to 70 dB). Scale bar, 1 mm. B, Size of frequency representation for the naive and early tone exposure groups. The WT early tone exposure group shows a significant increase in representation of 16 kHz (**condition main effect p = 0.00029 and interaction p = 0.024). A small decrease is also observed at 21.1 kHz (*interaction p = 0.0002). C, Representation of 16 kHz across four conditions (same as A with the addition of early tone exposure animals mapped immediately after removal from tone exposure box). Early tone exposure resulted in a significant increase in 16 kHz representation in the WT animals in both mapping windows (one-way ANOVA main effect p < 0.0005, post hoc tests: *p < 0.05 and **p < 0.005). Sound exposure did not cause map changes in KO animals (one-way ANOVA, p = 0.13).

This impairment in plasticity could be due to a delay in the critical period (Harlow et al., 2010). To address this possibility, additional litters of animals were exposed to 16 kHz between P20 and P30. A 2 × 3 ANOVA [genotype × exposure condition (naive, early exposure, late exposure)] revealed a significant main effect for exposure condition (p = 0.0001) and a significant interaction (p = 0.015). We find that only the WT mice that were sound exposed in the early window, but not the late window, had enlarged representation of 16 kHz (Fig. 1C). Neither the WT nor the KO mice that were sound-exposed in the late window showed enhanced representations of 16 kHz. Thus, the critical period for frequency-representation in mice does not extend beyond P20, and KO mice do not have a delayed critical period.

The lack of exposure-induced frequency map reorganization in the KO mice could also be due to previously reported hyperplasticity (Dölen et al., 2007), which might have allowed a rapid reversal of the altered frequency map during the period of normal sensory experience before the auditory cortex was mapped. To address this possibility, additional litters of animals (WT: n = 4; KO: n = 6) were exposed to 16 kHz continually from P9 until the day of AI mapping (P20-P25; Fig. 1C). A one-way ANOVA comparing 16 kHz representation in naive, early exposure, and early exposure immediately mapped WT animals reveal a significant effect (p = 0.0022), with post hoc analyses showing no significant differences between the two early exposure groups that were mapped at different ages. A similar ANOVA on the KO animals shows a nonsignificant effect (p = 0.135). Although the immediately mapped KO animals appear to show a slight increase in representation of 16 kHz, even an uncorrected comparison of the KO naive and immediately mapped animals did not show a significant effect (p = 0.0988).

Auditory brainstem response differences are not frequency specific

Fmr1 KO animals are impaired in experience-dependent regulation of potassium channels in the auditory brainstem (Strumbos et al., 2010). To investigate potential subcortical plasticity, we compared ABRs between WT and KO mice that were either naive or 16 kHz-exposed (P9–P20; Fig. 2A). Sound exposure reduced the ABR amplitude in WT mice and increased the amplitude in KO mice (genotype × exposure × frequency × intensity repeated measures four-way ANOVA: genotype effect, p = 0.038; exposure, p = 0.0069; their interaction, p < 0.0001; Fig. 2B). However, this effect does not appear to be frequency specific, as the frequency interactions with genotype (p = 0.53) and exposure (p = 0.41) are not significant. The ABR threshold was not different between the genotypes and was not altered by sound exposure (ANCOVA on genotype × exposure with frequency as a covariate: genotype, p = 0.587; exposure, p = 0.909; interaction, p = 0.0609; Fig. 2C).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

ABRs are altered in Fmr1 KO mice, but not in a frequency-specific manner. A, Example ABR traces recorded from WT and KO animals that were either naive (NV, left) or 16 kHz-exposed (EXP, right). Responses were activated by 16 kHz tone pips. B, Amplitudes of the five ABR waves recorded with 16 kHz tone pips at 70 dB. Sound exposure had different effects on WT and KO mice, increasing ABR amplitudes in KOs and reducing ABR amplitudes in WTs (genotype × exposure interaction, p < 0.0001). C, The ABR threshold was not different between the genotypes and was not altered by sound exposure. D, Latencies of wave I (left) and wave IV (right). Latency for wave I was shorter for KOs than WTs (p = 0.0073) and was not altered by sound exposure. Latency for wave IV was shortened by sound exposure in WTs, but delayed in KOs (genotype × exposure interaction, p < 0.001). These effects were consistent across frequency (8 and 16 kHz) and sound level (45 and 70 dB).

