Abstract
Odors are highly evocative, yet how and where in the brain odors derive meaning remains unknown. Our analysis of the Drosophila brain extends the role of a small number of hunger-sensing neurons to include food-odor value representation. In vivo two-photon calcium imaging shows the amplitude of food odor-evoked activity in neurons expressing Drosophila neuropeptide F (dNPF), the neuropeptide Y homolog, strongly correlates with food-odor attractiveness. Hunger elevates neural and behavioral responses to food odors only, although food odors that elicit attraction in the fed state also evoke heightened dNPF activity in fed flies. Inactivation of a subset of dNPF-expressing neurons or silencing dNPF receptors abolishes food-odor attractiveness, whereas genetically enhanced dNPF activity not only increases food-odor attractiveness but promotes attraction to aversive odors. Varying the amount of presented odor produces matching graded neural and behavioral curves, which can function to predict preference between odors. We thus demonstrate a possible motivationally scaled neural “value signal” accessible from uniquely identifiable cells.
Introduction
Chemosensation is a critical regulator of food-seeking behaviors across animal species (Dethier, 1976; Wilson and Stevenson, 2006; Asahina et al., 2008; Chow and Frye, 2009). Food odor alone can trigger the appropriate approach or investigative behaviors essential for survival (Dobzhansky et al., 1956; Ruebenbauer et al., 2008). Yet very little is known about how the brain determines odor value, let alone of potential food sources specifically.
In flies, odor quality is first represented by the activation of an ensemble of olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs) and then by the distributed activity pattern of the projection neurons (PNs) within the primary olfactory processing center, the antennal lobe (AL) (Su et al., 2009). Specific subsets of cells at this stage are essential to produce odor-related approach or avoidance behaviors (Suh et al., 2004; Kreher et al., 2008; Semmelhack and Wang, 2009; Ai et al., 2010; Root et al., 2011); but because it is well documented that neurons at this level are broadly tuned (Wang et al., 2003; Wilson et al., 2004; Fishilevich and Vosshall, 2005; Lin et al., 2006; Kreher et al., 2008; Riffell et al., 2009; Johnson et al., 2010), any specific category or value information representation is likely to emerge only in later stages of the brain.
AL PNs bifurcate, projecting to two central brain structures: the Kenyon cells (KCs) of the mushroom body (MB), and the lateral horn neurons (LHNs) (Su et al., 2009). LHNs are broadly tuned (Gupta and Stopfer, 2012) and have been implicated in odor repulsion behavior (de Belle and Heisenberg, 1994; Wang et al., 2003). For these reasons, we turned our attention to the MB. MB responses are highly odor-specific, and there is evidence that KC activity is necessary for odor attraction (Wang et al., 2003; Honegger et al., 2011). In addition to the MB we also targeted neurons expressing Drosophila neuropeptide F (dNPF), the functional homolog of mammalian orexigenic neuropeptide Y (NPY) (Brown et al., 1999; Garczynski et al., 2002; Nassel and Wegener, 2011). Although not previously known to respond to odorant stimuli, the release of dNPF, and its mammalian counterpart NPY, is involved in appetite transduction and has been suggested as a hallmark of food deprivation in the brain (Flood and Morley, 1991; Bannon et al., 2000; Wu et al., 2003, 2005; Day et al., 2005; Krashes et al., 2009). Although dNPF/NPY secretion is considered a prominent correlate of the drive to eat, evidence suggests that, rather than driving food ingestion per se, these peptides are linked to motivational regulation of feeding (de Bono and Bargmann, 1998; Wu et al., 2003, 2005; Day et al., 2005; Keen-Rhinehart and Bartness, 2007; Xu et al., 2010) and more recently to the rewarding properties of stimuli in general (Shohat-Ophir et al., 2012).
By exploiting Drosophila melanogaster's natural and variable preference for differing food odors and coupling behavioral data with functional imaging and genetic manipulation, we show that, whereas KCs appear favorably tuned to code unique odor identity, odor-evoked dNPF activity is tightly correlated to food-odor attraction.
Materials and Methods
Transgenic flies.
D. melanogaster were reared on standard cornmeal agar medium with yeast additive. The following fly stocks were used: cry-Gal4/CyO, cry-Gal80/TM6B,D3, dNPF-Gal4, UAS-dTRPA1/dNPF-Gal4, dNPF2-Gal4, UAS-hid/CyO; dNPF2-Gal4, elav-Gal4, UAS-GCaMP3;OK107-Gal4, UAS-hid/CyO, UAS-mcd8::GFP;MB247DsRED, UAS-npfr1dsRNA, UAS-KIR2.1ts, and UAS-dTRPA1. Flies were raised at 25°C, excepting crosses and controls used for temperature-sensitive experiments, which were raised at 20°C.
Odor stimuli.
