Abstract
During the early postpartum period or following estrogen/progesterone administration, pups elicit maternal behavior accompanied by a robust dopamine (DA) response in the nucleus accumbens (NAC) of female rats (Afonso et al., 2009). To determine whether DA responds to ostensibly “salient” stimuli in the absence of consummatory behaviors, we examined NAC shell DA responses during restricted (stimuli placed in a perforated box), and unrestricted access to pup and food stimuli. Microdialysis samples were collected from female rats that were either cycling and postpartum (Experiment 1), or after ovariectomy and treated with empty and hormone-filled capsules (Experiment 2). Relative to nonprimed controls, hormonally primed females had suppressed basal DA concentrations and facilitated pup-evoked DA responses, regardless of stimulus access condition. In contrast, food-evoked DA responses were unchanged by hormonal priming and were greater when females consumed food compared with distal (restricted) exposure to food. During pup and food restriction conditions, the lack of any “appetitive” behavioral differences, even in pup experienced postpartum females, was surprising. In Experiment 3, we confirmed that postpartum dams allocated time equivalently to restricted pup and food stimuli, even after pup deprivation. This was in sharp contrast to the effects of deprivation during the unrestricted access phase. Together, our data demonstrated that, in hormonally primed females, distal pup cues could evoke DA responses without prior stimulus experience, ongoing maternal (behavioral) responses, or clear evidence of robust pup saliency. The results suggest that NAC DA response reflects a state of responsiveness related to basal DA suppression in the hormonally primed female rat.
Introduction
The maternally inexperienced mother rat shows the full repertoire of maternal behavior during the end of pregnancy, birthing, and first encounters with her young (Wiesner and Sheard, 1933; Fleming and Rosenblatt, 1974; Numan and Insel, 2003). In the absence of hormonal changes, virgin rats avoid pups on initial exposure (Li and Fleming, 2002; Numan et al., 2006); however, with continuous pup exposure, they show maternal behavior (4–10 d) to foster pups (Rosenblatt, 1967; Fleming and Rosenblatt, 1974; Orpen and Fleming, 1987). The administration of hormones associated with late pregnancy and parturition facilitates a rapid onset (1–6 d) of maternal responsiveness (Moltz et al., 1970; Krehbiel and LeRoy, 1979; Mayer and Rosenblatt, 1979, 1980, 1987; Bridges, 1984, 1990).
Estradiol and progesterone enhance mothering by acting on both hypothalamic and mesolimbic mechanisms (Numan et al., 2006). The nucleus accumbens (NAC), a terminal site of the mesolimbic dopamine (DA) system, regulates stimulus salience, reinforcement learning, and motor output in a variety of motivational contexts (Graybiel et al., 1994; Berridge and Robinson, 1998; Cagniard et al., 2006). On initial pup exposure, the recently parturient mother rat shows a substantial DA elevation, an observation not seen in virgin animals, even after experience (Hansen et al., 1991a,b, 1993; Champagne et al., 2004; Olazábal et al., 2004; Ferris et al., 2005; Afonso et al., 2009, 2011). The two groups also differ in baseline DA, with levels considerably reduced in postpartum rats (Afonso et al., 2009, 2011). A similar baseline DA suppression and pup-stimulated DA elevation are obtained in virgin animals treated with hormones (Afonso et al., 2009, 2011). Although ovarian hormones can bias the DA system to respond to pup stimuli in the absence of pup experience, undetermined is whether pup-evoked DA relates to pup saliency alone or to the actual expression of maternal behavior, as DA responses correlate to licking behavior (Afonso et al., 2009, 2011).
