Abstract
Neuritic dystrophy is one of the important pathological features associated with amyloid plaques in Alzheimer's disease (AD) and age-dependent neuronal dysfunctions. We reported previously that reticulon-3 (RTN3) immunoreactive dystrophic neurites (RIDNs) are abundantly present in the hippocampus of AD patients, in AD mouse models, and in aged wild-type mice. Transgenic mice overexpressing the human RTN3 transgene spontaneously develop RIDNs in their hippocampi, and the formation of RIDNs correlates with the appearance of RTN3 aggregation. To further elucidate whether the formation of RIDNs is reversible, we generated transgenic mice expressing wild-type human RTN3 under the control of a tetracycline-responsive promoter. Treatment with doxycycline for 2 months effectively turned off expression of the human RTN3 transgene, confirming the inducible nature of the system. However, the formation of hippocampal RIDNs was dependent on whether the transgene was turned off before or after the formation of RTN3 aggregates. When transgenic human RTN3 expression was turned off at young age, formation of RIDNs was essentially eliminated compared with the vehicle-treated transgenic mice. More importantly, a fear conditioning study demonstrated that contextual associative learning and memory in inducible transgenic mice was improved if the density of RIDNs was lowered. Additional mechanistic study suggested that a reduction in BDNF levels in transgenic mice might contribute to the reduced learning and memory in transgenic mice overexpressing RTN3. Hence, we conclude that age-dependent RIDNs cannot be effectively cleared once they have formed, and we postulate that successful prevention of RIDN formation should be initiated before RTN3 aggregation.
Introduction
The reticulon (RTN) protein family consists of four members, RTN1 to RTN4. RTN4, also known as Nogo, has been shown to have a putative role in the inhibition of neurite outgrowth (Strittmatter, 2002; Schwab, 2004). Among the RTN family, the C-terminal domain of RTN proteins, encoded by five exons, is known as the reticulon homology domain (RHD), whereas the N-terminal domain is completely divergent (Oertle et al., 2003; Yan et al., 2006). Topological studies indicate that the RHD, containing two long hydrophobic stretches (31–34 amino acids) separated by a loop region of ∼66 aa (Fournier et al., 2002), directs RTN proteins (RTNs) to adopt ω-shape membrane topology with both ends facing the intracellular space (Voeltz et al., 2006; He et al., 2007). RTNs on the membrane surface are found to interact with Alzheimer's β-secretase (BACE1) and negatively modulate BACE1 enzymatic activity by reducing β-amyloid (Aβ) levels (He et al., 2004; Murayama et al., 2006; Shi et al., 2009b).
While exploring the in vivo role of RTN in Alzheimer's disease (AD) pathogenesis, we discovered that overexpression of neuronal RTN3 in transgenic (Tg) mice resulted in the spontaneous development of RTN3 immunoreactive dystrophic neurites (RIDNs) in their hippocampi (Hu et al., 2007). Morphologically, RIDNs exhibit abnormally swollen neurites and resemble the previously described dystrophic neurites marked by ubiquitin, GAP-43, or neurofilament in AD brains (Rifenburg and Perry, 1995). RIDNs seen in young RTN3 Tg mice are similar to those found in aged normal mouse hippocampi, indicating that the occurrence of RIDNs in RTN3 Tg mice represents an acceleration of a naturally occurring event (Shi et al., 2009a). More importantly, we demonstrated that RIDNs are the most abundant type of dystrophic neurites in surrounding amyloid plaques in brains of AD patients (Hu et al., 2007). The underlying molecular cause of RIDNs is attributed to the aggregation of RTN3 (Hu et al., 2007). Because the presence of RIDNs in hippocampi of mouse models correlates with the reduction of dendritic spine density, long-term potentiation (LTP) and learning behavior in the Barnes maze test (Hu et al., 2007), it is postulated that the presence of RIDNs in AD brains contributes to the cognitive dysfunction seen in patients (Prior et al., 2010).
To determine whether the formation of RIDNs is reversible and whether a reduction in RTN3 aggregation could potentially ameliorate cognitive dysfunction in elderly and AD patients, we generated Tg mice expressing wild-type human RTN3 under the control of tetracycline (Tet) responsible element (TetOff promoter). We found that increased expression of the RTN3 transgene in the mouse forebrain facilitated the formation of RIDNs significantly earlier than in wild-type littermates. By manipulating the expression of the transgene either before or after the formation of RIDNs, we were able to answer the question of whether reducing RTN3 levels would have a significant effect on the formation of RIDNs. We conclude that reducing RTN3 levels before RTN3 aggregation could significantly inhibit the formation of RIDNs and that RIDN density is correlated with impairments in learning and memory. We therefore postulate that targeted early inhibition of RTN3 aggregation has therapeutic potential for AD patients.