ABR latency was analyzed with two-way ANOVAs (genotype × exposure condition) for each discernible wave. In wave I, which represents early responses in the auditory nerve, KO mice had shorter latencies than WT mice and this difference was not altered by experience (genotype, p = 0.0073; sound exposure, p = 0.87; interaction, p = 0.55; Fig. 2D). No significant effects were found for wave II or wave III, which represent responses from the spiral ganglion and cochlear nucleus, respectively (data not shown). Sound exposure reduced latencies in the WT groups, but increased them in the KO groups for waves IV and V, which represent responses from the superior olivary complex and inferior colliculus, respectively (interaction, p < 0.001 and p = 0.006, respectively; Fig. 2D). These effects were consistently seen for both the exposed (16 kHz) and nonexposed (8 kHz) frequencies, and at 45 and 70 dB sound pressure levels (Fig. 2D). These results indicate that, although FMRP deficiency and sound exposure significantly impacted ABRs, the effects are frequency-nonspecific and therefore cannot account for frequency-specific map reorganization in AI. Furthermore, the ABRs in the KO mice are not grossly impaired, and the ABR differences probably do not account for the impairment in auditory map plasticity.

Daily injection of MPEP rescues plasticity deficit in KO mice

It has been suggested that fragile X phenotypes are related to overactive Group 1 metabotropic glutamate receptors (Bear et al., 2004). Indeed, genetic manipulations to suppress mGluR5 have been shown to rescue many fragile X phenotypes (Dölen et al., 2007). In addition, pharmacological suppression of mGluR5 using MPEP or CTEP has been shown to correct many deficits in both mouse and Drosophila melanogaster models of fragile X syndrome (McBride et al., 2005; Yan et al., 2005; de Vrij et al., 2008; Meredith et al., 2011; Su et al., 2011; Michalon et al., 2012; Thomas et al., 2012).

To investigate the role of mGluR in auditory critical period plasticity, we exposed additional litters of mice to 16 kHz during either the early (P9–P20) or late (P20–P30) window and systemically administered either MPEP or vehicle saline to littermates daily. We found that both MPEP- and saline-injected WT animals had larger representations of 16 kHz than naive WT animals when they were exposed in the early window, but not the late window (Fig. 3A). A one-way ANOVA on the WT animals across the five conditions revealed a significant effect (p = 0.023), with the early window saline and MPEP groups having significantly larger 16 kHz representations than the naive groups [p < 0.05 and p < 0.005, respectively; Fig. 3B; post hoc least significant difference (LSD)]. The two late window groups did not differ from the naive group, suggesting that saline or MPEP injection does not enhance plasticity outside the normal critical period in WT animals (p > 0.05). In the KO animals, only those exposed to 16 kHz between P9–P20 in conjunction with daily MPEP injection showed an increase in representation of 16 kHz (Fig. 3C). A one-way ANOVA on the KO animals across the five conditions revealed a significant effect (p = 0.0021), driven exclusively by the early window MPEP group (p < 0.001, compared with naive, post hoc LSD). The four remaining groups show no significant differences (p > 0.05, for all). Our data suggest that although the blockade of mGluR5 does not interfere with critical period map plasticity in WT animals, such a blockade is sufficient to rescue the plasticity deficit observed in KO animals within the classical critical period window.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

MPEP injections rescue plasticity deficit. A, Example maps of WT and KO animals injected daily with either vehicle saline or MPEP while being exposed to 16 kHz during an early window (P9–P20) or a late window (P20–P30). Areas outlined in black indicate sites with CFs near the exposure frequency of 16 kHz (±0.2 octaves), as in Figure 1. Example receptive fields are shown for each map; location of receptive the field is indicated by white roman numerals (x-axis is frequency from 2 to 74 kHz; y-axis is dB from 0 to 70 dB). Scale bar indicates 1 mm. B, Representation of 16 kHz in AI of WT animals. Naive group is the same as shown in Figure 1C. A significant increase in representation (compared with the naive group) was observed for both early window saline and MPEP injection groups. C, Representation of 16 kHz in AI of KO animals. Naive group is as shown in Figure 1C. A significant increase in representation of 16 kHz is seen only in the early window MPEP group; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.005, and ***p < 0.001 in post hoc LSD pairwise analyses with respective naive group.

