Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Cortical Metabolic Activity Matches the Pattern of Visual Suppression in Strabismus

Daniel L. Adams, John R. Economides, Lawrence C. Sincich and Jonathan C. Horton
Journal of Neuroscience 27 February 2013, 33 (9) 3752-3759; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3228-12.2013
Daniel L. Adams
1Beckman Vision Center, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, California 94143,
2Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, The University of Trento, I-38122 Trento, Italy, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
John R. Economides
1Beckman Vision Center, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, California 94143,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Lawrence C. Sincich
3Department of Vision Sciences, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, Alabama 35294
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jonathan C. Horton
1Beckman Vision Center, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, California 94143,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

When an eye becomes deviated in early childhood, a person does not experience double vision, although the globes are aimed at different targets. The extra image is prevented from reaching perception in subjects with alternating exotropia by suppression of each eye's peripheral temporal retina. To test the impact of visual suppression on neuronal activity in primary (striate) visual cortex, the pattern of cytochrome oxidase (CO) staining was examined in four macaques raised with exotropia by disinserting the medial rectus muscles shortly following birth. No ocular dominance columns were visible in opercular cortex, where the central visual field is represented, indicating that signals coming from the central retina in each eye were perceived. However, the border strips at the edges of ocular dominance columns appeared pale, reflecting a loss of activity in binocular cells from disruption of fusion. In calcarine cortex, where the peripheral visual field is represented, there were alternating pale and dark bands resembling ocular dominance columns. To interpret the CO staining pattern, [3H]proline was injected into the right eye in two monkeys. In the right calcarine cortex, the pale CO columns matched the labeled proline columns of the right eye. In the left calcarine cortex, the pale CO columns overlapped the unlabeled columns of the left eye in the autoradiograph. Therefore, metabolic activity was reduced in the ipsilateral eye's ocular dominance columns which serve peripheral temporal retina, in a fashion consistent with the topographic organization of suppression scotomas in humans with exotropia.

Introduction

Many people with strabismus alternate fixation between the eyes. This ability is especially common in exotropia (Cotter et al., 2009). It has been controversial how diplopia is avoided in strabismus, given that the ocular axes point in different directions (Joose et al., 2000). In a recent study, the visual fields were mapped binocularly under dichoptic conditions in a cohort of subjects with alternating exotropia and normal visual acuity in each eye (Economides et al., 2012). Perception was not suppressed completely in the deviated eye. Instead, visual field maps showed a vertical border passing approximately midway between the center of gaze in each eye, dividing the binocular field into two separate zones (Fig. 1). In each zone, perception was governed by only one eye, while the other eye was suppressed. The zone of suppression corresponded to the peripheral temporal retina of each eye. Surprisingly, the fovea of the deviated eye was not suppressed. To avoid visual confusion, its perceived location was shifted to offset the ocular deviation, a phenomenon known as anomalous retinal correspondence (von Noorden and Campos, 2002).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Schematic diagram showing perception of the visual scene by subjects with exotropia. Here, the right eye is shown looking at a central cross and the left eye is deviated by 16°, causing the image of the cross to land on its temporal retina. When the left eye fixates centrally, the converse occurs. Dashed lines represent the nasal extent of each eye's visual field. Blue and red shading of eyes indicates portions of each retina engaged in perception; gray shading denotes suppressed temporal retina. The smaller the exotropic deviation, the closer temporal suppression extends toward the vertical meridians in each eye. In striate cortex, dark and light CO columns (depicted by blue/gray or red/gray shading) occur in the peripheral visual field representation (from 8° to 64°), reflecting suppression of neuronal activity in ocular dominance columns supplied by the peripheral temporal (ipsilateral gray) retinas. In the central visual field representation (from 0° to 8°), both retinas are active perceptually. Consequently, CO staining density is equal in the cores of the ocular dominance columns, indicated by alternating red and blue shading. It is reduced, however, in binocular border strips (thin white lines) along the edges of ocular dominance columns because fusion is absent.