Materials and Methods
Mouse strains.
Tet response element (TRE)–CMV–hRTN3 Tg mice were generated in the laboratory. Briefly, the transgene of Tet-responsive PhCMV*-1 promoter–hRTN3 was constructed by insertion of an hRTN3 cDNA into the pTRE2hyg plasmid vector (Clontech Laboratories) between BamHI and NotI in the multiple cloning site, which allows the expression of hRTN3 under the control of the Tet-responsive PhCMV*-1 promoter. A new PvuI site was created downstream of the β-globin poly(A) DNA sequence in the plasmid, which allowed us to isolate the DNA fragment of Tet-responsive PhCMV*–hRTN3–β-globin poly(A) from the plasmid by restriction enzyme digestion at XhoI and PvuI sites. The purified transgene DNA fragment was microinjected into the male pronuclei of fertilized mouse oocytes (B6C3F1). The oocytes were then implanted into the oviduct of pseudo-pregnant mothers. More than three founders were generated from the injection. The offspring were maintained by breeding to C57BL/6J mice. Their genotype was determined using Southern blotting and PCR. The PCR primers were 5′-CAGTCCCATTCCATCTCCTC-3′ and 5′-CAATCGGGACACTGAAAATG-3′. The probe for Southern blotting was synthesized using the PCR primers and the template of hRTN3 cDNA. CaMKII–trans-activator protein (tTA) Tg mice were purchased from The Jackson Laboratory and were bred with TRE–CMV–hRTN3 Tg mice (line 8) to produce compound mice, named RTN3TetOff. The compound RTN3TetOff mice expressed hRTN3 when the trans-activator tTA from CaMKII–tTA transgene bound to TRE and activated transcription from CMV–hRTN3. All mice in the study were maintained and used according to the protocols approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the Cleveland Clinic Foundation.
Reagents.
Antibodies against calnexin (C4731) and β-actin (A5441) were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich. Antibodies R454 and R458 were generated by the laboratory (He et al., 2004). Antibody R454 exclusively recognizes the N terminus of human RTN3 protein, whereas antibody R458 recognizes the C termini of both human and mouse RTN3. Anti-brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) antibody (sc546) was purchase from Santa Cruz Biotechnology. Alexa Fluor 488- and 568-labeled goat anti-rabbit secondary antibodies were purchased from Invitrogen. Doxycycline (Dox) was purchased from Sigma-Aldrich. The stock solution of Dox was made in 66.66 mg/ml and frozen in aliquot at −20°C. To turn off TRE–CMV–hRTN3 expression, the test group of mice was fed with Dox solution (0.5 mg/ml) freshly prepared with autoclaved tap water containing 2% sucrose. The feeding Dox solution was changed every 3–4 d and covered with light-preventive foil. Four to 12% Bis–Tris NuPage gels for Western blotting and reagents for PCR analysis were purchased from Invitrogen. Protease inhibitor cocktail tablets were purchased from Roche Biosciences.
Western blot analysis of mouse hippocampi.
Mouse hippocampi were dissected from the mouse brain and frozen at −80°C. For Western blotting analysis, frozen dissected hippocampi were homogenized in a solution containing 50 mm Tris-HCl, pH 7.5, 150 mm NaCl, 0.5% NP-40, RochR complete protease inhibitors (1×), and phosphatase inhibitor Na3VO4 (0.1 mm) on ice. Total proteins were extracted from the homogenates by agitating on a rotator for 30 min at 4°C. After centrifugation at 15,000 × g for 120 min, supernatants were collected. Total protein concentrations of the supernatants were determined with BCA protein assay reagent (Pierce). Equal amounts of the supernatants (40 μg of total protein) were resolved on 4–12% Bis–Tris NuPage gel, followed by standard Western blotting with antibodies specified in the text. Chemiluminescent signals for proteins on Western blots were scanned, and their integrated density values were calculated with a chemiluminescent imaging system (Alpha Innotech).
Immunofluorescence confocal microscopy and quantification.
Immunofluorescence confocal microscopy experiments were performed as described previously (He et al., 2004). The right halves of mouse brains were sectioned in the sagittal plane at a 14 μm thickness using a cryostat after 4% paraformaldehyde fixation and O.C.T. compound embedding. Brain sections were stored at −80°C. After three washes in PBS (1×), brain sections were incubated in 0.3% Triton X-100 in PBS (1×) for 30 min. After additional washes in PBS (1×), the sections were blocked in 5% goat serum in PBS (1×) for 30 min. Then the sections were incubated in primary antibody against RTN3 overnight. The sections were then washed in PBS (1×) three times and incubated in secondary antibody goat anti-mouse IgG conjugated with either Alexa Fluor 488 or Alexa Fluor 568. The fluorescence images were examined and captured with a Leica SP5 confocal microscope. We manually determined the counts of RIDNs in evenly spaced six hippocampi per mouse brain. Group means were calculated based on the counts of individual animals.