We compared the overall size of AI and the response latency, response threshold, and bandwidth of AI neurons between the two genotypes and among the seven age-matched experimental conditions using two-way ANOVAs (Table 1). A significant main effect for rearing condition was found for both overall AI size (p = 0.0012) and latency (p = 0.036). These results indicate that sound exposure can lead to marginally smaller total AI area and faster response latency. No main effects for genotype or interaction were found for AI size or latency. No differences were observed in threshold or bandwidth, between genotypes or experimental conditions.

Discussion

In the present study, we have demonstrated that sound exposure-induced cortical map plasticity is severely impaired in the Fmr1 KO mouse and that this deficit can be rescued by the pharmacological blockade of mGluR5. These findings support the notion that impaired critical period plasticity leads to abnormal sensory processing in fragile X syndrome, which could then lead to impaired development of higher cognitive functions, such as language learning (LeBlanc and Fagiolini, 2011). The results are also consistent with a role of overactive mGluR functions in fragile X syndrome (Bear et al., 2004; Dölen et al., 2007).

In the present study, we observed grossly impaired frequency map plasticity in the Fmr1 KO animal. By contrast, whisker lesion-induced barrel map plasticity was found to be normal in Fmr1 KO mice (Harlow et al., 2010). Lid suture-induced ocular dominance plasticity has also been shown to be present, albeit altered, in Fmr1 KO mice (Dölen et al., 2007). These differences suggest that cortical map plasticity in different sensory systems is mediated by different cellular and synaptic mechanisms, only some of which involve Fmr1.

The normal emergence of the cortical frequency map and impaired critical period plasticity in the Fmr1 KO mouse indicates that the initial cortical development and subsequent plasticity in the critical period are mediated by different mechanisms. Electrophysiological studies have shown enhanced hippocampal long-term depression and deficient cortical long-term potentiation (LTP) in Fmr1 KO mice in visual and somatosensory cortices (Li et al., 2002; Zhao et al., 2005; Wilson and Cox, 2007). Spike timing-dependent synaptic potentiation was also impaired in the somatosensory cortex of Fmr1 KO mice, but spike timing-dependent synaptic depression was intact (Desai et al., 2006; Meredith et al., 2007). In addition, one form of homeostatic plasticity is impaired in hippocampal neurons of Fmr1 KO mice (Soden and Chen, 2010). The deficient cortical LTP may underlie the impaired frequency map plasticity observed in the present study.

The mechanisms by which MPEP rescues the impaired cortical frequency map plasticity are unknown. In WT animals, mGluR5 antagonists block mGluR-dependent LTP (Wang and Daw, 2003; Wilson and Cox, 2007). In Fmr1 KO mice, impaired cortical LTP may be due to mGluR5-mediated overproduction of proteins (Dölen et al., 2007; Dölen and Bear, 2008). mGluR5 antagonists may rescue impaired LTP in Fmr1 KO mice by reducing the protein overproduction. Alternatively, MPEP may also facilitate cortical map plasticity in Fmr1 KO mice by rebalancing excitation and inhibition in the cortical circuits (Chuang et al., 2005; Selby et al., 2007; Curia et al., 2009), which has been shown to regulate the critical period for monocular deprivation and underlie auditory cortical plasticity (Hensch, 2004; Dorrn et al., 2010).

Subcortical contributions to the induction and expression of cortical map plasticity are not entirely clear (Barkat et al., 2011; Oliver et al., 2011; Miyakawa et al., 2013). In this study, ABR amplitudes were reduced by sound exposure in WTs, but enhanced in KOs. These effects are consistent with abnormal gene regulation of the brainstem of Fmr1 KO mice (Strumbos et al., 2010). However, ABR differences were not specific for the exposure frequency; therefore, it is unlikely that the exposure-enhanced subcortical responses directly impaired cortical map plasticity in Fmr1 KO mice. Nevertheless, the interactions between the cortical and subcortical abnormalities remain to be investigated.

