Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Reputational Priors Magnify Striatal Responses to Violations of Trust

Elsa Fouragnan, Gabriele Chierchia, Susanne Greiner, Remi Neveu, Paolo Avesani and Giorgio Coricelli
Journal of Neuroscience 20 February 2013, 33 (8) 3602-3611; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3086-12.2013
Elsa Fouragnan
1Interdepartmental Centre for Mind/Brain Sciences (CIMeC), University of Trento, 38060 Trento, Italy,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Gabriele Chierchia
1Interdepartmental Centre for Mind/Brain Sciences (CIMeC), University of Trento, 38060 Trento, Italy,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Susanne Greiner
2NeuroInformatics Laboratory (NILab) of Bruno Kessler Foundation, Neuroimaging Laboratory (LNIF) of CIMeC, University of Trento, 38060 Trento, Italy,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Remi Neveu
3Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), UMR 5229, University of Lyon, 69003 Lyon, France, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Paolo Avesani
2NeuroInformatics Laboratory (NILab) of Bruno Kessler Foundation, Neuroimaging Laboratory (LNIF) of CIMeC, University of Trento, 38060 Trento, Italy,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Giorgio Coricelli
1Interdepartmental Centre for Mind/Brain Sciences (CIMeC), University of Trento, 38060 Trento, Italy,
3Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), UMR 5229, University of Lyon, 69003 Lyon, France, and
4Department of Economics, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California 90089-0253
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Figures

  • Tables
  • Figure 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 1.

    Experimental design. A, One round of the two-player RTG. P1 is the payoff of the participant, who always plays as investor; P2 is the payoff of his counterpart, who plays as trustee. Before each round both players are endowed with €1. The participant moves first and chooses either to “keep” or “share.” If he keeps, both players maintain their initial endowments. If he shares the participant's endowment is multiplied by 3 and passed to the counterpart. The trustee then decides whether to share in turn (by returning €2), or to keep (by returning nothing). RTGs consisted of several consecutive rounds with the same counterpart. Participants played with many different counterparts and were told that their counterparts had already made their choices. B, Experimental conditions. Two conditions were adopted: (1) the “type” of counterpart and (2) the presence versus absence of “reputational priors.” Types: counterparts could be either “cooperative” or “individualistic” in their (simulated) behavior in RTGs; the former shared and the latter kept in 80% of RTG rounds. Reputational priors: participants were told that cues indicated whether the current counterpart had obtained a high or low score in a social orientation task (triangles indicated low scores, circles indicated high scores). Such priors reliably differentiated between the two counterpart types. C, Time line of the first RTG round. Presentation: face of the counterpart (with a prior or no-prior) was displayed for 3.5 s, and only presented for the first round of an RTG. Fixation: Fixation cross was presented during a jittered ISI. Choice: participants made their choice by pressing “Keep” or “Share.” Delay: ISI corresponding to the (simulated) decision of the counterpart. Outcome: outcome of the game and the payoffs of each player.

  • Figure 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 2.

    Behavioral results. A, Average percentage of decision to trust across conditions. Mean ± SE of participants' decision to trust (share) are broken down for trustee's type (Cooperative/Individualistic) and prior condition (Prior/No Prior); ***p < 0.001. Priors enabled participants to match (on average) their choices with the counterpart's level of trustworthiness. B, Learning dynamics across RTG rounds. Average percentage of the decision to trust for each round when playing with a “cooperative” versus “individualistic” counterpart, and when priors were present versus absent. When participants know nothing of their counterparts they tend to randomize between trusting and not trusting during initial rounds and adjust their choices to their counterparts' type in succeeding rounds. On the other hand, when priors are present, participants tend to rely on them already from early rounds. Shaded areas above and below the curves are SEs. C, Average payoffs in the Prior and No-Prior conditions. Average payoffs ± SE (in €) in Prior/No Prior conditions. When priors are available, participants significantly earn more when they adjust their choices to counterparts' types; **p < 0.01. D, Choices following unexpected behavior of cooperative and individualistic counterparts. Average (±SE) of percentage of “keep” choices in prior versus no-prior condition at time t, following rounds in which participants shared and a cooperative counterpart violated their trust by deciding to keep (at t − 1). Decisions to Keep at time t (i.e., retaliation) were less frequent when priors were available. E, Percentage of “share” choices (at t) following rounds in which participants had kept and an individualistic counterpart has shared (at t − 1).

  • Figure 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 3.

    mPFC encodes reputational priors when a new counterpart is first presented. A, Random effect analysis. When contrasting (Prior) > (No Prior) conditions at time of counterpart presentation, activity in the mPFC survived FWE correction, p < 0.05. B, Functional ROI analysis in mPFC. Functional ROI analyses further revealed percentage signal changes in the mPFC cortex MNI (0, 62, 31). The figure shows an increased activity when priors were present, regardless of their type, and decreased activity when there were no priors.

  • Figure 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 4.

    Brain regions parametrically correlated with the estimated “optimistic” and “pessimistic” decision value from the Prior model. A, Random effect fMRI analysis. To look for neural correlates of value signals (Qt) at time of choice, we entered the trial-by-trial estimates of the values of the two stimuli (Share and Keep) into a regression analysis against the fMRI data. We found enhanced activation in mPFC and dLPFC, surviving FWE correction, p < 0.05. B, Functional ROI analysis in mPFC. Percentage signal change by condition in the mPFC area represented in A. A similar pattern of activity was found in the dLPFC (not reported). These regions encoded prior valence (positive and negative) that guided decision to trust at time of choice. Error bars indicate SE.

  • Figure 5.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 5.