The present study determined whether: (1) distal pup cues (e.g., olfactory, visual, auditory emanating from a Plexiglas box with holes) provoke the same DA release pattern as found with unrestricted pup interactions; (2) ovarian hormones in the absence of pup experience contribute to restricted pup-evoked DA responses; and (3) selectivity exists in restricted pup-evoked DA responses by comparisons with restricted food responses. In a nonhomecage chamber, microdialysis samples were collected from the NAC shell of (1) virgin and postpartum (Experiment 1) or (2) pup-naive ovariectomized (OVX) sham-and hormone-treated (Experiment 2) virgin female rats interacting with pup and food stimuli under restricted and unrestricted access. Previously, postpartum females attended predominately to pups when given simultaneous unrestricted access to pups and food in the dialysis chamber (Afonso et al., 2009). Using a much larger apparatus compared with the dialysis chamber, in Experiment 3, postpartum rats were given simultaneous restricted access to pups and food, followed by unrestricted access to these stimuli. Performed after deprivation conditions, this experiment was designed to alter the animals' specific “motivational” state as reflected by differential time spent in proximity to pups versus food in unrestricted phases.
Materials and Methods
Subjects
Virgin Sprague Dawley female rats (225–300 g), obtained from Charles River, were individually housed in Plexiglas shoebox cages (21 cm high × 26 cm wide × 38 cm deep), given wooden shavings for bedding. Purina Rat Chow and water were available ad libitum. Animals were housed in a colony room maintained on a 12:12 h light/dark cycle with lights on 8 am to 8 pm, at ∼24°C ± 1°C.
Habituation to apparatus and food
Before experimental manipulations, all females were given 4 Honey Nut Cheerios daily for a week in their homecage. Then for 5 d (1 h sessions) during which they were given 4 Cheerios, females were habituated to the dialysis cage (Experiments 1 and 2; clear circular plastic; 24.77 cm diameter, 29.8 cm height, with two stainless steel food dispensers and plastic floor), or to the large testing apparatus (Experiment 3; clear rectangular Plexiglas; 34 cm wide, 108 cm length, 38 cm height, with corn cob-covered floor).
Experiments 1 and 2: general methods
Microdialysis procedure.
Rats were anesthetized using a 2:2 isofluorane/oxygen ratio for all surgical procedures. Females were stereotaxically implanted with a unilateral guide cannula with a stylet (15 mm in length, Bioanalytical Systems) aimed above the NAC shell (flat skull; 1.7 mm caudal to bregma, 0.5–1.0 mm lateral to midline, 6.9–7.3 mm beneath the surface of the skull) (Paxinos and Watson, 1986). During sampling, a probe (320 μm OD, 2 mm length, cutoff 20,000 Da, Bioanalytical Systems) extending 2 mm below the guide was attached to tubing; a needle dispensed the collection samples at a rate of 1 μl/min in 8 min bins into refrigerated vials (4°C) containing a standard antioxidant (2.5 mm ascorbic acid in 0.9% saline, Sigma). The dialysis cage had limited space but allowed freedom of movement in all directions while maintaining the integrity of infusion lines. A counterbalanced arm and tethering system kept test lines of the animal's reach while permitting the animal to rise and stretch ad libitum.
Stimuli presentation during microdialysis.
After probe placement into the guide cannula, baseline samples were collected (15 samples, 120 min). After this basal sampling, pups or food, counterbalanced during the restricted phase, were presented to the female in a secured Plexiglas box (6 × 7.8 × 7.9 cm, with perforated holes); hence, females had only restricted, distal exposure to the stimuli. Stimuli consisted of donor pups (2 males and 2 females that were postnatal day 2–4, warm and recently fed) or food (40 Cheerios in the box). One of the boxed stimuli would be available for 16 min (i.e., two samples collected) followed by a 16 min period with no stimulus before the second boxed stimuli was available for 16 min.
The restricted phase was followed by unrestricted access to pups and then food, not counterbalanced. The unrestricted phase first consisted of 16 min availability of the pups (same pups as in the box) placed on shredded paper towel (nesting material) in one of the food dispensers. This was followed by 16 min of no stimulus, and then 2 Cheerios were placed directly on the chamber floor and after an 8 min period another 2 Cheerios were placed into the chamber. Food-stimuli were eaten within 1 min during these two 8 min sampling bins. Digital video recording collected behavioral data (see Figs. 2B, 4B for the sampling procedure).
HPLC.