Because early experience-induced map reorganization has a long-lasting impact on sound perception, impaired critical period plasticity, if present in human fragile X patients, could plausibly result in the observed delay in language development (Finestack et al., 2009). The stimulus-nonspecific sensitization of brainstem responses observed in the present study could also result in hypersensitivity in fragile X patients and Fmr1 KO mice (Miller et al., 1999; Chen and Toth, 2001; Nielsen et al., 2002; Tsiouris and Brown, 2004). Further investigation of acoustic processing in Fmr1 KO mice may reveal how the gene mutation results in the documented sensory and cognitive abnormalities of fragile X syndrome patients.

Footnotes

  • This study was supported by National Institute on Deafness and Other Communicative Disorders (DC009259). H.K. was supported by the NSF GRFP and the NIDCD NRSA predoctoral fellowship. C.K. and R.G. were supported by the NSF GRFP. We thank L. Hamilton and A. Miyakawa for assistance with the electrophysiological recordings, animal husbandry, sound exposure, and injections.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Shaowen Bao, Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, CA 94720-3190. sbao{at}berkeley.edu

References

  1. ↵
    1. Barkat TR,
    2. Polley DB,
    3. Hensch TK
    (2011) A critical period for auditory thalamocortical connectivity. Nat Neurosci 14:1189–1194, doi:10.1038/nn.2882, pmid:21804538.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Bear MF,
    2. Huber KM,
    3. Warren ST
    (2004) The mGluR theory of fragile X mental retardation. Trends Neurosci 27:370–377, doi:10.1016/j.tins.2004.04.009, pmid:15219735.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Chen L,
    2. Toth M
    (2001) Fragile X mice develop sensory hyperreactivity to auditory stimuli. Neuroscience 103:1043–1050, doi:10.1016/S0306-4522(01)00036-7, pmid:11301211.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Chuang SC,
    2. Zhao W,
    3. Bauchwitz R,
    4. Yan Q,
    5. Bianchi R,
    6. Wong RK
    (2005) Prolonged epileptiform discharges induced by altered group I metabotropic glutamate receptor-mediated synaptic responses in hippocampal slices of a fragile X mouse model. J Neurosci 25:8048–8055, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1777-05.2005, pmid:16135762.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    1. Curia G,
    2. Papouin T,
    3. Séguéla P,
    4. Avoli M
    (2009) Downregulation of tonic GABAergic inhibition in a mouse model of fragile X syndrome. Cereb Cortex 19:1515–1520, doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn159, pmid:18787232.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    1. Desai NS,
    2. Casimiro TM,
    3. Gruber SM,
    4. Vanderklish PW
    (2006) Early postnatal plasticity in neocortex of Fmr1 knock-out mice. J Neurophysiol 96:1734–1745, doi:10.1152/jn.00221.2006, pmid:16823030.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    1. de Villers-Sidani E,
    2. Chang EF,
    3. Bao S,
    4. Merzenich MM
    (2007) Critical period window for spectral tuning defined in the primary auditory cortex (A1) in the rat. J Neurosci 27:180–189, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3227-06.2007, pmid:17202485.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    1. de Vrij FM,
    2. Levenga J,
    3. van der Linde HC,
    4. Koekkoek SK,
    5. De Zeeuw CI,
    6. Nelson DL,
    7. Oostra BA,
    8. Willemsen R
    (2008) Rescue of behavioral phenotype and neuronal protrusion morphology in Fmr1 KO mice. Neurobiol Dis 31:127–132, doi:10.1016/j.nbd.2008.04.002, pmid:18571098.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Dölen G,
    2. Bear MF
    (2008) Role for metabotropic glutamate receptor 5 (mGluR5) in the pathogenesis of fragile X syndrome. J Physiol 586:1503–1508, doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2008.150722, pmid:18202092.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    1. Dölen G,
    2. Osterweil E,
    3. Rao BS,
    4. Smith GB,
    5. Auerbach BD,
    6. Chattarji S,
    7. Bear MF
    (2007) Correction of fragile X syndrome in mice. Neuron 56:955–962, doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.12.001, pmid:18093519.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Dorrn AL,
    2. Yuan K,
    3. Barker AJ,
    4. Schreiner CE,
    5. Froemke RC