    Brain regions parametrically correlated with the estimated prediction error of the best-fitting RL model. A, Random effect fMRI analysis: Activity of the caudate showed significant correlation to the estimated prediction error signal in the no-prior condition (p < 0.05 FWE corrected). Such activities were not observed in this brain area in the prior condition. Peak coordinates are given in MNI space. Color bars indicate T values. B, Parameter estimates were extracted from the left caudate (−12, 20, 8) for the direct comparison between prior and no-prior conditions. Caudate activity correlates with prediction error in the no-prior condition only.

  • Figure 6.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 6.

    Functional connectivity between the caudate nucleus and vLPFC correlates with the choice to retaliate after violation of trust in the prior condition. A, PPI analysis. With a caudate seed, bilateral vLPFC shows stronger connectivity with this region in the prior compared with the no-prior conditions. B, vLPFC prevents retaliation to violation of trust in the prior condition. vLPFC anticorrelates with retaliation rate in the prior condition after participants experimented violation of trust from a cooperative counterpart. Spearman r = −0.6, p < 0.009. C, Reputational priors magnify striatal response to violation of trust. The caudate shows a stronger deactivation to violation of trust from a cooperative counterpart in the prior condition compared with the no-prior condition. D, Striatal responses to violation of trust and learning rates. The correlation between caudate and learning rates is significant only in the no-prior condition, thus striatal responses to violation of trust in the prior condition are not reflected in learning.

Tables

  • Figures
    • View popup
    Table 1.

    Learning model comparison

    Learning model comparison
    Classical model-free TD learning modelPrior+ and Prior− expectations RL learning model
    BIC76196460
    Log likelihood (random model = −4442)−3809−3230
    Pseudo r20.140.273
    • Bayesian information criterion value (BIC), Log likelihood, and the pseudo r2 suggest that the Prior+ and Prior− expectations TD learning model fits the observed behavior better the other TD learning models.

    • View popup
    Table 2.

    Averaged best-fitting parameter estimates (across subjects) SE

    Parameter estimate for best behavioral model, depicted as mean ± SE
    MeanSE
    Learning rate Prior condition αP0.3373± 0.0456
    Estimates for Cooperative counterparts0.327± 0.0424
    Estimates for Individualistic counterparts0.3475± 0.0398
    Learning rate No Prior condition αNP0.5075± 0.0689
    Estimates for Cooperative counterparts0.4686± 0.0701
    Estimates for Individualistic counterparts0.539± 0.0599
    Estimates learning rates for Invest trials (participants shared)0.3845± 0.0459
    Estimates learning rates for Non-Invest trials (participants kept)0.4603± 0.0476
    Softmax inv. Temp Betha β4.7769± 0.3149
    Initial value Cooperative Prior condition, QP+(0)1.3814± 0.1031
    Initial value Individualistic Prior condition, QP−(0)0.9838± 0.1055
    Initial value No Prior condition QNP(0)1.0641± 0.126
    • View popup
    Table 3.

    Activations correlated with contrasts of interest

    Analysis/LocationBASideCluster sizeTp value FWE cor.MNI coordinates (mm)
    XYZ
    Prior > No Prior (GLM 1)
        mPFC10958.266.8 × 10−0606231
        VTA—143.1770.0032 unc.0−1−5
    No Prior > Prior (GLM 1)
        Anterior insula44Left1063.9120.0009 unc.−36−415
        Anterior insula44Right553.4500.0017 unc.38310
    Parametric regression of Choice (GLM 2)
        mPFC10—876.5622.7 × 10−06−26410
        Lateral PFC46Left1225.9877.8 × 10−05−383832
        Lateral PFC46Right1096.3422.1 × 10−06303834
        Superior parietal lobule48Left435.016.7 × 10−04−38624
    Parametric regression at Outcome for the No Prior condition (GLM 2)
        Caudate nucleus—Left777.0918.9 × 10−06−14202
        Caudate nucleus—Right568.2987.9 × 10−0612168
    Violation of rust in the Prior condition (GLM 3, Cons > Incons)
        Caudate nucleus—Left826.782.8 × 10−06−101811
        Caudate nucleus—Right566.342.4 × 10−0612215
    • Note: BA, Brodmann area; mPFC, medial prefrontal cortex; VTA, ventral tegmental area.

Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 33 (8)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 33, Issue 8
20 Feb 2013
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Reputational Priors Magnify Striatal Responses to Violations of Trust
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Reputational Priors Magnify Striatal Responses to Violations of Trust
Elsa Fouragnan, Gabriele Chierchia, Susanne Greiner, Remi Neveu, Paolo Avesani, Giorgio Coricelli
Journal of Neuroscience 20 February 2013, 33 (8) 3602-3611; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3086-12.2013

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Reputational Priors Magnify Striatal Responses to Violations of Trust
Elsa Fouragnan, Gabriele Chierchia, Susanne Greiner, Remi Neveu, Paolo Avesani, Giorgio Coricelli
Journal of Neuroscience 20 February 2013, 33 (8) 3602-3611; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3086-12.2013
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Articles

  • Choice Behavior Guided by Learned, But Not Innate, Taste Aversion Recruits the Orbitofrontal Cortex
  • Maturation of Spontaneous Firing Properties after Hearing Onset in Rat Auditory Nerve Fibers: Spontaneous Rates, Refractoriness, and Interfiber Correlations
  • Insulin Treatment Prevents Neuroinflammation and Neuronal Injury with Restored Neurobehavioral Function in Models of HIV/AIDS Neurodegeneration
Show more Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Dissociable Neural Mechanisms Underlie the Effects of Attention on Visual Appearance and Response Bias
  • Rhythmic Entrainment Echoes in Auditory Perception
  • Impedance Rhythms in Human Limbic System
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.