Odors were delivered using custom-built olfactometers at a flow rate of 100 ml/min from 100 ml vials. For consistency, identical olfactometers were used in both in vivo imaging and behavioral experiments. Complex natural food odors: yeast (1 g in 5 ml water; Saf-Instant yeast, Lesaffre Yeast), banana powder (3 g in 5 ml water), apple cider vinegar (1:2 water dilution; Foodhold), lemon juice (100%, Lakewood Organic), apple juice (100%, Lakewood Organic), mango juice blend (100%, Lakewood Organic), monomolecular components of yeast as determined previously by gas chromatograph analysis (Lin and Phelan, 1991): acetaldehyde (1:1000 dilution), 2-methylbutanol (1:1000), and 1-propanol (1:1000), further monomolecular odors: 3-octanol (1:1000), 4-methylcyclohexanol (1:1000), ethyl acetate (1:1000), and allyl cyclohexyl propionate (ach) (1:100). Two odors were non-naturally occurring synthetic monomolecular odors: propyl nonanoate (1:100), or “synthetic yeast,” described as having a fermented and yeasty scent, and butylated hydroxyanisole (1 g in 5 ml water), or “rubber,” described as having a mild rubbery scent. Two odors were complex nonfood odor mixtures: pine needle extract (1:100; Givaudan) and motor oil (Penzoil). Male pheromone is a cis-vaccenyl-acetate solution in ethanol (Cayman Chemical), and ethanol vehicle control is at a comparable dilution (1:100). All dilutions are in made in mineral oil unless otherwise stated. All odors are purchased from Sigma-Aldrich unless otherwise stated. For neurometric/psychometric experiments, odor concentrations are expressed as microliters of odorant applied to 4.25-cm-diameter filter paper and placed in 100 ml vials and delivered as above. Odor amount ranged from 0.1 to 1000 μl applied.
In vivo imaging experiments.
Live-fly preparation for calcium imaging followed that previous published (Honegger et al., 2011), using female flies. Flies in the starved condition were transferred to vials containing 4.25-cm-diameter filter paper (Whatman) saturated with ∼1 ml water ≥24 h before experiment onset. Images were acquired using a custom-built two-photon laser scanning microscope implementing a Chameleon Ti:sapphire laser (Coherent) tuned to a wavelength of 910 nm and a 40× water-immersion objective lens (0.80 numerical aperture; Olympus). Recordings were taken from a single optical section of the fly brain in which both the peduncle of the MB and the ipsilateral medial dNPF neuron were visible. The 256 × 256 pixel images were acquired using Fluoview 300 software (Olympus America) at 2 Hz with 4.5× digital magnification. Odor stimuli were delivered for 3 s each from inert tubing located ∼8 mm from the antennae. Presentation order for odor delivery was determined at random by sorting in Excel (Microsoft). Each sample represents a different fly, except where noted for repeated presentations of the same odor in Figure 2e, f. All testing occurred between 8 A.M. and 8 P.M.
Behavioral attraction assays.
Testing was performed in a 25°C (or 18–20/30°C, for selective inactivation and activation experiments) room with 70% relative humidity. A custom-built four-field olfactometer chamber was used to measure the response of flies to a series of odors. The circular chamber had a radius of 5.5 cm and was 2 mm deep, with four odor ports located equidistant along the diameter, illuminated from the bottom by a panel of light emitting diodes (MXHC). Plain or odorized air was delivered with dedicated lines at a flow rate of 100 ml/min from 100 ml vials, and a central vacuum suction accessed the chamber from below. Flies in the fed condition were transferred to vials containing 4.25-cm-diameter filter paper saturated with 1 ml water 60–90 min before testing, accounting for the live-fly imaging preparation period, thus making time of last food access consistent for both imaging and behavioral experiments. For starved flies, transfer to such vials occurred ≥24 h before testing. For each experiment ∼30 flies of either sex (3–4 d of age after eclosion) were loaded into the center of the chamber from above. Additional experiments were performed on separate groups of male and female flies to ensure there were no sex-specific differences in behavior. Flies were given 15 min to accommodate to the chamber before the onset of each experiment. Each experiment was 10 min in duration, and 240 × 320 pixel images were acquired at 15 fps using a Logitech HD Pro C910 webcam and Logitech Webcam Software 2.0. Control experiments consisted of plain, filtered air delivered to all four odor ports concurrently. For odor attraction experiments, one of the panel of 19 odors was delivered to one odor port with remaining ports delivering filtered air. For odor preference experiments, two odors were presented simultaneously (yeast vs banana, yeast vs acv, banana vs acv) at two of the four odor ports with the remaining ports delivering filtered air, only starved flies were used for preference experiments. For genetic manipulation experiments, each odor was delivered to one port with remaining ports delivering filtered air. The location of the odorized port(s) was determined at random for each experiment by sorting Excel (Microsoft). Degree of odor-associated attraction was evaluated as the percentage of flies in the odorized quadrant relative to the total number of flies in the chamber across the experiment duration. These measurements were obtained every 10 s for the 10 min experiment duration and then averaged to yield a single value indicating overall quadrant occupancy. The chamber was divided into four quadrants associated with each of the four air streams; and as such, 25% occupancy was equivalent to chance performance. Each sample represents a different group of flies. All testing occurred between 8 A.M. and 8 P.M.