Fear conditioning assay.
Contextual and cued memory of mice were examined in a fear conditioning chamber (Med Associates) using Ponder's protocol (Ponder et al., 2008). Each mouse was trained in the same conditioning chamber. On the day of conditioning (day 1), each mouse was individually placed in the chamber and allowed to explore the chamber for 3 min. After the exploration, the mice were exposed to a conditioned stimulus (a tone of 3 kHz and 85 dB for 30 s), followed by an unconditioned stimulus (an electric footshock of 0.7 mA for 2 s) that was delivered during the last 2 s of the conditioned stimulus. The conditioning was repeated after a 30 s interval. On the second day (day 2), the mice were returned to the same chamber to measure freezing in response to the context. To determine contextual memory, freezing time was recorded by a video camera for 150 s, starting from 30 s after entry into the chamber. Neither tone nor footshock was delivered. On the third day, the same mice were again placed into the same chamber with significantly altered context, including different walls, floor, and background odor. Two conditioned stimuli (two 30 s tones) were presented to the mice in the same way as those presented on day 1, in the absence of the footshock. Freezing during the presentation of tones was recorded by video camera to assess tone-mediated cued memory. Fear conditioning data were acquired using software Freezeframe and analyzed using software Freezeview (Coulbourn Instruments and Actimetrics).
Determination of BDNF levels by ELISA.
BDNF levels were measured with BDNF Emax Immunoassay System (Promega), a kit showing no significant cross-reactivity with other neurotrophic factors (NGF and neurotrophins 3 and 4/5) as described previously (Szapacs et al., 2004). Briefly, proteins were extracted from the hippocampal and cortical regions from wild-type and RTN3 Tg mice by sonication in the following lysis buffer: 100 mm PIPES, pH 7.0, 500 mm NaCl, 0.5% NP-40, 0.1% NaN3, 2% BSA, 2 mm EDTA.Na2–2H2O, and 3× protease inhibitor cocktail. The crude extracts were centrifuged for 30 min at 13,000 rpm at 4°C to remove insoluble materials, and total protein concentration of supernatants was measured by Bio-Rad protein assay. Equal amounts of protein extracts from each sample were used for measuring of BDNF by ELISA according to the procedures described in the protocol.
Statistical analyses.
For comparison between two groups at a single time point, an unpaired t test was performed. When comparing multiple groups, one-way ANOVA followed by a Tukey's post hoc test was used. All statistical analysis was conducted using GraphPad Prism software. Data are expressed as group mean ± SEM, and significance of difference is indicated as *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, and ***p < 0.001.
Results
Characterization of Tg mice overexpressing RTN3
To achieve inducible expression of RTN3 in vivo, we generated Tg mice expressing human RTN3 cDNA under the control of inducible TRE (Fig. 1A). The assembled construct was then linearized by enzymatic digestions, and the gel-purified plasmid DNA was injected into mouse pronuclei (B6C3F1 strain). After screening 107 pups, we recovered 13 positive founder mice, which were verified by both PCR genotyping (Fig. 1B) and Southern blotting (data not shown). Although 13 founder lines of Tg mice were generated, we found that most of them had similar levels of the transgene integrated into the mouse genome (data not shown). Therefore, four lines of mice were chosen to further breed with Tg mice expressing Tet-controlled tTA driven by CaMKII promoter (Tg–CaMK–tTA). By examining the brain lysates from the offspring carrying both RTN3 and tTA, we further validated that RTN3/CaMKII–tTA Tg mice (or called RTN3TetOff mice) indeed produced human RTN3 protein, detected by antibody R454, which specifically reacts with the N-terminus-specific epitope of human, but not murine, RTN3 (Fig. 1C). Among four lines of RTN3TetOff Tg mice examined for RTN3 expression, we found that RTN3 levels among these lines did not vary significantly, and so the highest expressing line was therefore chosen for subsequent experiments.
Generation and characterization of TetOff-inducible RTN3 Tg mice. A, DNA construct of TRE–CMV–hRTN3 (tcRTN3) transgene contains sequences of Tet-responsive PhCMV*-1 promoter (TRE–CMV), human RTN3 cDNA, and β-globin poly(A) region. B, Genotyping PCR product of 565 bp is specific to the human RTN3 cDNA. A representative image shows positive PCR products present in four founder lines (lanes 3, 6–8). C, RTN3TetOff mouse contained both hRTN3 and tTA transgenes. A representative Western blot shows production of human RTN3 protein detected with R454, which is specific to human RTN3 epitope. RTN3 from human HEK293 cells serves as a positive control. Treatment of RTN3TetOff mouse with Dox turns off expression of the human RTN3 transgene but did not affect the endogenous human RTN3 expression in the HEK293 cells. The tcRTN3 mice did not express human RTN3 protein because they lacked the TetOff trans-activator.