After collection of the samples, DA concentrations were determined with HPLC. Briefly, a BAS 460 HPLC system with electrochemical detection (Bioanalytical Systems) was used together with a Uniget C-8 reverse phase column (catalog no. 8949). The mobile phase consisted of buffer [0.1 m monochloroacetic acid, 0.5 mm Na-EDTA, 0.15 g/L Na-octylsylfonate, and 10 nm sodium chloride, pH 3.1], acetonitrile and tetrahydrofuran (Sigma) at a ratio 94:3.5:0.7. The flow rate was 0.5 ml/min, and the working electrode (Iniget 3 mm glassy carbon, BAS P/N MF-1003) was set at 700 mV versus Ag/Ag/Cl. Detection gain was 1.0 nA, filter was 0.2 Hz, and the detection limit was set at 500 nA. Of the 8 μl of collected per sample, 5 μl was directly injected into the HPLC system for analysis. The remaining 3 μl of the sample was used in combination with external standards of DA (Sigma) to quantify and identify the peaks on the chromatographs. The retention times for DA were ∼3.32 min under the set conditions.
Probe placement.
For an animal to be included in the statistical analysis, the guide cannula had to end above the NAC and between 0.3 and 1.0 mm from midline. Figure 1 shows the anatomical placements of the “active” zones of the probe.
Experiment 1: parity methods
Females were either placed with males for mating (postpartum, n = 5) or left alone (virgin and cycling, n = 6) in their homecages for 10 d. Pregnant (d14–16 gestation) and cycling females had guide cannulae placed into the NAC shell. Sampling began 24 h after parturition for postpartum females. Dialysate samples were collected from a cycling female on the same day as a postpartum dam. During sampling donor pups, rather than their own pups, were used as the pup stimuli.
Experiment 2: hormone treatment methods
One week after OVX, virgin pup-naive females were randomly assigned to sham (n = 5) or hormone (n = 7) treatment groups. OVX animals were anesthetized and a single 17 h-estradiol 3-benzoate (E, Sigma) capsule was administered subcutaneously via a 20 mm SILASTIC implant (0.078 in ID × 0.125 in OD; Dow Corning) in the nape region. Two days after the E capsule implants, animals were again anesthetized and an additional 3 progesterone (P, Sigma) capsules (40 mm) were administered subcutaneously in the dorsal region. Before implantation, the SILASTIC capsules were sealed with 5 mm wood piece (CAN Lab) and SILASTIC Medical Adhesive (Dow Corning) after being filled with either E or P. The 100% crystalline E- and P-filled portions of the implants were 10 and 30 mm in length, respectively. The capsules were left to dry for a 24 h period, after which they were washed in ethanol and incubated for 48 h in PBS (pH 7.0). At the same time points, control rats received empty (unfilled) SILASTIC implants that resembled the hormone-filled capsules. Progesterone capsules were removed 10 d after implantation and sampling commenced the following day. Previously, the serum E levels with these E treatments were in the range of 75–80 pg/ml, similar to those found during pregnancy d15–22, whereas the P treatment at 32 ng/ml yield less than pregnancy levels of 80 ng/ml at gestation d4–10, or 120 ng/ml at gestations d12–20 (Stern and McDonald, 1989).
Experiment 3: time allocation methods
Female rats (N = 16) were placed with males for 10 d of mating. Two days after parturition, time allocation assessments were made from these postpartum females given 20 min tests with simultaneous presentation of pups (2 males and 2 females, PND2–5) and food (Cheerio) stimuli, each on opposing sides of the test chamber. For the first 10 min of the test, females were given restricted access to pups and food, each stimulus in their own perforated box; then for the next 10 min, the boxes were opened (unrestricted). For the food side, during the restricted phase the box contained ∼40 Cheerios, and during the unrestricted phase this box was replace with an open box containing 4 Cheerios.
Females were given two 20 min tests daily: (1) under a nondeprived condition and (2) later in the day after one of four possible deprivation conditions. For the first test of the day, postpartum rats were removed from their homecage and litter and placed in the center of the large chamber for the 20 min test period. Rats were then placed back into their homecage before a second test was performed. While in their homecage, postpartum rats were deprived of their pups (P) and food (F) for either 5 or 10 h durations, thus creating four deprivation conditions: 5P5F, 5P10F, 10P5F, and 10P10F. The conditions were counterbalanced across 4 d so that each female was given a second test under each deprivation condition, which was performed after the nondeprived condition (designated as 0P0F), performed daily across the 4 d. Based on videotape coding, the time spent with each of the stimuli for the each of the 10 min access phases was calculated by partitioning the chamber into three equal compartments with the center considered neutral.