Ingestion assay.
The circular chamber had a radius of 5.5 cm and was 6 mm deep, illuminated from the bottom by a panel of light-emitting diodes (MXHC). Flies were transferred to vials containing 4.25-cm-diameter filter paper saturated with 1 ml water 24 h before testing. Circular filter paper 7 mm in diameter was infused with 20 μl of blue-dyed odorant (Erioglaucine disodium salt for 18 of 19 odors) mixed in water (with the exception of motor oil mixed with Sudan Blue II; Sigma-Aldrich) to achieve the same concentration used in the behavioral attraction assay and placed in the center of the chamber. For each experiment, ∼30 flies of either sex (aged 3–4 d after eclosion) were loaded into the chamber from the side. Each experiment was 20 min in duration; and after this time period, flies were anesthetized with CO2 and visually inspected for the presence of blue dye in the abdomen. Each sample represents a different group of flies. All testing occurred between 8 A.M. and 8 P.M.
Data analysis.
Analyses for both imaging and behavioral data were performed using custom scripts written in MATLAB (MathWorks). For calcium imaging, data were preprocessed to minimize lateral motion artifacts, correct photo-bleaching, and specify ROIs for further analysis. Change in fluorescence (ΔF/F) was determined relative to baseline fluorescence during the first five frames of recording for each ROI. The area underneath the ΔF/F time course between 0 and 5 s after odor delivery was integrated to determine the odor-associated responsiveness for each ROI. For KC analysis, significantly responding cells were computed as previously reported (Honegger et al., 2011). Multidimensional scaling and hierarchical analysis were performed within each fly on the odor-responses of the entire population of KCs (∼150 per experiment). Hierarchical analysis to obtain the presented dendrograms was performed on the complete KC dataset. MDS was used only for visualization of the patterns of responses in 3-space. For the behavioral assay, fly coordinate information was determined every 10 s, and the percentage of flies within a specified ROI (e.g., the odorized quadrant) was determined. The analyzed quadrant in control experiments was determined at random. Two-dimensional histograms were calculated to visualize location distributions across the entire 10 min testing period.
Statistical analyses.
Statistical analyses were performed with SPSS (SPSS). Sample sizes were determined with anticipated large effect size and desired statistical power level of 0.8. Large effect sizes were confirmed post hoc using Cohen's d. Normality was assessed with Levene's test for equality of variance, and parametric testing was determined appropriate. Overall, ANOVAs, when significant, were followed by Dunnett's post hoc test when contrasts were made in reference to a control group mean; and for all other pairwise contrasts, multiplicity-adjusted p values were obtained using Dunn-Sidak's Multiple-Comparisons Test. All tests were two-tailed. Significance level was set at p < 0.05.
Results
Food odors have different attraction, which is altered by motivational state
We first established D. melanogaster's partiality for differing food odors. For consistency, we used flies of the same genotype to be used in imaging experiments (dNPF-Gal4/UAS-GCaMP3;OK107-Gal4). Flies were placed in a custom-built behavioral chamber (Fig. 1a) and exposed to filtered air delivered to four ports with one of them, randomly determined, carrying odorized air in the experimental conditions. Degree of attraction to an odor stream was determined by calculating the percentage of flies occupying the odorized quadrant, or a randomly determined quadrant for control experiments, every 10 s across the 10 min experiment duration and then averaged to yield a single percentage for each session. Flies with no partiality were expected to distribute randomly about the four quadrants in the chamber; and, as such, chance performance was set at 25% occupancy. We tested six complex, natural food odor stimuli: yeast (a major food source for D. melanogaster) (Dobzhansky et al., 1956; Powell, 1997), lemon, apple, mango, banana, and apple cider vinegar (acv).
In addition to the six food odors, we tested 13 nonfood control odors. Food and nonfood categories were experimentally determined using a separate ingestion assay in which flies could access and ingest dye-infused odors on filter paper (Fig. 1c). Three of the control odors that we found did not elicit feeding were main volatile monomolecular components of yeast: 1-propanol, methybutanol, and acetaldehyde (Lin and Phelan, 1991). Additionally, the synthetic monomolecular compound propyl nonanoate, described as having a “yeasty” aroma, was also tested. Further controls included another synthetic monomolecular compound not thought to be food-related (butylated hydroxyanisole, “rubber”) and two complex nonfood odors (pine needle extract and motor oil), lest attraction behavior was driven simply by the complexity of the natural food odors, being composed of many volatile components (Lin and Phelan, 1991; Stensmyr et al., 2003), and not their association with food. As a positive control, we also tested the volatile male-specific pheromone 11-cis-vaccenyl-acetate (cVA) using groups of male, mated female, and virgin female flies. cVA increases female receptivity to male courtship advances and is reported to increase aggregation behaviors for both male and female flies, although measured using trap assays over relatively long time periods (Bartelt et al., 1985; Xu et al., 2005; Kurtovic et al., 2007). Additionally, to determine whether flies were biased to occupy specific locations within the chamber unrelated to odor presence, in control experiments flies were exposed to filtered air delivered to each of the four ports. We first tested fed flies and then altered motivational state by starving flies for 24 h.