The primary (striate) visual cortex contains alternating bands of inputs serving each eye, called ocular dominance columns (Hubel and Wiesel, 1977; LeVay et al., 1985). It also contains an orderly retinotopic map (Daniel and Whitteridge, 1961; Van Essen et al., 1984). If information from each eye's peripheral temporal retina is suppressed in alternating exotropia, one should expect a decrement in neuronal activity in the ipsilateral eye's ocular dominance columns in the representation of the peripheral visual field (Fig. 1). The relative level of neuronal activity in the cortex can be assessed by cytochrome oxidase (CO) histochemistry (Deyoe et al., 1995). In normal animals, there is a uniform distribution of CO staining in layer 4C (Horton, 1984). In contrast, after removal of one eye, ocular dominance columns become visible, owing to reduced activity in neurons driven formerly by the missing eye (Horton and Hedley-Whyte, 1984; Adams et al., 2007).

We report a pattern of CO staining in striate cortex of macaques raised with alternating exotropia that is consistent with the prediction that activity is reduced in the ipsilateral eye's ocular dominance columns in the peripheral representation of the visual fields. In previous studies, changes in cortical CO activity were induced by eye removal, eyelid suture, laser lesion, or tetrodotoxin injection (Wong-Riley, 1979; Horton, 1984; Wong-Riley and Carroll, 1984; Tychsen and Burkhalter, 1997; Horton and Hocking, 1998a). The abnormal CO activity we now describe in strabismus stems not from a reduction of retinal activity, but rather, appears to arise from inhibition of cortical activity related to visual suppression.

Materials and Methods

Experiments were conducted in four male Rhesus monkeys obtained from the California National Primate Research Center (Davis, CA). Procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees at the University of California, Davis, and the University of California, San Francisco.

In each monkey, an exotropia was induced by disinsertion of the medial rectus muscle tendon in both eyes. The procedures were performed at ages 10, 14, 26, and 32 d. General anesthesia was provided with ketamine HCl (15 mg/kg, i.m.), supplemented by local anesthesia with 1% proparacaine HCl. After tenotomy, the muscle was released and allowed to retract into the orbit. The animals were returned to their mothers within an hour and healed rapidly from the eye muscle surgery. The day after surgery, all monkeys had a large exotropia, measuring 30–45°, and limited adduction in each eye.

In each animal, adduction improved gradually and the magnitude of the exotropia decreased steadily over the months following surgery. In macaques raised with exotropia caused by free tenotomy of the medial rectus muscles, the severed muscles spontaneously reattach to the globe (Economides et al., 2007). This phenomenon presumably accounts for the partial recovery of adduction and the eventual reduction in exotropia. However, the monkeys never regained fusion because their eyes were misaligned throughout the critical period for binocular vision.

Ocular alignment was measured by photographing the monkeys periodically in their cages at a distance of ∼1 m, using a 100 mm macro lens with a ring-flash. In macaques, each millimeter of decentration of the corneal light reflex is equal to 14° (Quick and Boothe, 1989). The animals' eye preference for fixation was assessed by observing their spontaneous behavior and by testing their ocular fixation while reaching for small food treats.

Two animals were killed at age 3 years by intravenous injection of pentobarbital (150 mg/kg). The unperfused, unfixed brains were removed immediately and placed into chilled 0.1 m phosphate buffer. Flatmounts of striate cortex were prepared from each hemisphere and fixed overnight in 1% paraformaldehyde and 30% sucrose. Tangential sections were cut at 60 μm on a freezing microtome and processed for CO activity.

In the remaining two animals, we injected tritiated proline to label the ocular dominance columns. After general anesthesia with ketamine HCl (20 mg/kg, i.m.) and topical anesthesia with 1% proparacaine HCl, the pupils of both eyes were dilated with 1% tropicamide and 2.5% phenylephrine HCl. Both fundi were examined with an indirect ophthalmoscope to confirm that they were normal. Two millicuries of L-[2,3,4,5-3H]proline (PerkinElmer) in 25 μl of sterile balanced salt solution were injected into the vitreous of the right eye. The injection was made through the pars plana, 3.0 mm from the corneal limbus, using a 30 gauge U-100 0.3 cc insulin syringe. The fundus was examined immediately after the tracer injection to make sure that the retina was undamaged.