Like the Tg mice constitutively expressing a myc-tagged RTN3 reported in our previous study (Hu et al., 2007), RTN3TetOff Tg mice did not show obviously discernible growth defects (data not shown). RIDNs appeared to be detectable at the age of 5 months (data not shown), and the density of RIDNs at the age of 8 months was moderate. This is consistent with a previous report (Hu et al., 2007) that the density of RIDNs is highly age dependent and correlated with the expression level of RTN3 transgene.
Inducible expression of RTN3 transgene to monitor RTN3 aggregation
To determine whether the formation of RIDNs is reversible, we first aged RTN3TetOff Tg mice to 10 months and then turned off the expression of the transgene by adding 0.5 mg/ml Dox, a more stable derivative of Tet, to the drinking water. Dox treatment for 2 months completely shut down the expression of human RTN3 because the human transgene was no longer detectable by antibody R454 (Fig. 2A). Because C-terminal RTN3 antibody R458 detects both human and mouse RTN3, we noticed that the total RTN3 level in the treated RTN3TetOff Tg mice was essentially comparable with that in wild-type controls (Fig. 2B).
RTN3 transgene was effectively turned off by Dox treatment. A, Equal amounts of hippocampal proteins from indicated genotypes of 12-month-old (12m) mice were analyzed by Western blotting. RTN3TetOff mice were treated with either Dox or 2% sucrose (vehicle) for 2 months (2m). Antibody R454 exclusively recognizes the N terminus of Tg RTN3 with human origin. Antibody R458 recognizes both C termini of Tg human RTN3 and endogenous mouse RTN3. In the representative Western blot image, wild-type (WT) and Tg RTN3 served as controls (lanes 1, 2). The proteins from RTN3TetOff mice treated with vehicle are shown in lanes 3 and 4, whereas the proteins from RTN3TetOff mice treated with Dox are shown in lanes 5 and 6. B, The relative protein levels of RTN3 over the loading control calnexin in each genotype group were shown in bar graphs. Tg RTN3 mice expressed Tg human RTN3 constitutively at a level fourfold higher than that of wild-type mice, whereas the total hippocampal RTN3 protein of human and mouse in RTN3TetOff mice was increased by twofold compared with wild-type mice (n = 4, 189.3 ± 14.3 vs 94.2 ± 15.4, *** p < 0.001, Tukey's multiple comparison test). Two-month Dox treatment completely turned off expression of RTN3 transgene in RTN3TetOff mice as shown in Western blotting with R454.
Contrary to our expectations, the density of RIDNs in Dox-treated RTN3TetOff Tg mice was not visibly different from that of vehicle-treated RTN3TetOff Tg mice (Fig. 3A,B). Because the human RTN3-specific antibody R454 could also detect RIDNs in Dox-treated RTN3TetOff Tg mouse hippocampi, it indicated that the human RTN3 transgene in the RTN3 aggregates was not removed during the Dox treatment period (Fig. 3C,D). Additional quantification confirmed no significant alterations in RIDN density in the hippocampal region because total numbers of RIDNs between two genotypes of mice were not significant different (1095 ± 65.9 RIDNs Dox-treated vs 1157 ± 78.1 in vehicle-treated sections, n = 4, p > 0.1, Student's t test). Thus, RTN3 aggregates in the dystrophic neurites appear to be not readily degradable or cleared during a 2 month period in which there was a clear reduction of the RTN3 transgene. It is possible that the proteasome or protein degradation system in the neuritic region is less efficient.
RIDNs remained in the hippocampus of RTN3TetOff mice after Dox treatment. Sagittal mouse brain sections (14 μm) were reacted with antibody R458 and anti-rabbit Alexa Fluor 488 secondary antibody (A, B) or with antibody R454 and anti-rabbit Alexa Fluor 568 secondary antibody (C, D). RIDNs in hippocampal CA1 region were visible by using confocal immunofluorescence microscopy. Scale bar, 40 μm. As shown in the confocal images of hippocampal CA1 region, the number of RIDNs was not significantly reduced in Dox-treated RTN3TetOff mice (B, D) compared with the vehicle-treated littermates (A, C). 2m, Two months; 12m, 12 months.