Flies displayed different levels of attraction to individual odors (Fig. 1b,d) consistent with previously published reports using the rather disparate t-maze and 24 h trap assays (Tully and Quinn, 1985; Knaden et al., 2012). Three of the 13 control odors produced unanticipated attraction behavior. Delivery of our predicted positive control odor, cVA, also produced a clear, and sexually dimorphic, attraction behavior. Female flies, regardless of their sexual experience, displayed a preference for the quadrant odorized with cVA, but distributed randomly for its vehicle (ethanol) presented in isolation (Fig. 1d). Importantly, of the four nonfood odors that elicited attraction, none was ingested by flies when given access, nor did they elicit increases in attraction upon starvation.
In contrast, food odors were more consistently attractive and state-dependent. Four of six food odors elicited significant attraction in the fed state. All six food odors were attractive to starved flies. Attraction degree was immediate and persistent for the 10 min experiment duration and did not differ between males and females (data not shown). Interestingly, attraction behavior was not uniform across individual food odors. For fed flies, attraction was mild, although significant for three of five fruity odors (apple, mango, and banana) and was more apparent for yeast. After flies were starved, attraction to all six food odors was significant (two-way ANOVA, satiety: F(1,8) = 60.78, odor: F(18,144) = 22.42, satiety × odor: F(18,144) = 3.92, all p < 0.0001); and as with fed flies, attraction level differed consistently between individual odors with yeast still the most attractive odor.
Behavior is not predicted by KC population responses
We next sought to determine the brain area responsible for our observed behavior. To accomplish this, neural activity needed to account for both the behavioral differences observed to individual odors and the changes in behavior due to starvation. We first examined odor-evoked activity of the KCs of the MB (Fig. 2a). We targeted expression of the genetically encoded calcium indicator GCaMP3 (Tian et al., 2009) to an MB-specific Gal4 line, OK107 (Connolly et al., 1996; Aso et al., 2009). Using two-photon laser scanning microscopy in the live fly, we characterized the responses of populations of KCs to a subset of monomolecular and food odors (Lin and Phelan, 1991).
Patterns of odor-evoked responses were clearly observable with single-cell resolution and differed among the odors presented (Fig. 2b). In agreement with previously published work using flies carrying the same transgenes (Honegger et al., 2011), responses to complex natural food odors did not differ from those elicited by monomolecular odors either in terms of the proportion of significantly responding cells (Fig. 2c) or in the overall magnitude of the response (Fig. 2d). Additionally, these characteristics of the odor-evoked response did not differ between fed and starved flies (two-way ANOVA, satiety: F(1,6) = 3.37, p = 0.07; odor: F(5,30) = 13.39, p < 0.0001; satiety × odor: F(5,30) = 1.73, p = 0.15). Because the relevant factor in the KC population response may be the pattern of active cells, rather than the number or magnitude of response, we created a representation of odor space using hierarchical cluster analysis based on the angular distances between odors tested (Fig. 2e,f). We then used multidimensional scaling to reduce the data's complexity to visualize odor-response patterns for individual flies in 3D space (Fig. 2e,f). Angular distance was used because it is reportedly more sensitive to patterns of responding cells rather than magnitude of responses (Kreher et al., 2008). Repeated presentations of the same odor clustered together, verifying the robustness of this analysis, although there was no evidence that food odors formed a distinct category in either fed or starved flies. Together, regardless of the manner by which we assess KC responses, we see no simple relationship between KC activity and the food-odor behavior we report.
dNPF odor-evoked activity is highly correlated with food-odor attractiveness
We then examined whether dNPF neurons respond to odors and their response specificity, specifically to food-related odors. We targeted expression of GCaMP3 to four dNPF-immunoreactive neurons in the dorsal protocerebrum, using a dNPF promoter-driven Gal4 (Wu et al., 2003; Krashes et al., 2009) (Fig. 3a), and simultaneously to OK107 (Connolly et al., 1996; Aso et al., 2009) (Fig. 3b). This allowed us to monitor one medial dNPF neuron and concurrent activity in the ipsilateral MB peduncle, formed by the axons of KCs, as an internal control for the viability of our imaging preparation (Fig. 3c,d). The intact, live fly preparation limited our access to the two lateral dNPF neurons. However, in using this preparation, we were able to maintain the integrity of the whole animal, critical when studying questions of stimulus attraction or context dependence in brain function, and allowed us to identically match stimulus presentation parameters for both behavioral and neural experiments.