After a week to allow time for transneuronal transport, the monkey was placed under general anesthesia with ketamine HCl. The pupil reflexes were tested for an afferent pupil defect. After pupil dilation, the fundi were examined again with an indirect ophthalmoscope. Neither monkey showed any evidence of ocular injury or inflammation from the [3H]proline tracer injection.

One monkey was killed at age 18 months, and the other animal at age 4 years. After intravenous injection of pentobarbital (150 mg/kg), transcardial perfusion was performed with normal saline followed by 1 L of 1% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 m phosphate buffer, pH 7.4. The entire primary visual cortex from each occipital lobe was flatmounted. The first 10 sections were cut tangentially at 40 μm with a freezing microtome and processed for CO. Thereafter, alternating sections were cut either for autoradiography (30 μm) or CO (40 μm). Autoradiographs were processed as described previously (Horton and Hocking, 1997). Digital photographs were taken of each cortical section using a SPOT RT color CCD camera (Diagnostic Instruments) mounted on an Olympus SZH10 microscope. The columns visible in CO-processed sections were identified as dark or light before examining adjacent autoradiographs to determine whether they belonged to the right eye or the left eye.

Results

In two monkeys raised with alternating exotropia, the primary visual cortex in each hemisphere was processed for CO activity at age 3 years. In the calcarine cortex, where the peripheral visual field is represented, there were alternating pale and dark bands in layer 4C (Fig. 2). They appeared to be ocular dominance columns, but no second label was available to prove their identity. In the opercular region, where the central visual field is represented, this pattern was absent. Instead, there were pale, thin zones in layer 4C that resembled the border strips located along the boundaries of ocular dominance columns (Horton and Hocking, 1998a; Takahata et al., 2009). The pale strips were faint, making them difficult to appreciate, but the main point is that CO activity was equal in the cores of the ocular dominance columns serving each eye.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

CO patterns in striate cortex in exotropia. a, c, Alternating light and dark CO columns (arrows) in layer 4C in the peripheral visual field representation of a monkey with a 20° exotropia induced at age 14 d. The dark columns are 700 μm apart, consistent with the width of a pair of ocular dominance columns. b, d, In the central visual field representation, thin pale strips are present (arrows), spaced about twice as closely as the columns in a and c. Following their trajectory into lighter-staining layer 4B, they emerge in the pale areas between rows of patches, suggesting that they are located at the border of each ocular dominance column.

In two other monkeys, the ocular dominance columns were labeled transneuronally by injection of [3H]proline into the right eye (Wiesel et al., 1974). Figure 3 shows a monkey at age 4 years, a week before tracer injection. It had undergone eye muscle surgery at age 32 d to induce exotropia. The animal alternated fixation between the eyes, but had a slight preference for the right eye. The nasal decentration of the corneal light reflex in the left eye corresponded to an exotropia of 7°.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Assessment of exotropia. The monkey is fixating on the camera with his right eye. The left eye is deviated outward, causing nasal decentration of its corneal light reflex by 0.5 mm, which corresponds to 7°. The monkey alternated fixation freely, but preferred the right eye.

Figures 4a and 5a show flatmount sections of each primary visual cortex processed for CO from this monkey. These sections were chosen because large portions passed through 4C, which is the main termination layer for projections from the lateral geniculate body. In cortex representing the central 4°, CO staining revealed no ocular dominance columns. Instead, there were pale thin zones in layer 4C (Figs. 4b, 5b), as observed in the first two monkeys. These zones were outlined by hand (Figs. 4d, 5d) and compared with the ocular dominance columns in adjacent autoradiographs (Figs. 4c, 5c). The pale CO zones corresponded precisely to the border strips straddling the edges of ocular dominance columns (Figs. 4e, 5e).