Formation of RIDNs is prevented by reducing RTN3 levels at early time points
Because RTN3 aggregates are not readily removed after their formation, we asked whether the formation of RIDNs is preventable if RTN3 levels are reduced just before the formation of RTN3 aggregates. We found that RTN3TetOff Tg mice began to produce RIDNs at ∼5 months of age. We therefore treated RTN3TetOff Tg mice beginning at the age of 5 months with Dox and continued treatment for 4 months. A 4 month treatment of RTN3TetOff Tg mice with Dox completely turned off the expression of the RTN3 transgene (Fig. 4A,B). When RIDNs were imaged by confocal microscopy, we found that antibody R454 failed to detect RIDNs in Dox-treated RTN3TetOff Tg mice (Fig. 5, compare C, A), whereas vehicle-treated RTN3TetOff Tg mice at the age of 9 months produced a low density of RIDNs, detectable by both R454 and R458 (Fig. 5B,E). Quantification of RIDN density further confirmed that RIDNs were mainly present in vehicle-treated RTN3TetOff Tg mice (Fig. 5G). Together, these data demonstrate that reducing RTN3 transgene expression before the formation of RTN3 aggregates can prevent the formation of RIDNs.
Turning off expression of RTN3 transgene in young Tg RTN3TetOff mice. A, RTN3TetOff Tg mice treated with either vehicle or Dox for 4 months (4m), and prepared hippocampal protein extracts were analyzed by Western blotting analysis. In vehicle-treated RTN3TetOff mice, RTN3 transgene was highly expressed as revealed by antibody R454 (lanes 3–7), whereas no RTN3 transgene was detected using R454 after 4 months Dox treatment (lanes 8–12). 9m, Nine months. B, The relative expression of RTN3 was normalized by β-actin expression. After normalization with β-actin, the total level of hippocampal RTN3 protein was reduced by 46% in the Dox-treated mice compared with that of vehicle-treated mice (n = 5, p < 0.001, Tukey's multiple comparison test), whereas its level was not significantly different from that of the tcRTN3 Tg mice (n = 5, p > 0.05, Tukey's multiple comparison test). One-way ANOVA statistical analysis indicated that there was significant variance among the three groups of mice (p < 0.001, n = 5).
Elimination of RIDNs in RTN3TetOff mice with early Dox treatment. Sagittal mouse brain sections were labeled by antibody R454 (A–C) or antibody R458 (D–F). Anti-rabbit Alexa Fluor 488 secondary is the secondary antibody. Antibody R458 recognizes both Tg RTN3 and endogenous mouse RTN3. To detect the presence of only RTN3 transgene in RIDNs, antibody R454 was used for detection. When antibody R458 was applied, hippocampal RIDNs were easily detected in the vehicle-treated RTN3TetOff mice, but only sparse hippocampal RIDNs were detected in both the tcRTN3 mice (D) and the Dox-treated RTN3TetOff mice (F). Scale bar, 40 μm. G, The hippocampal RIDN counts in the vehicle-treated RTN3TetOff mice were significantly higher than in the tcRTN3 mice (118 ± 21.5 vs 2.6 ± 0.7 in 6 sections per mouse, n = 5, p < 0.01, Tukey's multiple comparison test). After 4 months of Dox treatment, hippocampal RIDN counts were significantly reduced (4.6 ± 1.5 in sections per mouse, n = 5, p < 0.01, Tukey's multiple comparison test).
Reduced BACE1 processing of APP by overexpressing RTN3
Our previous studies indicate that RTN3 is a negative modulator of BACE1, and its inhibitory activity is adversely affected when it aggregates in the hippocampus of Tg RTN3 mice (Shi et al., 2009b). Because the RTN3TetOff mice formed RIDNs in the CA1 region at a modest level, we asked whether elevated expression of RTN3 would affect BACE1 activity. We performed standard Western blotting analysis on total protein extracts from hippocampal homogenates of both the 4-month vehicle-treated and 4-month Dox-treated RTN3TetOff mice to determine the effect of RTN3 on BACE1 activity. We found that the endogenous full-length APP level was higher in vehicle-treated mice than that in Dox-treated mice (Fig. 6A). Quantification showed that APP processing in vehicle-treated mice (RTN3 levels were higher as shown in Fig. 6A) was ∼13% lower than that in the Dox-treated mice (38.71 ± 0.7918 vs 33.82 ± 1.875, respectively, n = 5, p < 0.05; Fig. 6B).
Reduction of APP processing by overexpressed RTN3. A, Hippocampal protein lysates were analyzed by Western blotting with antibody specific to APP C terminus (antibody A8717). APP C99 is the BACE1-cleaved product from APP, whereas APP C83 is the product from α-secretase cleavage of APP. β-Actin antibody was used to verify the loading. B, Relative BACE1 activity was assessed by the ratio of C99 over C83 or C99 over APP. Statistic significance was calculated by one-way ANOVA statistical analysis, which showed significance (p < 0.05 or p < 0.01, n = 5). 4m, Four months; 9m, 9 months.