Experiments 1 and 2: statistical analyses
Basal DA.
Before the 128 min sampling procedure, which included presentation and removal of various stimuli, basal DA concentrations were obtained from means of three consecutive samples that differed from one another by no >10%. An independent samples t test was performed on DA concentrations (pg/μl) for both experiments to assess group differences (parity, cycling vs postpartum [Experiment 1]; or treatment, sham vs hormone [Experiment 2]).
Stimulus-evoked DA.
For each experiment to assess stimulus-evoked DA, a 3-factor ANOVA, 2-group (parity, cycling vs postpartum [Experiment 1] or treatment, sham vs hormone [Experiment 2]) × 2 stimulus (pups vs food) × 2 access (restricted vs unrestricted), was performed on DA responses (% of basal concentrations, averaged across the two samples). For significant interactions, Tukey HSD post hoc analysis was performed on individual means with appropriate corrections. As basal DA measurements differed with hormonal status in both experiments, to investigate further the influence of basal DA levels on DA responses to stimuli for each experiment, we included the basal concentrations as a covariate in the ANOVAs.
Behavior.
During microdialysis, we analyzed behavioral differences between the groups under the restricted phase, using a 2 group (parity [Experiment 1] or treatment [Experiment 2]) × 2 stimulus (pups vs food) ANOVA on the time spent on sniffing or touching the box containing either pups or food. In terms of pup stimuli, a second ANOVA was performed to determine the behavioral effects of restricting access to pups. A 2 group (parity or treatment) × 2 access (restricted vs unrestricted) ANOVA was performed on time spent with pup stimuli. Food-related behavior during the unrestricted phase was not included in a similar analysis, as females ate the 2 Cheerios within 1 min in each of the two 8 min bins, thereby eliminating the presence of the stimuli.
Experiment 3: statistical analysis
A 2 (access: restricted vs unrestricted) × 2 (stimuli: pups vs food) × 5 (conditions: 0P0F vs 5P5F vs 5P10F vs, 10P5F vs 10P10F) repeated-measures ANOVA was performed on the duration (sec) a postpartum rat spent on a given stimulus side. The nondeprived condition (i.e., 0P0F) was performed daily before a given deprivation conditions, and we averaged the time spent with each stimulus across the 4 d for the 0P0F condition.
Results
Experiment 1: parity effects
Basal DA
t tests revealed that postpartum dams had significantly reduced basal DA concentrations compared with cycling females ([t(9) = 2.46, p = 0.04], Fig. 2A). The time course of the DA responses (% of basal concentrations) after basal collection is shown in Figure 2B.
Stimulus-evoked DA
The ANOVA for stimulus-evoked DA revealed significant main effects of parity [F(1,9) = 12.34, p = 0.007] and access [F(1,9) = 14.52, p = 0.004]. Postpartum dams had increased DA responses compared with cycling females; and compared with restriction, unrestricted access to stimuli produced increased DA responses in all females. There were also significant interactions of parity × stimulus [F(1,9) = 13.36, p = 0.005] and stimulus × access [F(1,9) = 5.47, p = 0.044]. Post hoc analyses showed that postpartum females had increased pup-evoked DA responses compared with the cycling group (Fig. 3A), regardless of access type. During restriction, postpartum females had increased pup-evoked compared with food-evoked DA responses; and during the unrestricted phase, cycling females showed the reverse. Both parities showed increased DA responses to unrestricted compared with restricted access to food (Fig. 3A). When basal concentrations were partialled out, these effects remained significant.
Behavior
The parity × stimulus ANOVA revealed no significant effects, demonstrating that during restricted access females sniff and touch the two boxes equivalently, regardless of parity or stimulus. The parity × access ANOVA revealed a significant interaction (p < 0.001). During unrestricted access to pups, the postpartum females spent more time with pups (i.e., sniffing, retrievals, hovering, licking, mouthing) than during restricted pup-access and compared with the cycling group (Fig. 3B).