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Patterns of CO activity in left striate cortex in exotropia. a, Single section from a flatmount shows no ocular dominance columns in layer 4C from the foveal representation to an eccentricity of about 4°. From 4° to the monocular crescent representation, there are alternating light and dark CO columns. b, Boxed area from 1° to 2° in the section above. CO stain reveals extremely faint, pale strips in layer 4C, but no ocular dominance columns. c, Adjacent [3H]proline autoradiograph labeling the ocular dominance columns in layer 4C in the same region. d, Hand tracing of the pale CO strips, superimposed on the image in b. e, Tracing of the pale strips in d transferred onto the autoradiograph in c, showing that the pale strips straddle the borders of the ocular dominance columns.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Patterns of CO activity in right striate cortex in exotropia. a, Just as in the left cortex (Fig. 4), there are no ocular dominance columns in layer 4C from the foveal representation to 4°, but from 4° to the monocular crescent representation there are alternating light and dark CO columns. b, Boxed area from central field representation of right striate cortex in a, showing subtle, pale border strips in 4C, but no pattern of ocular dominance columns. c, Adjacent autoradiograph of ocular dominance columns. d, Hand tracing of pale strips, superimposed on b. e, Tracing in d, transferred onto c, confirming that pale strips are located at borders of ocular dominance columns.

In striate cortex representing visual field beyond the central 4°, there were alternating light and dark CO bands in layer 4C. In the left cortex, the pale CO columns matched the dark, unlabeled columns serving the left eye in the proline autoradiograph (Fig. 6a,b). This correlation means that CO activity was reduced in ocular dominance columns supplied by the peripheral temporal retina of the ipsilateral eye. In the right cortex, the pale CO columns matched the bright, labeled proline columns of the right eye (Fig. 6c,d). Again, in this hemisphere CO activity was reduced in ocular dominance columns driven by the peripheral temporal retina of the ipsilateral eye. These findings were replicated in the other exotropic monkey that received a [3H]proline eye injection. It had an exotropia of 10°. At the representation of 5° eccentricity in each hemisphere, a transition occurred from the pale border strip pattern to the ocular dominance column pattern. Pale CO staining appeared in the columns serving the temporal, ipsilateral retina.

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Reduced CO activity in ipsilateral eye's columns in cortex representing peripheral visual field. a, Boxed region from the peripheral left cortex in Figure 4, showing alternating light and dark CO columns surrounding the blind spot representation (asterisk), which is not complete because the lower border enters layer 5. Three pale columns are marked with arrows. b, Adjacent autoradiograph in darkfield, with dark columns representing the ocular dominance columns of the unlabeled left eye (arrows). They correspond to the pale CO columns above, indicating that CO activity is reduced in the left eye's columns. c, Boxed region from the peripheral right cortex in Figure 5, showing alternating light and dark CO columns around the blind spot. Three pale columns are marked by arrows. d, Adjacent autoradiograph, showing that bright, labeled columns from the right eye match the pale columns above, indicating that CO activity is reduced in the right eye's columns in this hemisphere. Note solid labeling from the right eye of the left eye's blind spot “representation.”

In each hemisphere, the pattern of alternating light and dark ocular dominance columns was interrupted by a large, oval region corresponding to the blind spot of the contralateral eye (Fig. 6a,c). In this monocular area, CO staining was uniform. The tissue in the blind spot regions qualitatively matched the density of pale columns in the surrounding mosaic of ocular dominance columns. Striate cortex corresponding to the blind spot “representation” is supplied exclusively by temporal retina from the ipsilateral eye (Fig. 6b,d). Reduced CO activity in the blind spot representations was consistent with the conclusion derived from the proline autoradiographs, namely, that temporal retina in each eye serving the peripheral visual field was suppressed.

CO activity was abnormal in other cortical layers besides 4C. In the central visual field representation, patches serving each eye were labeled with equal intensity in layer 2/3, as were the ocular dominance columns in layer 4C. However, the rows formed by the patches were unusually distinct, owing to reduction of CO in the border strips situated between each row. In cortex serving the peripheral visual field, patches were arrayed in alternating light and dark rows, in register with the light and dark core zones in layer 4C (Fig. 7). The contrast between light and dark rows was subtle, compared with the strong effect of enucleation or eyelid suture (Horton, 1984), but it demonstrated that the metabolic dominance of the contralateral eye was propagated into the upper layers. Layer 4B and 4A also showed CO columns, with the darker columns serving the contralateral eye.