To determine whether this change resulted from APP processing by α-secretase or β-secretase, the ratio of APP CTF99/APP CTF83 was calculated based on the Western blotting data. We found the ratio of CTF99/CTF83 was lowered by 22% in the vehicle-treated mice compared with Dox-treated mice (0.2437 ± 0.01990 vs 0.3119 ± 0.02608, respectively, n = 5, p < 0.05; Fig. 6C), suggesting a small reduction in the β-secretase activity in the vehicle-treated mice. We also found that the ratio of APP CTF99 over full-length APP was significantly lowered in vehicle-treated mice compared with Dox-treated mice (0.1357 ± 0.01048 vs 0.2460 ± 0.03152, respectively, n = 5, p < 0.01; Fig. 6D). Thus, our data confirm that elevated levels of RTN3 monomer have inhibitory effects on BACE1 activity, consistent with recent findings in animal models (Shi et al., 2009b; Araki et al., 2013).
The density of RIDNs in the hippocampus correlates with impaired learning and memory
The hippocampus is known to play an important role in the acquisition and consolidation of contextual fear memory (Kim and Fanselow, 1992; Phillips and LeDoux, 1992; Lee and Kesner, 2004; Daumas et al., 2005). Because RIDNs are mostly enriched in the mouse hippocampal CA1 region and less in the CA3 region, we asked whether the occurrence of RIDNs affects the formation of contextual memory. To address this, a fear conditioning test was conducted in RTN3TetOff Tg mice with or without Dox treatment for 4 months, beginning at the age of 5 months. As demonstrated previously, Dox treatment prevents the formation of RIDNs in RTN3TetOff Tg mice (Fig. 5). Fear conditioning was used to measure hippocampal-dependent associative learning. In this test, the associative learning of a cue (sound) or a context (environment) with a brief aversive stimulus (electric shock) was measured by analyzing the freezing response of the mice. Before the training, both groups of mice had normal levels of freezing, similar to those of wild-type mice (Fig. 7A, Pre-con). On day 1, mice were placed in the fear conditioning chamber for contextual learning by exposure to a sound, followed by a footshock. During the training period, we observed no difference in the percentage of freezing between these two groups (t test, p > 0.1, n = 10). On day 2, mice were placed back in the chamber without exposure to sound or shock for the same length of time as the previous day. Context-dependent freezing was measured according to published procedures (Ponder et al., 2008) to evaluate the learned aversion of an animal for the environment associated with the mild aversive stimulus. We found that the Dox-treated RTN3TetOff Tg mice showed significantly increased freezing time in response to the context, which was much greater than that in the vehicle-treated RTN3TetOff Tg mice (Fig. 7A, Context, t test, p < 0.001). This demonstrated that preventing the formation of hippocampal RIDNs significantly improved contextual learning and memory capability in these mice. More interestingly, RIDN density was positively correlated with the severity of hippocampus-dependent contextual memory deficits (Fig. 7B). On day 3, mice were placed in a context-altered chamber, in which they were exposed to the same sound at the same time points as day 1. We measured their freezing in response to the tone, a test that is less dependent on hippocampal function but more related to the amygdala (Phillips and LeDoux, 1992). Mice in both groups (with or without Dox treatment) showed dramatically increased freezing to the tone in a context-altered chamber (Fig. 7A, Tone). Although Dox-treated animals showed an even higher percentage of freezing than vehicle-treated RTN3TetOff Tg mice, the difference was not statistically significant (t test, p > 0.1). This suggests that the tone-mediated cued learning and memory was less associated with the density of hippocampal RIDNs. Together, these data show that the higher the density of RIDNs, the greater the impairment of contextual memory in this mouse model.
Reducing formation of RIDNs improves contextual associative learning and memory in RTN3TetOff mice. A, Fear conditioning assay of learning and memory included a testing procedure of 3 d in a fear conditioning chamber. There was no difference in the percentage of freezing time before conditioning on day 1 (Pre-con). On day 2, contextual associative learning and memory of the mice were analyzed. Percentage of total freeze time of mice on day 2 was recorded as an ability of contextual associative learning and memory. The percentage of total freeze time was significantly improved from 24.5 ± 5.5 to 63.2 ± 5.0% after treatment of RTN3TetOff mice with Dox (n = 12, p < 0.01). B, The relationship between the RIDN counts and the contextual associated learning and memory was examined by statistical correlation analysis. Percentage of total freeze was plotted against the counts of RIDNs in hippocampal CA1 region. Their correlation coefficient r = −0.816, R2 = 0.666 (n = 12, p < 0.01), indicating a negative correlation between the hippocampal RIDN formation and the contextual associated learning and memory in the mice.