Experiment 2: hormone treatment effects
Basal DA
As with the parity effect, the t test performed on DA concentrations revealed that hormone-treated females had significantly reduced basal DA release compared with sham females [t(10) = 5.00, p = 0.001]. See Figure 4A for basal data and Figure 4B for time course of DA responses (percentage over basal).
Stimulus-evoked DA
The ANOVA revealed significant main effects of treatment [F(1,10) = 30.80, p < 0.001] and access [F(1,10) = 26.30, p < 0.001]. Hormone-treated females had increased DA responses compared with sham females; and compared with restriction, unrestricted access to the stimuli produced increased DA responses in all females. There were also significant interactions of treatment × stimulus [F(1,10) = 5.57, p = 0.040] and treatment × access [F(1,10) = 10.14, p = 0.010]. Post hoc analyses showed similar results to Experiment 1 in that hormone-treated females had increased pup-evoked DA responses compared with the sham group, regardless of access type. Like Experiment 1, hormone-treated females had increased pup-evoked compared with food-evoked DA responses during the restricted phase; and during the unrestricted phase, sham-treated females showed the reverse. Both treatment groups showed increased DA responses to unrestricted food compared with restricted access (Fig. 5A). Unlike postpartum females, hormone-treated females had increased DA responses to unrestricted pup access compared with the restricted condition. Different from earlier effects showing parity differences, in this hormone experiment, when basal concentrations were partialled out of the analyses, all significant effects were eliminated.
Behavior
The treatment × stimulus ANOVA revealed no significant results, demonstrating that during restricted access females sniff and touch the two boxes equivalently, regardless of treatment or stimulus. The treatment × access ANOVA revealed a significant interaction (p < 0.001). During unrestricted access to pups, the hormone-treated females spent more time with pups (i.e., sniffing, retrievals, hovering, licking, mouthing) than during restricted pup access and compared with the cycling group (Fig. 5B).
Experiment 3: time allocation
The ANOVA showed significant main effects of stimulus [F(1,48) = 15.22, p = 0.002] and condition [F(4,48) = 6.95, p < 0.001]; and, interactions of access × stimulus, [F(1,48) = 19.95, p = 0.001], access × condition [F(4,48) = 7.14, p = 0.001], stimulus × condition [F(4,48) = 3.61, p = 0.012], and access × stimulus × condition [F(4,48) = 5.90, p = 0.001]. Tukey post hoc analysis, performed on individual means with appropriate corrections, revealed that only after 5 or 10 h of deprivation from the pup and food stimuli (i.e., 5P5F, 10P10F, 5P10F, 10P5F) and only during unrestricted access did postpartum rats show altered time allocation to stimuli. During unrestricted access under all deprivation conditions, postpartum dams spent increased time with pups and decreased time with food compared with (1) the no deprivation condition (0P0F, average across the 4 d), (2) the restricted access to stimuli, and (3) each other. In the maximal (i.e., 10P10F) pup and food deprivation condition, postpartum dams had further decreases in time allocated to the side associated with the opened food box (Fig. 6). The restricted stimuli did not generate unequal time allocation either before or after deprivation conditions or between the two stimuli.
Discussion
The present study demonstrates that pup stimuli can elicit accumbal DA release in hormonally primed rats without actual pup interactions. Unlike nonprimed females, these hormonally primed DA responses were independent of the females' previous pup experience, observed with distal pup stimuli, and expressed in the absence of maternal behavior. Relative to DA patterns in untreated control females, priming by parturition-like hormones suppressed basal DA levels in the postpartum or hormonally treated virgin animals. As well, pup presentation results in large DA increases relative to baseline. In general, lower baseline concentrations were associated with greater pup-evoked DA responses. Hence, the hormonally induced basal DA suppression may facilitate the DA responses to pups. Although previous evidence suggests that DA responses correlate with pup sniffing (Afonso et al., 2009) or pup licking (Afonso et al., 2009, 2011), the present results indicate DA responses can occur independently of the animals' prior maternal experience or the exhibition of many of the maternal consummatory behaviors (e.g., licking, retrieving).