The dNPF neuron responded to most odors, although with a wide range of activity levels (Fig. 3e–g). On average, responses were significantly higher to food odors than to nonfood odors. Average responses to food odors were also significantly higher than nonfood odors (two-way ANOVA, category: F(1,34) = 45.485, p < 0.000), even in fed flies (food vs nonfood, fed flies only: t = 3.211, p = 0.005). Notably, not only did the dNPF neuron respond to food odors, it did so in a satiety-dependent manner (two-way ANOVA, satiety: F(1,34) = 10.889, p = 0.003, satiety × category: F(1,34) = 4.376, p = 0.035) (Fig. 3e–g). Five of six complex natural food odors elicited state-dependent responsiveness with significantly higher activity observed in starved flies, whereas 12 of the 13 nonfood odors remained unchanged with the exception of 1-propanol. Furthermore, the dNPF neuron showed varying degrees of responsiveness to different food odors with the relationship between odors typically preserved in both fed and starved flies (e.g., yeast>banana>acv).
Remarkably, the response amplitude of the dNPF neurons was highly correlated to the food-odor attractiveness determined behaviorally: the larger the dNPF response, the stronger the attraction (r = 0.956, p < 0.0001, across food odors and satiety states; Fig. 3h). Although the correlation between dNPF activation and attraction to food odors was particularly strong in the starved state (r = 0.98, p < 0.0001), it was significant when considering only data from the fed state (r = 0.77, p = 0.045). In contrast, although responses in the peduncle were robust to all odors tested, activity was unrelated to both dNPF and behavioral profiles with no state-dependent observed food: (r = −0.156, p = 0.46; Fig. 3i). Neither dNPF nor peduncle activity correlated with attraction to nonfood odors (dNPF: r = −0.305, p = 0.166; peduncle: r = −0.196, p = 0.53; Fig. 3h,i).
Given the strong correlation between food-odor attraction and the amplitude of dNPF activity, a threshold can be drawn to determine the level of dNPF activity necessary to elicit food-odor attraction behavior (dashed line Fig. 3g). Food odors that produced attraction behavior were anticipated to elicit activity levels above this threshold. Nonfood odors that produced attraction behavior (e.g., the sex pheromone cVA) were not anticipated to elicit activity levels above this threshold. Not a single nonfood odor activity significantly surpassed this threshold, including the four nonfood odors that elicited behavioral attraction (Fig. 1d). In contrast, it is important to note that all food odors that surpassed this threshold produced behavioral attraction, including half of the food odors in the fed state. This observation supports the ideas that dNPF activity: (1) relates to attractiveness exclusively for food odors and (2) is not simply a hunger signal.
Activity of the dNPF neuron is both necessary and sufficient to define food-odor attractiveness
To ascertain whether the dNPF neuron is necessary in mediating our observed attraction to food odors, we expressed the inward-rectifying potassium channel KIR2.1, which inhibits the generation of action potentials (Baines et al., 2001), in dNPF neurons. KIR2.1 was expressed under temperature-sensitive Gal80ts control to alleviate possible developmental confounds. At 20°C, Gal80ts inhibits Gal4 activity, blocking KIR2.1 expression, whereas at 30°C Gal80ts is inactivated, releasing Gal4-driven KIR2.1 expression (McGuire et al., 2004). Parental controls displayed typical attraction levels to yeast, the most preferred odor in our panel, in the starved condition regardless of alterations in temperature (Fig. 4a,b). We next tested dNPF-Gal4/UAS-KIR2.1Gal80ts flies' attraction to yeast. At permissive temperatures (20°C), dNPF-Gal4 /UAS-KIR2.1Gal80ts flies displayed a normal attraction profile. However, after 30°C heat shock (3 d) to express the channels attraction to yeast was suppressed with the percentage of flies in the odorized quadrant reaching only chance levels (paired t test, t = 0.05, p = 0.95).
As dNPF is expressed in ∼20–25 neurons in the adult brain (Lee et al., 2006), we next sought to determine whether specifically the subset of four dNPF neurons we have both monitored and manipulated are required for food-odor attraction. Of the dNPF neurons present in both males and females (Lee et al., 2006), dNPF neurons might be broadly classified into two major distinct groups on the basis of soma size and localization: (1) the four large dNPF-positive neurons, labeled by the so-called dNPF-Gal4 used in this study and characterized in others (Shen and Cai, 2001; Wu et al., 2003; Krashes et al., 2009), are nicely spaced out across the two hemispheres with one large dorsomedial neuron and one large lateral neuron per hemisphere; and (2) approximately seven smaller dNPF- and cryptochrome (cry)-positive neurons clustered laterally in each hemisphere, which can be visualized and targeted, together with the four large dNPF-positive neurons we describe, by what we will term for clarity in this study as the dNPF2-Gal4 line, previously characterized (Lee et al., 2006; Hermann et al., 2012). We expressed the apoptotic gene head-involution defective (hid) using the dNPF2-Gal4 line to genetically ablate ∼20 dNPF neurons per brain (McNabb et al., 1997). Genetic ablation of this larger complement of dNPF-positive cells led to a complete reduction in yeast odor attraction in starved flies (paired t test, vs plain air, dNPF2/hid: t = 0.23, p = 0.82; Fig. 4c,d). Because the seven smaller lateral dNPF-positive cells are cry-positive and the four larger dNPF-positive cells are not (Hermann et al., 2012), we were able to repress the expression of hid specifically in these cells while retaining ablation of just the four large dNPF-positive cells by using cry-Gal80. Flies expressing UAS-hid/cry-Gal80; dNPF2-Gal4 phenocopied dNPF2-Gal4/UAS-hid flies with a complete loss of attraction to yeast (paired t test, vs plain air, dNPF2;hid/cryGal80: t = 0.97, p = 0.38). However, ablation of the cry-positive cells only (cry-Gal4/UAS-hid) did not alter attraction to yeast (paired t test, vs plain air, cryGal4/hid: t = 12.56, p < 0.0001), suggesting the subset of four large dNPF neurons upon which we report are the cells relevant for food-odor attraction.