Reduced BDNF levels attributable to the formation of RIDNs
We demonstrated previously that hippocampal RIDNs disrupt dendritic arborization and reduce the density of dendritic spines in Tg RTN3 mice (Hu et al., 2007; Shi et al., 2009a). Such a structural alteration correlates with impaired learning and memory. To understand the molecular mechanisms underlying the impairment of learning and memory, we postulated that RIDN-induced structural alterations in the hippocampal CA1 region may disrupt the Schaffer collateral pathway via regulatory neurotrophic factors. Among many neurotrophic factors, BDNF is the most plausible candidate because it is transported in and released from both axons and dendrites, as well as primarily stored in presynaptic vesicles (Adachi et al., 2005; Dieni et al., 2012), which are likely affected by the local presence of RIDNs. In addition, BDNF plays important roles not only in neuritic arborization (Cohen-Cory et al., 2010) but also in memory-related LTP (Egan et al., 2003; Zakharenko et al., 2003; Aicardi et al., 2004; Pang et al., 2004).
We determined the level of BDNF by a sensitive ELISA method (Szapacs et al., 2004; Ma et al., 2012). The total level of BDNF was reduced by ∼22.8% in the hippocampus of RTN3TetOff mice compared with their single Tg tcRTN3 littermates (n = 6, p < 0.01; Fig. 8A). To validate this reduction further, we also measured BDNF levels in RTN3 Tg mouse hippocampus in which more abundant RIDNs are formed. Our results showed a greater reduction of BDNF (by ∼33%, n = 6, p < 0.05; Fig. 8B). Consistently, confocal staining also showed a clear reduction of BDNF staining in CA1 pyramidal neurons and processes (Fig. 8C). Interestingly, the level of BDNF in the cortex of RTN3 Tg mice was only slightly affected, which is consistent with the rarity of cortical RIDNs and the absence of abnormal dendritic arbors in the 9-month-old RTN3 Tg mice. Hence, our data imply that there is a correlation between RIDN density and reduction of BDNF.
Reduction of BDNF in the hippocampus of the RTN3 Tg mice. ELISA was performed to determine the levels of BDNF in protein extracts that were prepared from either RTN3TetOff mice (A) or Tg RTN3 mice (B) and their littermate controls. The levels of hippocampal BDNF were significantly reduced in hippocampi of both mouse models (n = 6 in RTN3TetOff mice group and n = 6 in Tg RTN3 mice group; ***p < 0.01). However, the levels of cortical BDNF between Tg RTN3 mice and their control littermates showed no statistical difference (n = 6; p > 0.05). All results are presented as a mean ± SE. C, Immuno-confocal microscopy revealed that the positive BDNF immunity in the CA1 pyramidal neurons of the control mice (WT), whereas the BDNF immunity was mostly reduced in the Tg RTN3 mice. The mouse anti-BDNF antibody and the goat anti-mouse Alexa Fluor 488 antibody were used to label sagittal mouse brain sections (14 μm) in the immunofluorescent study.
Discussion
For the past decade, pharmaceutical interventions for AD have been centered primarily on decreasing amyloid deposition in brain (Weiner and Selkoe, 2002; Gandy et al., 2003; Chow et al., 2010). Active vaccination of AD patients with Aβ peptides, an approach that has previously received great attention, showed a marked reduction of amyloid deposition (Tarawneh and Holtzman, 2009). However, cognitive function in these treated patients showed little improvement. Because aggregation of Aβ peptides appears to facilitate the formation of dystrophic neurites, which are found to surround amyloid deposits together with reactive astrocytes and activated microglia, it is plausible that removing amyloid deposition may not be sufficient to reduce other pathological features, such as dystrophic neurites in AD brains. This study provides evidence that RIDNs, which contain RTN3 aggregates, are not readily removable and that the continuing presence of RIDNs may hinder the complete reversal of cognitive dysfunction in AD patients, even in cases of dramatic reductions in amyloid plaques. It is likely that preventing the formation of dystrophic neurites or RIDNs will be a necessary therapeutic strategy for the successful treatment of AD patients.
RIDNs appear to be the most abundant dystrophic neurites found surrounding amyloid plaques in brains of AD patients (Hu et al., 2007). In addition, the age-dependent occurrence of RIDNs in mice has also been documented (Shi et al., 2009a). Because the presence of RIDNs can cause a reduction of dendritic spine density and cognitive dysfunctions, it is possible that aging-dependent memory decline, encountered in normal elderly people (Winocur and Moscovitch, 1990; Gallagher and Rapp, 1997), may also be attributable to the high density of RIDNs. The occurrence of RIDNs correlates with the abnormal aggregation of RTN3 (Hu et al., 2007). In this study, we examined whether the formation of RIDNs or abnormal RTN3 aggregation is reversible by using an inducible expression system in a mouse model. Our results indicate that the formation of RIDNs is not readily reversible.