The strong association between pups and pup-predicting cues develops quickly during the early postpartum period under the influence of hormones (Magnusson and Fleming, 1995; Lee et al., 2000; Mattson et al., 2001; Seip and Morrell, 2007, 2009; Seip et al., 2008; Pereira and Morrell, 2010). Primed females come to prefer pups to many other stimuli, effects not seen in the inexperienced nonmaternal virgin animal (Fleming et al., 1980, 1989; Fleming and Luebke, 1981; Fleming and Walsh, 1994; Magnusson and Fleming, 1995; Lee et al., 2000; Mattson et al., 2001; Afonso et al., 2009, 2011). A similar hormonally induced preference can also be seen in the sexual context (Young et al., 1935; Boling et al., 1938; Afonso and Pfaus, 2006) where female rats tend to spend more time with, and bar press more frequently for, sexually active males during proestrus than at other times in the cycle (Bermant, 1961; Bolles et al., 1968). Following appropriate hormonal priming, the willingness of females to traverse electric grids, learn to bar press, and develop a conditioned place preference for appropriate reproductive stimuli (i.e., pups or males) are mediated through dopaminergic ventral tegmental area–NAC mechanisms (Berridge and Robinson, 1998; Everitt and Robbins, 2005).
Maternal responsiveness in female rats is dependent on pregnancy/parturition hormones acting on the medial preoptic area (Numan et al., 1977, 2006; Rosenblatt et al., 1998; Numan and Insel, 2003). It is likely that through either glutamatergic or oxytocinergic projections from the medial preoptic area/bNST to mesolimbic DA neurons in the ventral tegmental area, lead to facilitatory actions of the mesolimbic DA reward circuitry to enhance maternal behavior (Numan and Insel, 2003; Afonso et al., 2009, 2011 ,Numan and Stolzenberg, 2009; Shahrokh et al., 2010). Our data suggest that hormones not only facilitate actions of the DA system to enhance maternal behavior in the experienced mother, in the absence of pup experience these hormones may ensure that maternal behavior develops. A dissociation between postpartum and hormonally treated females emerged when basal DA concentrations were partialled out of the stimulus-induced DA responses. Whereas covarying basal concentrations did not influence the statistical outcomes comparing parities on pup-evoked DA responses, this analysis eliminated all significant differences between the hormonally treated and control OVX groups. The difference between postpartum and hormonally primed females most likely was a result of actual pup experience differences between the two conditions. Postpartum females had had 24–48 h of postparturition pup experience, whereas all other groups were pup-naive at time of sampling. Thus, in addition to hormonal suppression of basal DA levels, pup experience can modify DA signals to pups over and above the hormonal effects on basal DA.
Hormonal effects on the DA reward system most likely involve the ability of gonadal steroid hormones, especially estrogens, to alter the release and metabolism of biogenic amines (DuPont et al., 1981; McEwen and Parsons, 1982; Thompson and Moss, 1994; McEwen and Alves, 1999). For example, chronic estrogen priming increases NAC D2 DA receptors, enhances binding on D2 DA receptors and DA transporter density, all of which could result in lower basal DA concentrations and an increase in stimulus-evoked DA release and excitability of receptors on DA terminals (Di Paolo et al., 1982a,b, 1986, 1988; Morissette et al., 1992; Thompson and Moss, 1994; Becker, 1999; Lammers et al., 1999; Zhou et al., 2002; Lonstein et al., 2003; Becker and Hu, 2008; Chavez et al., 2010). The synergistic effects of these multiple dopaminergic adaptations after chronic estrogen likely influence the pup-evoked DA responses by acting to suppress chronic basal levels. Additionally, progesterone interacts with estradiol and possibly affects parturitional DA mechanisms as it does within other contexts (Russo et al., 2003).