    (2010) Developmental sensory experience balances cortical excitation and inhibition. Nature 465:932–936, doi:10.1038/nature09119, pmid:20559387.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Finestack LH,
    2. Richmond EK,
    3. Abbeduto L
    (2009) Language development in individuals with fragile X syndrome. Top Lang Disord 29:133–148, doi:10.1097/TLD.0b013e3181a72016, pmid:20396595.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Han YK,
    2. Köver H,
    3. Insanally MN,
    4. Semerdjian JH,
    5. Bao S
    (2007) Early experience impairs perceptual discrimination. Nat Neurosci 10:1191–1197, doi:10.1038/nn1941, pmid:17660815.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Hanson DM,
    2. Jackson AW 3rd.,
    3. Hagerman RJ
    (1986) Speech disturbances (cluttering) in mildly impaired males with the Martin-Bell/fragile X syndrome. Am J Med Genet 23:195–206, doi:10.1002/ajmg.1320230114, pmid:3953648.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Harlow EG,
    2. Till SM,
    3. Russell TA,
    4. Wijetunge LS,
    5. Kind P,
    6. Contractor A
    (2010) Critical period plasticity is disrupted in the barrel cortex of FMR1 knockout mice. Neuron 65:385–398, doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.024, pmid:20159451.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Hensch TK
    (2004) Critical period regulation. Annu Rev Neurosci 27:549–579, doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144327, pmid:15217343.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Insanally MN,
    2. Köver H,
    3. Kim H,
    4. Bao S
    (2009) Feature-dependent sensitive periods in the development of complex sound representation. J Neurosci 29:5456–5462, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5311-08.2009, pmid:19403813.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    1. Insanally MN,
    2. Albanna BF,
    3. Bao S
    (2010) Pulsed noise experience disrupts complex sound representations. J Neurophysiol 103:2611–2617, doi:10.1152/jn.00872.2009, pmid:20200123.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. Jin P,
    2. Warren ST
    (2003) New insights into fragile X syndrome: from molecules to neurobehaviors. Trends Biochem Sci 28:152–158, doi:10.1016/S0968-0004(03)00033-1, pmid:12633995.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Kim H,
    2. Bao S
    (2009) Selective increase in representations of sounds repeated at an ethological rate. J Neurosci 29:5163–5169, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0365-09.2009, pmid:19386912.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    1. LeBlanc JJ,
    2. Fagiolini M
    (2011) Autism: A “critical period” disorder? Neural Plast 2011:921680, doi:10.1155/2011/921680, pmid:21826280.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Li J,
    2. Pelletier MR,
    3. Perez Velazquez JL,
    4. Carlen PL
    (2002) Reduced cortical synaptic plasticity and GluR1 expression associated with fragile X mental retardation protein deficiency. Mol Cell Neurosci 19:138–151, doi:10.1006/mcne.2001.1085, pmid:11860268.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. McBride SM,
    2. Choi CH,
    3. Wang Y,
    4. Liebelt D,
    5. Braunstein E,
    6. Ferreiro D,
    7. Sehgal A,
    8. Siwicki KK,
    9. Dockendorff TC,
    10. Nguyen HT,
    11. McDonald TV,
    12. Jongens TA
    (2005) Pharmacological rescue of synaptic plasticity, courtship behavior, and mushroom body defects in a Drosophila model of fragile X syndrome. Neuron 45:753–764, doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.01.038, pmid:15748850.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Meredith RM,
    2. Holmgren CD,
    3. Weidum M,
    4. Burnashev N,
    5. Mansvelder HD
    (2007) Increased threshold for spike-timing-dependent plasticity is caused by unreliable calcium signaling in mice lacking fragile X gene FMR1. Neuron 54:627–638, doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.04.028, pmid:17521574.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Meredith RM,
    2. de Jong R,
    3. Mansvelder HD
    (2011) Functional rescue of excitatory synaptic transmission in the developing hippocampus in Fmr1-KO mouse. Neurobiol Dis 41:104–110, doi:10.1016/j.nbd.2010.08.026, pmid:20817093.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Michalon A,
    2. Sidorov M,
    3. Ballard TM,
    4. Ozmen L,
    5. Spooren W,
    6. Wettstein JG,
    7. Jaeschke G,
    8. Bear MF,