Additionally, to determine the role of the dNPF peptide specifically, we knocked down dNPF receptors across the fly brain. Silencing dNPF receptors (elav-Gal4/UAS-npfr1dsRNA) phenocopied both the transient inactivation and genetic ablation of the four large dNPF-positive neurons (Fig. 4e,f). Attraction to yeast was abolished in starved flies (paired t test, vs plain air, t = 0.75, p = 0.49). This supports our theory that dNPF neuropeptides are critical in mediating the food-odor attraction we observed.
The above experiments show that dNPF is a critical part of the circuitry underlying food-odor attraction because silencing these neurons abolishes attraction to even the most attractive food odor on our panel. We next wanted to demonstrate that we can influence the flies' attraction behavior in a predictable manner by amplifying the activity of dNPF neurons in fed flies. To determine sufficiency, we selectively activated dNPF neurons by ectopically expressing the D. melanogaster transient receptor potential (TRP) channel dTRPA1, which excites target cells by conducting Ca2+ at temperatures >25°C (Hamada et al., 2008), in dNPF neurons. Stimulation of dNPF neurons at 30°C led UAS-dTRPA1/+;dNPF-Gal4 flies to display increased levels of odor attraction relative to temperature and parental controls (two-way ANOVA, temperature, F(1,18) = 74.8, p < 0.0001; odor, F(2,54) = 40.24, p < 0.0001; temperature × odor, F(2,54) = 6.34, p = 0.003; Fig. 5a,b). Asymptotic attraction was elicited toward the nonoptimal food odor acv, which normally generates only modest attraction even in the starved state (Fig. 1d). Additionally, attraction was promoted to normally slightly attractive (ethyl acetate) and aversive (2-methylbutanol) monomolecular odors (Fig. 5b). Attraction profiles for individual odors after selective activation appeared to reflect a summation of dTRPA1- and normal odor-evoked activity (Fig. 3g). Increases in odor attraction at 30°C also corresponded to increases in the level of dTRPA1-evoked dNPF activation as flies carrying two dNPF-Gal4 transgenes displayed stronger attraction to the monomolecular odors relative to flies carrying only one copy of the dNPF-Gal4 transgene (combined with UAS-dTRPA1) (Fig. 5b). Investigating the effect of further amplification of dNPF activity was not possible as flies homozygous for both dNPF-Gal4 and UAS-dTRPA1 displayed marked attraction to all odors in a non–temperature-sensitive fashion. We observed a similar phenotype for flies homozygous for UAS-dTRPA1 in the absence of a Gal4 (data not shown).
The level of dNPF activity predicts food odor preference when two odors are presented in competition
Because flies responded to individual food odors in differing degrees, with an intensity highly correlated to the level of dNPF activation, we speculated that the activity of the dNPF neuron may be used to encode the relative attractiveness of the encountered odors on a common internal scale. Preference for one potential food odor option over another, then, would be determined by comparing levels of dNPF activity elicited by each candidate odor and then pursuing that with the greater activity amount. To assess preference, we first presented two different odors simultaneously. Flies now had to choose between two odors that were both previously deemed attractive when presented alone (vs filtered air). Indeed, the neural data could be used to predict to which odor the flies were more attracted. Yeast, the odor with the highest dNPF activation (see Fig. 3e–g), was chosen over other food odors when presented in competition. Banana and acv were mainly ignored whether presented in the company of yeast. Flies quickly moved toward the yeast odor and remained there for the experiment duration (Fig. 6a,b,d,e). When the less preferred odors were presented in competition, banana was preferred over acv as predicted from the difference in dNPF response amplitude (Fig. 6c,f). Looking at individual experiment traces (Fig. 6c), flies appeared to oscillate between the two odors as though determining odor desirability was more difficult when differences in the dNPF responses were modest.