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

Suppression causes loss of CO activity in patches in layer 3. a, CO activity in layer 4C showing three pale columns serving the ipsilateral eye (arrows) in Figure 6c at higher magnification. The asterisk marks the blind spot area, where ocular dominance columns are absent. b, Section through layer 3 from the same region, showing that patches in rows (arrows) aligned with the pale columns in layer 4C are slightly pale and small, compared with patches in intercalated rows serving the contralateral eye. Toward the upper right, in the blind spot area, the pattern of alternating light and dark rows of patches disappears, because only the ipsilateral eye is represented.

In area V2, the alternating pattern of pale-thin-pale-thick stripes was normal. This was not surprising, given that even removal of one eye does not affect the appearance of V2 CO stripes (Horton and Hocking, 1998b). Although thick stripes have been linked to stereopsis, they appear normal in animals with no stereopsis.

Discussion

The pattern of suppression that occurs in human subjects with exotropia has been tested by using a variety of manual techniques to map the visual fields (Travers, 1938; Herzau, 1980; Cooper and Record, 1986; Melek et al., 1992; Joosse et al., 1999). More recently, automated perimetry under dichoptic stimulus conditions has been used (Economides et al., 2012). These studies have shown that signals arising from each eye's peripheral temporal retina are suppressed in exotropia. We now provide anatomical evidence that signals emanating from the peripheral temporal retina are suppressed in the primary visual cortex. A reduction was observed in neuronal activity, assayed by CO staining, in ocular dominance columns serving the ipsilateral eye in the peripheral visual field representation of striate cortex in each hemisphere of monkeys raised with alternating exotropia. The reduced activity cannot be attributed to ocular injury from the [3H]proline tracer injection, because pale ocular dominance columns were also present in animals that received no eye injection.

In the representation of the central visual fields, CO activity remained equal in the core zones of the ocular dominance columns serving each eye. This balanced CO activity was due to two factors. First, the monkeys alternated fixation, acquiring targets with each fovea for a substantial fraction of the time. Second, the fovea of the deviated eye probably was not suppressed. The evidence for this latter conclusion is indirect, coming from exotropic humans who alternate fixation freely and have no amblyopia. In such individuals, visual field studies have shown that the central retina of each eye is engaged simultaneously in perception (Economides et al., 2012). Although our monkeys were not tested to define regions of visual field suppression, we have shown that monkeys with exotropia from medial rectus muscle surgery alternate fixation and lack amblyopia (Economides et al., 2007).

Although ocular dominance columns were not labeled preferentially in the central visual field representation, the pattern of CO activity in striate cortex was not normal. There was a subtle reduction of CO staining along the boundaries of the ocular dominance columns in layer 4C in slender regions known as border strips (Horton and Hocking, 1998a; Takahata et al., 2009). In the macaque, layer 4C contains mostly monocular cells, but there is a small population of binocular cells along the borders of ocular dominance columns (Hubel and Wiesel, 1977; LeVay et al., 1980). In the cat, disruption of ocular alignment reduces the overall firing rate of binocular cells (Kara and Boyd, 2009). For obligatory binocular cells, the reduction is absolute (Poggio, 1995). Presumably, strabismus reduced the firing rate of binocular cells, causing a relative fall in CO levels within border strips. Interestingly, pale border strips are also present in newborn monkeys (Horton and Hocking, 1996a). In newborn monkeys, they disappear gradually as disparity tuning matures in binocular neurons (Endo et al., 2000).