    9. Lindemann L
    (2012) Chronic pharmacological mGlu5 inhibition corrects fragile X in adult mice. Neuron 74:49–56, doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.009, pmid:22500629.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Miller LJ,
    2. McIntosh DN,
    3. McGrath J,
    4. Shyu V,
    5. Lampe M,
    6. Taylor AK,
    7. Tassone F,
    8. Neitzel K,
    9. Stackhouse T,
    10. Hagerman RJ
    (1999) Electrodermal responses to sensory stimuli in individuals with fragile X syndrome: a preliminary report. Am J Med Genet 83:268–279, doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-8628(19990402)83:4<268::AID-AJMG7>3.0.CO%3B2-K, pmid:10208160.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Miyakawa A,
    2. Gibboni R,
    3. Bao S
    (2013) Repeated exposure to a tone transiently alters spectral tuning bandwidth of neurons in the central nucleus of inferior colliculus in juvenile rats. Neuroscience 230:114–120, doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.10.068, pmid:23168325.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Moy SS,
    2. Nadler JJ,
    3. Young NB,
    4. Nonneman RJ,
    5. Grossman AW,
    6. Murphy DL,
    7. D'Ercole AJ,
    8. Crawley JN,
    9. Magnuson TR,
    10. Lauder JM
    (2009) Social approach in genetically engineered mouse lines relevant to autism. Genes Brain Behav 8:129–142, doi:10.1111/j.1601-183X.2008.00452.x, pmid:19016890.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Nielsen DM,
    2. Derber WJ,
    3. McClellan DA,
    4. Crnic LS
    (2002) Alterations in the auditory startle response in Fmr1 targeted mutant mouse models of fragile X syndrome. Brain Res 927:8–17, doi:10.1016/S0006-8993(01)03309-1, pmid:11814427.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. O'Connor LH,
    2. Erway LC,
    3. Vogler CA,
    4. Sly WS,
    5. Nicholes A,
    6. Grubb J,
    7. Holmberg SW,
    8. Levy B,
    9. Sands MS
    (1998) Enzyme replacement therapy for murine mucopolysaccharidosis type VII leads to improvements in behavior and auditory function. J Clin Invest 101:1394–1400, doi:10.1172/JCI1773, pmid:9525982.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Oliver DL,
    2. Izquierdo MA,
    3. Malmierca MS
    (2011) Persistent effects of early augmented acoustic environment on the auditory brainstem. Neuroscience 184:75–87, doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.04.001, pmid:21496479.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Osterweil EK,
    2. Krueger DD,
    3. Reinhold K,
    4. Bear MF
    (2010) Hypersensitivity to mGluR5 and ERK1/2 leads to excessive protein synthesis in the hippocampus of a mouse model of fragile X syndrome. J Neurosci 30:15616–15627, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3888-10.2010, pmid:21084617.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    1. Popescu MV,
    2. Polley DB
    (2010) Monaural deprivation disrupts development of binaural selectivity in auditory midbrain and cortex. Neuron 65:718–731, doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.019, pmid:20223206.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Selby L,
    2. Zhang C,
    3. Sun QQ
    (2007) Major defects in neocortical GABAergic inhibitory circuits in mice lacking the fragile X mental retardation protein. Neurosci Lett 412:227–232, doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2006.11.062, pmid:17197085.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Soden ME,
    2. Chen L
    (2010) Fragile X protein FMRP is required for homeostatic plasticity and regulation of synaptic strength by retinoic acid. J Neurosci 30:16910–16921, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3660-10.2010, pmid:21159962.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    1. Strumbos JG,
    2. Brown MR,
    3. Kronengold J,
    4. Polley DB,
    5. Kaczmarek LK
    (2010) Fragile X mental retardation protein is required for rapid experience-dependent regulation of the potassium channel Kv3.1b. J Neurosci 30:10263–10271, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1125-10.2010, pmid:20685971.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  38. ↵
    1. Su T,
    2. Fan HX,
    3. Jiang T,
    4. Sun WW,
    5. Den WY,
    6. Gao MM,
    7. Chen SQ,
    8. Zhao QH,
    9. Yi YH