Next, we generated a neurometric curve by varying the concentration of yeast odor delivered to the flies and examining changes in dNPF responses relative to the response to a single concentration of acv. We then tested behavioral attraction to corresponding concentrations of both yeast and acv to generate a psychometric curve. Finally, we asked whether differences in concentration-dependent yeast-evoked dNPF responses could predict preference between attraction to acv and yeast at these differing concentrations. Increased concentrations of yeast produced higher levels of dNPF activity (Fig. 6g). The acv-evoked response fell within the graded responses to yeast. The psychometric curve mirrored the neurometric curve (Fig. 6h). Additionally, comparison of the differing levels of dNPF activity elicited by differing concentrations of yeast relative to that elicited by a single concentration of acv could be used to predict to which odor the flies would be more attracted (Fig. 6i). When presented simultaneously, yeast was chosen over acv only when a given concentration of yeast elicited dNPF activity levels higher than that elicited by acv. Flies chose acv over yeast when yeast concentration was sufficiently low to produce a level of dNPF activity below that produced by acv. Together, these data show that, although odor-evoked activity in dNPF neurons is correlated with attraction to differing odors, activity also can be graded within a single odor and can be used to predict the degree of preference an odor holds over rival odors.
Discussion
The assessment of potential food sources provides an expedient framework to address value representation in the brain given food-seeking's ubiquity in nature. Most animals, including D. melanogaster, are extremely discerning about what food sources to approach, even when given the choice between multiple viable options (Dobzhansky et al., 1956; Ruebenbauer et al., 2008), and odors are one of the most important sensory cues all animals use to track, evaluate, and select among available foods (Dethier, 1976; Wilson and Stevenson, 2006; Asahina et al., 2008; Chow and Frye, 2009). For generalist species that have broad niches and varied diets, such as fruit files or primates, the problem of food odor approach is complicated by the need to select the best possible from many options over a variety of environments (Keller, 2007). This requires flexibility from location to location and thus may not be accounted for by hardwiring at the brain's periphery. We report that differing food odors provoke varying levels of approach in D. melanogaster and that this approach is altered as a function of context (Fig. 1). Remarkably, the range of behavior we observe within and between food odors can be accounted for by looking at the odor-evoked activity of dNPF neurons within the central brain (Figs. 3 and 6), demonstrating the possibility of one of perhaps several discrete brain sites that explicitly encodes odor value.
We show that dNPF neural activity and food odor behavior are strongly correlated: the greater the odor-evoked dNPF response, the greater the attraction to that odor (Figs. 1 and 3). Additionally, the graded responsiveness of this neuron can be used to predict choice when two odors are in competition where the option with the higher activity level is preferred (Fig. 6). dNPF neurons respond more to food odors and activity levels are also augmented to account for internal context, increasing with hunger, which is reflected by corresponding changes in behavior (Figs. 1 and 3). The responses of the subset of dNPF neurons we monitored through functional imaging are essential to mediate the observed food-odor attraction behavior (Fig. 4) and sufficient to promote odor attraction to monomolecular odorants, even those normally considered aversive (Fig. 5).
The evoked activity represents neither individual odor identity nor the global state of hunger in the brain. The levels of neural and behavioral responses observed for acv in the starved condition and yeast in the fed condition illustrate these points nicely. Both stimulate the same level of activity and command the same degree of attraction yet differ with respect to odor identity and satiety state. Additionally, higher levels of activity (and attraction behavior) are present independent of whether the food odor was one with which the flies had prior experience (yeast, comprising part of the standard medium on which flies are reared) or was novel (2 of 5 and 5 of 5 fruity odors, in the fed and starved states, respectively) (Fig. 3g). The data also suggest a threshold of dNPF activity necessary to elicit significant food-odor attraction behavior. From responses that are significantly higher for the food odor category to sufficiency in mediating food odor-like behavioral responses to previously unattractive odors, the dNPF neuron meets all outlined criteria circumscribing odor value coding.
A novel role for neuropeptide signaling
dNPF, and the mammalian homolog NPY (Brown et al., 1999; Garczynski et al., 2002; Nassel and Wegener, 2011), has long been known to play a role in establishing hunger/satiety states and as such to play a critical role in regulating motivational aspects of food consumption (de Bono and Bargmann, 1998; Wu et al., 2003, 2005; Day et al., 2005; Keen-Rhinehart and Bartness, 2007; Krashes et al., 2009; Xu et al., 2010). Indeed, recent work demonstrates that dNPF is involved in odor cue-driven feeding (Wang et al., 2013) and reward processing in general (Shohat-Ophir et al., 2012). However, identifying that dNPF correlates with attraction level to, and even preference between, individual food odors sheds new light on the manner by which it may regulate food-related behaviors and suggests that neuromodulators in general may play a more explicit role in sensory coding than previously understood. Our approach allows us to further commonly understood roles for neuromodulators in sensory processing, suggesting that they are not solely a gate, recruiting or suppressing circuit firing. Rather, we have identified graded levels of activity from neuromodulatory cells that can be directly associated with odor value as it relates to food-seeking behavior.