Fear conditioning assays have been widely used to evaluate hippocampal-dependent contextual learning and memory in AD mouse models (Corcoran et al., 2002; Oddo et al., 2003). Previous studies have revealed that the hippocampus is involved in contextual, but not cued, fear conditioning, whereas the amygdala is involved in both contextual and cued fear conditioning (Phillips and LeDoux, 1992; Johansen et al., 2011). In the present study, prevention of RIDN formation in hippocampus alleviated impairment in contextual learning and memory in the compound Tg mice. RIDN formation had no effect on cued learning and memory. There was no difference in pretraining activity between the two groups of mice, indicating that, in the absence of the aversive stimulus, Dox itself did not alter the mobility of the mice. The pretraining mobile activity and the level of the cued memory of RTN3TetOff mice of both groups are similar to those of wild-type C57BL/6J mice (Stiedl et al., 2000). We showed here that the density of RIDNs present in the hippocampus inversely correlated with cognitive function in our animals. Our results also suggest that inhibition of RTN3 aggregation should be targeted early, before the formation of RIDNs.
It should be noted that reduction of BDNF has been reported in brains of AD patients (Phillips et al., 1991; Laske et al., 2006). Aggregation of Aβ has been linked previously to the reduction of BDNF (Tong et al., 2004). Our study suggests that RTN3 aggregation in RIDNs can also contribute to the reduction (Fig. 8). In the CNS, BDNF is synthesized and secreted by neurons from soma in a constitutive manner and from nerve terminals, predominantly dendritic postsynaptic terminals, in a regulatory manner (Lessmann et al., 2003; Brigadski et al., 2005; Santos et al., 2010). BDNF can stimulate TrkB receptor signaling pathways on both postsynaptic and presynaptic terminals via autocrine and paracrine pathways (McAllister, 2002). Although how RTN3 aggregation causes a reduction of BDNF remains to be determined, we have already known that the presence of RIDNs impairs axonal terminus as demonstrated in our previous EM experiments (Hu et al., 2007). In addition, we observed a reduction of BDNF mRNA level in Tg RTN3 mouse hippocampi (data not shown). Our future biochemical assays are aimed to explore the underlying consequence.
As demonstrated in a current model, the physiological action of BDNF on both dendrites and axons is to promote their branching and synaptic connection (McAllister et al., 1999; Cohen-Cory et al., 2010). Consistent with the important role of BDNF in the control of dendritic arborization and synaptic connection, the impaired dendritic arborization and reduction of spine density in the CA1 region of Tg RTN3 mice have been reported previously (Hu et al., 2007; Shi et al., 2009a), and correlated changes were also seen in RTN3TetOff mice (data not shown). These structural alterations significantly reduced formation of synapses between the dendrites of pyramidal neurons in CA1 and afferent axons, such as Schaffer collaterals and the entorhinal cortex layer III fibers. Hence, a reduction in BDNF signaling and synaptic connections in association with the presence of RIDNs could account for a significant reduction of LTP (Hu et al., 2007). Together, severe neuritic dystrophy causes the reduction of BDNF levels, which in turn impairs cognitive function.
Hippocampal BACE1 activity in the RTN3TetOff mice was clearly lowered when the RTN3 level was modestly reduced (Fig. 6). If the RTN3 level was returned to the level comparable with the wild-type controls as a result of 4 month Dox treatment, BACE1 activity appears to revert to the control level. In this TetOff mouse model, the density of RIDNs was significantly lower than that of Tg RTN3 mice, in which higher expression of RTN3 also facilitates the formation of a large amount of RTN3 aggregates (Hu et al., 2007). Our result appears in line with a recent observation that a small increase of RTN3 in mouse neurons (by ∼40%) decreases amyloid deposition by ∼50% (Araki et al., 2013). Hence, future studies should target to a small elevation of RTN3 but just enough so as to prevent RTN3 aggregation. Because age-dependent RIDNs cannot be effectively cleared once they have formed, we postulate that successful prevention of RIDN formation should be initiated before RTN3 aggregation.
Footnotes
This work is partially supported by National Institutes of Health Grant AG025493 (R.Y.) and Alzheimer's Association Award NPSPAD-10-174543 (R.Y.). A postdoctoral fellowship from American Health Association Foundation (A2008–627) is awarded to M.P.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Riqiang Yan, Department of Neurosciences, Lerner Research Institute, The Cleveland Clinic Foundation, 9500 Euclid Avenue, Cleveland, OH 44195. yanr{at}ccf.org