Hormonally primed females will exhibit formerly unfamiliar maternal activities quickly (Mayer and Rosenblatt, 1979, 1980; Bridges, 1984; Stern and McDonald, 1989). For example, most hormone-treated rats will approach and lick pups on their first exposure to a litter and occasionally will retrieve a pup (Bridges, 1984; Afonso et al., 2009). In pup-naive cycling females and untreated OVX females given foster pups, these behaviors are rarely observed. This sensitivity toward pups is observed during preference tests (Afonso et al., 2009, 2011); females of varying hormonal states were given simultaneous access to pups and unlimited food rewards in the dialysis chamber. Postpartum dams and hormone-treated females with homecage pup experience (1 and 2–6 d, respectively) ignored food entirely to interact with pups. Pup-naive hormone-treated females alternated between eating and pup investigations. In contrast, nonhormonally primed controls actively avoided the pups and remained at the neighboring food dispenser, regardless of continuous homecage experience with donor pups. These results are reminiscent of other observations (Fleming et al., 1980, 1989; Fleming and Luebke, 1981; Magnusson and Fleming, 1995; Afonso et al., 2009, 2011) showing that virgin animals initially withdraw from pups, but with continuous pup exposure, approach behavior is observed. Thus, hormones play an important role to influence pup-biased behaviors, and pup experience under hormonal priming can further modify these behavioral responses.
One prominent idea is that DA receptor activation in the NAC constitutes an element of the neural computation of the “effort” that is required to obtain reward (Montague et al., 2004; Salamone et al., 2007; Floresco et al., 2008; Phillips et al., 2008). Numerous studies show that in male rats reward-seeking tasks requiring high degrees of effort are disrupted by reduced DA function in the NAC, whereas similar tasks that require less effort are much less affected (Cousins et al., 1996; Aberman and Salamone, 1999; Salamone et al., 2007). Recently, it has been shown that estradiol exerts modulatory control over cost–benefit decision-making in females (Uban et al., 2012). OVX rats chose to expend more effort for a larger reward, and this effect was reversed by a single high dose of estradiol. These data suggest that estradiol given intermittently appears to shift biases related to decision-making.
During dialysis, both postpartum and hormone-treated females show a greater preference for pups (over food) under unrestricted conditions. When tested in a larger arena, this enhanced preference occurred regardless of the amount of pup or food deprivation in postpartum dams. When both unrestricted pup and food stimuli were given simultaneously, postpartum females consumed the food rewards more quickly (data not shown) after deprivation, resulting in increased time available to spend with the pups. These results suggest that the food-related side could no longer generate food-seeking behavior when permitting active maternal responding, as time allocation is viewed as a critical behavioral marker of reinforcement value (Gallistel and Gibbon, 2000; Cousins et al., 2002). Preventing access to pups appears to diminish the robust preference observed in postpartum females. For example, placing a Plexiglas barrier to prevent postpartum rats from touching or retrieving pups delivered with each bar-press response extinguishes bar pressing in an operant apparatus (Lee et al., 2000). Presently, despite the inability to perform behavioral responses toward pups (e.g., licking, retrieval), a robust DA response was provoked by distal pup cues after hormonal priming, even in the pup-naive females. This result contrasts with findings showing that restricting access to palatable food in opaque boxes is ineffective at stimulating DA responses in the NAC shell (present findings), even after food deprivation (Bassareo and Di Chara, 1999a,b).
Together, our data demonstrate that, in hormonally primed females, distal pup cues evoke DA responses without prior pup experience, ongoing behavioral responses, or clear evidence that pups have a priori or hormonally mediated salience. The results suggest that NAC DA response reflects a state of responsiveness related to basal DA suppression in the hormonally primed female rat. Hormones associated with the early postpartum period increase the capacity of pup properties to influence the DA system and contribute to the development of initial appetitive responses through heightened responsiveness of NAC DA. As a result of reduced basal DA, a DA signal related to pup stimuli may be sharpened, thereby enhancing the initial pup experience such that saliency develops. Thus, hormones may bias the DA system to respond to distal pups cues (olfactory, vocalization, movement), providing a mechanism to promote maternal behavior.
Footnotes
This work was supported by a Natural Science and Engineering Research Council discovery grant and Canadian Research Council grant to A.S.F. We thank the University of Toronto, Mississauga vivarium staff for help in maintaining animals in good health.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to either Dr. Veronica M. Afonso or Dr. Alison S. Fleming, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Mississauga, 3359 Mississauga Road N, Mississauga, ON L5L 1C6, Canada, veronica_afonso{at}hotmail.com or alison.fleming{at}utoronto.ca