Selective processing in the early versus central brain
The vast majority of neurons in the early olfactory system of both mammals and invertebrates are broadly tuned, responding to a large range of differing types of odorants (Wang et al., 2001, 2003; Fishilevich and Vosshall, 2005; Wilson and Mainen, 2006; Lin et al., 2006; Kreher et al., 2008; Riffell et al., 2009; Johnson et al., 2010). These nonspecific response profiles mean that individual ORNs or PNs in D. melanogaster that respond to a behaviorally attractive odor can also respond with equal strength to a behaviorally aversive odor (Wang et al., 2001, 2003; Fishilevich and Vosshall, 2005; Kreher et al., 2008; Semmelhack and Wang, 2009). Although emerging evidence does suggest that there may be a broad topographic representation of attractive and aversive odors across multiple glomeruli within the AL (Knaden et al., 2012),odor representations at this level appear distributed, with behavior best predicted by examining the activity of the full complement of ORNs or PNs, for larva and adults, respectively (Kreher et al., 2008; Knaden et al., 2012).
In contrast, in individual dNPF neurons, all nonfood odors tested, including single components of food odors or attractive nonfood odors, evoke only nominal levels of activity, whereas natural food odors evoke levels of activity that correlate strongly to food-seeking behavior. This is most apparent in hungry flies, where both neural activity and behavioral attraction are increased, but is also true of fed flies responding to highly attractive food odors. Activity levels in dNPF neurons correspond to a range of attraction to a range of different food odors. Activity levels elicited by individual odors can even be used to predict food odor preference when two attractive odors are presented in competition with one another. These properties of dNPF activity: directly reflect stimulus appeal (Lebreton et al., 2009; Jenison et al., 2011), predict preference when faced with competing options (Tremblay and Schultz, 1999; Padoa-Schioppa and Assad, 2006), and account for changes in motivational context, such as hunger, that affect behavior (Montague and King-Casas, 2007; Rangel et al., 2008; Grabenhorst and Rolls, 2011), making it suitable to explicitly encode value, specifically of food odors.
In characterizing the specificity of dNPF activity to specifically food-odor attraction behavior, we are able to support the idea that there may be discrete value-processing centers specialized to assess only a particular type of stimulus, even within the fly brain. We speculate that separate sets of neurons may responsible for, for example, sex-related value and thus mediate attraction behavior to the pheromone cVA, whereas other neuronal subsets may be responsible for attraction to other categories of odors.
Central brain cells with the comprehensive food-odor valuation response properties of the dNPF neuron are likely not limited to D. melanogaster, although as yet have not been isolated. In monkeys and rats, populations of lateral hypothalamic and frontal cortical neurons respond to food-related anticipatory cues in a satiety-state-dependent manner (Rolls, 2002). In humans, evidence suggests that there may be significant overlap in areas activated by food odors and areas responsible for processing the hedonic value of chemosensory stimuli (Small et al., 2001; O'Doherty et al., 2002). As in D. melanogaster, activity in these areas can also be modulated by satiety state (Tataranni et al., 1999). These neurons are located within the same areas in humans and nonhuman primates that display response properties consistent with the encoding of rank value on a common scale (Padoa-Schioppa and Assad, 2008; Lebreton et al., 2009).
The central brain separates odor identity and odor value coding
Although neuronal populations at the early stages of olfactory processing, ORNs and PNs in D. melanogaster, are likely possessed of information that contributes to the coding of both odor identity and odor attractiveness (Suh et al., 2004; Kreher et al., 2008; Semmelhack and Wang, 2009; Ai et al., 2010; Root et al., 2011), our results indicate that the central brain may separate sensory and hedonic representations of odors. Although we acknowledge that the actual odor-evoked firing patterns of the neurons we investigate may contain additional information not present in the slower calcium signals we observe, the results are suggestive. dNPF activity is higher to food odors, but it does not distinguish their identities, only their relative attractiveness, which is modifiable with hunger. It correlates with what we speculate is food-odor value. Conversely, with their highly odor-specific responses patterns and indistinct treatment of natural and monomolecular, appetitive or aversive odors (Fig. 2) (Turner et al., 2008; Honegger et al., 2011), MB KCs appear to process unique odor identity. This separation provides insight into more general characteristics of information processing in the brain, suggesting the possibility that external stimuli are divided into constituent attributes, of which identity and appetitive value may be just two. The tractability of the fruit fly model should also provide clues as to the evolutionary and/or proximate origins of the value signaling we describe. Given that identity and value representations appear to diverge in the central brain, we are interested to see whether either or both are amenable to change as a function of experience.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory, National Institute of Deafness and Other Communication Disorders Grant RO1 DC005784, and National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grant RO1 NS064331 to Y.Z. We thank P. Shen for providing UAS-npfr1dsRNA flies; S. Waddell for providing dNPF-Gal4 and UAS-dTRPA1 flies; C. Hermann and C. Helfrich-Förster for providing dNPF2-Gal4, UAS-hid/CyO,cry-Gal80/TM6B,D3, and UAS-hid/CyO; dNPF2-Gal4 flies; K. Honegger for generating and sharing UAS-GCaMP3;OK107-Gal4 flies; Y. Zhang for work on a preliminary behavioral assay; R. Campbell for MATLAB programming assistance; and Y. Shuai and D. Tieman for comments on the manuscript.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Yi Zhong, Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory, PO Box 100, Cold Spring Harbor, NY 11724. zhongyi{at}cshl.edu