Several previous studies have examined CO activity in striate cortex of macaques with early ocular misalignment. Tychsen and Burkhalter (1997) examined two monkeys, one with naturally occurring esotropia and another with strabismus from daily alternating ocular occlusion. In the first animal, the authors concluded that CO activity was reduced in the ocular dominance columns serving temporal retina. The columns were identified by intraocular injection of wheat-germ agglutinin conjugated to horseradish peroxidase (WGA-HRP). Anatomical data from one label (either CO or WGA-HRP) was compared with a drawing of the other label (Tychsen and Burkhalter, 1997, their Fig. 10). A potential problem is that WGA-HRP has the disadvantage of producing uveitis (Smecka et al., 1984; Horton and Hocking, 1996b), which can affect retinal function. It is possible that intraocular inflammation altered CO activity in the cortex, thereby confounding the interpretation of the metabolic patterns induced by strabismus. In the second animal, which eventually developed exotropia, the authors also concluded that CO activity was reduced in the ocular dominance columns serving temporal retina. However, the ocular dominance columns were not labeled with an independent tracer, so the cortical CO pattern could not be interpreted unambiguously. Fenstemaker et al. (2001) examined striate cortex in two monkeys raised with esotropia. In both monkeys, two patterns were observed: (1) pale border strips; and (2) alternating pale and dark ocular dominance columns. Our data are in good agreement with their results, because we also observed these two distinct CO patterns from strabismus. They were not in a position to relate the CO patterns to visual suppression because the identity of the columns was not established with an anatomical tracer. Together, the studies by Tychsen and Burkhalter (1997) and Fenstemaker et al. (2001) show conclusively that esotropia also produces abnormal patterns of CO activity in striate cortex. These intriguing patterns merit further study, comparison with ocular dominance columns, and correlation with visual suppression.

We reported abnormal CO activity in striate cortex in monkeys with ocular misalignment induced by eye muscle surgery during adulthood (Horton et al., 1999). It was suggested that regions showing pale ocular dominance columns might correspond to regions of visual field suppression in one eye. However, disruption of fusion in adults causes diplopia and is thought to create a sensory state more akin to binocular rivalry than strabismic suppression (Smith et al., 1985). For this reason, we decided to examine CO patterns in macaques with exotropia since infancy. The CO staining abnormalities are similar in monkeys raised with strabismus and those with adult-onset ocular misalignment. Because visual suppression is nearly universal in subjects with strabismus since infancy (von Noorden and Campos, 2002), it is reasonable to infer that the loss of CO activity in the ipsilateral eye's columns in peripheral cortex is a functional correlate of suppression. This inference is supported by the correspondence between the CO staining pattern in exotropic monkeys and the visual field suppression data from exotropic humans (Fig. 1).

In Figure 6, a and c, the CO staining in the blind spot representation is pale in each hemisphere, relative to the dark CO activity in the surrounding ocular dominance columns that serve the contralateral eye. The blind spot representation appears pale, because it is supplied only by ipsilateral temporal retina, which is suppressed in exotropia. Why should temporal retina be suppressed at a cortical locus that corresponds anatomically to the optic disc in the other eye? The likely reason is that in exotropia, the region in the temporal retina that would normally fill in the other eye's blind spot overlaps instead with a point near the other eye's center of gaze (Fig. 1). Therefore, suppression of this region of temporal retina is necessary to avoid diplopia.

It has been unclear what neural mechanism enables children with strabismus to avoid diplopia, or where visual suppression takes place in the brain. These data provide anatomical evidence that suppression is mediated at the level of the primary visual cortex. This point in the visual pathway is logical, because it is the first place in the cerebral cortex where ocular inputs are brought together and the last place where they remain segregated. However, it remains unknown whether suppression originates in the lateral geniculate nucleus or striate cortex, or is governed by feedback projections from higher visual cortical areas that must reconcile conflicting images (Logothetis et al., 1996).

In striate cortex, intrinsic horizontal connections extend for only a few millimeters (Rockland and Lund, 1983; Fitzpatrick et al., 1985; Yoshioka et al., 1996; Bosking et al., 1997). This would not be sufficient, for example, to allow neurons serving the fovea in one eye's ocular dominance columns to inhibit directly neurons serving the peripheral temporal retina in the other eye's ocular dominance columns. Moreover, strabismus leads to a selective loss of projections between cells in opposite ocular dominance columns (Löwel and Singer, 1992; Tychsen et al., 2004; Schmidt and Löwel, 2008), reducing the substrate for such inhibitory interactions. For these reasons, feedback from cells in higher visual cortical areas, which have much larger receptive fields, is more likely to control interocular suppression.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by Grants EY10217 (J.C.H.) and EY02162 (Beckman Vision Center) from the National Eye Institute, the Disney Award from Research to Prevent Blindness (J.C.H.), and a University of Alabama at Birmingham Impact Fund (L.C.S.). The California Regional Primate Research Center is supported by NIH Base Grant RR00169. Technical support was provided by Cristina M. Jocson and Valerie L. Wu.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Jonathan C. Horton, Beckman Vision Center, University of California, San Francisco, 10 Koret Way, San Francisco, CA 94143-0730. hortonj{at}vision.ucsf.edu