    (2011) Early continuous inhibition of group 1 mGlu signaling partially rescues dendritic spine abnormalities in the Fmr1 knockout mouse model for fragile X syndrome. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 215:291–300, doi:10.1007/s00213-010-2130-2, pmid:21181121.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Sun YJ,
    2. Wu GK,
    3. Liu BH,
    4. Li P,
    5. Zhou M,
    6. Xiao Z,
    7. Tao HW,
    8. Zhang LI
    (2010) Fine-tuning of prebalanced excitation and inhibition during auditory cortical development. Nature 465:927–931, doi:10.1038/nature09079, pmid:20559386.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Thomas AM,
    2. Bui N,
    3. Perkins JR,
    4. Yuva-Paylor LA,
    5. Paylor R
    (2012) Group I metabotropic glutamate receptor antagonists alter select behaviors in a mouse model for fragile X syndrome. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 219:47–58, doi:10.1007/s00213-011-2375-4, pmid:21656124.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Tsiouris JA,
    2. Brown WT
    (2004) Neuropsychiatric symptoms of fragile X syndrome: pathophysiology and pharmacotherapy. CNS Drugs 18:687–703, doi:10.2165/00023210-200418110-00001, pmid:15330685.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Wang XF,
    2. Daw NW
    (2003) Long term potentiation varies with layer in rat visual cortex. Brain Res 989:26–34, doi:10.1016/S0006-8993(03)03321-3, pmid:14519508.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Wilson BM,
    2. Cox CL
    (2007) Absence of metabotropic glutamate receptor-mediated plasticity in the neocortex of fragile X mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104:2454–2459, doi:10.1073/pnas.0610875104, pmid:17287348.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  44. ↵
    1. Yan QJ,
    2. Rammal M,
    3. Tranfaglia M,
    4. Bauchwitz RP
    (2005) Suppression of two major fragile X syndrome mouse model phenotypes by the mGluR5 antagonist MPEP. Neuropharmacology 49:1053–1066, doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2005.06.004, pmid:16054174.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Zhang LI,
    2. Bao S,
    3. Merzenich MM
    (2001) Persistent and specific influences of early acoustic environments on primary auditory cortex. Nat Neurosci 4:1123–1130, doi:10.1038/nn745, pmid:11687817.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Zhao MG,
    2. Toyoda H,
    3. Ko SW,
    4. Ding HK,
    5. Wu LJ,
    6. Zhuo M
    (2005) Deficits in trace fear memory and long-term potentiation in a mouse model for fragile X syndrome. J Neurosci 25:7385–7392, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1520-05.2005, pmid:16093389.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 33 (40)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 33, Issue 40
2 Oct 2013
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Impaired Critical Period Plasticity in Primary Auditory Cortex of Fragile X Model Mice
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Impaired Critical Period Plasticity in Primary Auditory Cortex of Fragile X Model Mice
Heesoo Kim, Robert Gibboni, Colleen Kirkhart, Shaowen Bao
Journal of Neuroscience 2 October 2013, 33 (40) 15686-15692; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3246-12.2013

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Impaired Critical Period Plasticity in Primary Auditory Cortex of Fragile X Model Mice
Heesoo Kim, Robert Gibboni, Colleen Kirkhart, Shaowen Bao
Journal of Neuroscience 2 October 2013, 33 (40) 15686-15692; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3246-12.2013
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Articles

  • Choice Behavior Guided by Learned, But Not Innate, Taste Aversion Recruits the Orbitofrontal Cortex
  • Maturation of Spontaneous Firing Properties after Hearing Onset in Rat Auditory Nerve Fibers: Spontaneous Rates, Refractoriness, and Interfiber Correlations
  • Insulin Treatment Prevents Neuroinflammation and Neuronal Injury with Restored Neurobehavioral Function in Models of HIV/AIDS Neurodegeneration
Show more Articles

Development/Plasticity/Repair

  • Cbln1 Directs Axon Targeting by Corticospinal Neurons Specifically toward Thoraco-Lumbar Spinal Cord
  • Loss of Motor Cortical Inputs to the Red Nucleus after CNS Disorders in Nonhuman Primates
  • Astrocytes Transplanted during Early Postnatal Development Integrate, Mature, and Survive Long Term in Mouse Cortex
Show more Development/Plasticity/Repair
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.