References

  1. ↵
    1. Adams DL,
    2. Sincich LC,
    3. Horton JC
    (2007) Complete pattern of ocular dominance columns in human primary visual cortex. J Neurosci 27:10391–10403.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    1. Bosking WH,
    2. Zhang Y,
    3. Schofield B,
    4. Fitzpatrick D
    (1997) Orientation selectivity and the arrangement of horizontal connections in tree shrew striate cortex. J Neurosci 17:2112–2127.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    1. Cooper J,
    2. Record CD
    (1986) Suppression and retinal correspondence in intermittent exotropia. Br J Ophthalmol 70:673–676.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Cotter SA,
    2. Tarczy-Hornoch K,
    3. Song E,
    4. Lin J,
    5. Borchert M,
    6. Azen SP,
    7. Varma R
    (2009) Fixation preference and visual acuity testing in a population-based cohort of preschool children with amblyopia risk factors. Ophthalmology 116:145–153.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Daniel PM,
    2. Whitteridge D
    (1961) The representation of the visual field on the cerebral cortex in monkeys. J Physiol 159:203–221.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    1. Deyoe EA,
    2. Trusk TC,
    3. Wong-Riley MT
    (1995) Activity correlates of cytochrome oxidase-defined compartments in granular and supragranular layers of primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey. Vis Neurosci 12:629–639.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Economides JR,
    2. Adams DL,
    3. Jocson CM,
    4. Horton JC
    (2007) Ocular motor behavior in macaques with surgical exotropia. J Neurophysiol 98:3411–3422.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    1. Economides JR,
    2. Adams DL,
    3. Horton JC
    (2012) Perception via the deviated eye in strabismus. J Neurosci 32:10286–10295.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    1. Endo M,
    2. Kaas JH,
    3. Jain N,
    4. Smith EL 3rd.,
    5. Chino Y
    (2000) Binocular cross-orientation suppression in the primary visual cortex (V1) of infant rhesus monkeys. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 41:4022–4031.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    1. Fenstemaker SB,
    2. Kiorpes L,
    3. Movshon JA
    (2001) Effects of experimental strabismus on the architecture of macaque monkey striate cortex. J Comp Neurol 438:300–317.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Fitzpatrick D,
    2. Lund JS,
    3. Blasdel GG
    (1985) Intrinsic connections of macaque striate cortex: afferent and efferent connections of lamina 4C. J Neurosci 5:3329–3349.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  12. ↵
    1. Herzau V
    (1980) Untersuchungen über das binokulare gesichtsfeld schielender. Doc Ophthalmol 49:221–284.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Horton JC
    (1984) Cytochrome oxidase patches: a new cytoarchitectonic feature of monkey visual cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 304:199–253.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    1. Horton JC,
    2. Hedley-Whyte ET
    (1984) Mapping of cytochrome oxidase patches and ocular dominance columns in human visual cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 304:255–272.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Horton JC,
    2. Hocking DR
    (1996a) An adult-like pattern of ocular dominance columns in striate cortex of newborn monkeys prior to visual experience. J Neurosci 16:1791–1807.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. ↵
    1. Horton JC,
    2. Hocking DR
    (1996b) Anatomical demonstration of ocular dominance columns in striate cortex of the squirrel monkey. J Neurosci 16:5510–5522.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    1. Horton JC,
    2. Hocking DR
    (1997) Timing of the critical period for plasticity of ocular dominance columns in macaque striate cortex. J Neurosci 17:3684–3709.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    1. Horton JC,
    2. Hocking DR
    (1998a) Monocular core zones and binocular border strips in primate striate cortex revealed by the contrasting effects of enucleation, eyelid suture, and retinal laser lesions on cytochrome oxidase activity. J Neurosci 18:5433–5455.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. Horton JC,
    2. Hocking DR
    (1998b) Effect of early monocular enucleation upon ocular dominance columns and cytochrome oxidase activity in monkey and human visual cortex. Vis Neurosci 15:289–303.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Horton JC,
    2. Hocking DR,
    3. Adams DL
    (1999) Metabolic mapping of suppression scotomas in striate cortex of macaques with experimental strabismus. J Neurosci 19:7111–7129.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    1. Hubel DH,
    2. Wiesel TN
    (1977) The Ferrier lecture: functional architecture of macaque monkey visual cortex. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 198:1–59.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    1. Joose MV,
    2. Simonsz HJ,
    3. de Jong PT
    (2000) The visual field in strabismus: a historical review of studies on amblyopia and suppression. Strabismus 8:135–149.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Joosse MV,
    2. Simonsz HJ,
    3. van Minderhout EM,
    4. Mulder PG,
    5. de Jong PT
    (1999) Quantitative visual fields under binocular viewing conditions in primary and consecutive divergent strabismus. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol 237:535–545.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Kara P,
    2. Boyd JD
    (2009) A micro-architecture for binocular disparity and ocular dominance in visual cortex. Nature 458:627–631.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. LeVay S,
    2. Wiesel TN,
    3. Hubel DH
    (1980) The development of ocular dominance columns in normal and visually deprived monkeys. J Comp Neurol 191:1–51.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. LeVay S,
    2. Connolly M,
    3. Houde J,
    4. Van Essen DC
    (1985) The complete pattern of ocular dominance stripes in the striate cortex and visual field of the macaque monkey. J Neurosci 5:486–501.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  27. ↵
    1. Logothetis NK,
    2. Leopold DA,
    3. Sheinberg DL
    (1996) What is rivalling during binocular rivalry? Nature 380:621–624.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Löwel S,
    2. Singer W
    (1992) Selection of intrinsic horizontal connections in the visual cortex by correlated neuronal activity. Science 255:209–212.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Melek N,
    2. Shokida F,
    3. Dominguez D,
    4. Zabalo S
    (1992) Intermittent exotropia: a study of suppression in the binocular visual field in 21 cases. Binocular Vis Quart 7:25–30.
    OpenUrl
  30. ↵
    1. Poggio GE
    (1995) Mechanisms of stereopsis in monkey visual cortex. Cereb Cortex 5:193–204.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  31. ↵
    1. Quick MW,
    2. Boothe RG
    (1989) Measurement of binocular alignment in normal monkeys and in monkeys with strabismus. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 30:1159–1168.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    1. Rockland KS,
    2. Lund JS
    (1983) Intrinsic laminar lattice connections in primate visual cortex. J Comp Neurol 216:303–318.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Schmidt KF,
    2. Löwel S
    (2008) Strabismus modifies intrinsic and inter-areal connections in cat area 18. Neuroscience 152:128–137.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Smecka Z,
    2. Stankus P,
    3. Lichnovský V
    (1984) Experimental uveitis in rabbits induced with BSA and horseradish peroxidase. Acta Univ Palacki Olomuc Fac Med 106:139–144.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Smith EL 3rd.,
    2. Levi DM,
    3. Manny RE,
    4. Harwerth RS,
    5. White JM
    (1985) The relationship between binocular rivalry and strabismic suppression. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 26:80–87.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. ↵
    1. Takahata T,
    2. Higo N,
    3. Kaas JH,
    4. Yamamori T
    (2009) Expression of immediate-early genes reveals functional compartments within ocular dominance columns after brief monocular inactivation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:12151–12155.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    1. Travers TA
    (1938) Suppression of Vision in Squint and Its Association with Retinal Correspondence and Amblyopia. Br J Ophthalmol 22:577–604.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  38. ↵
    1. Tychsen L,
    2. Burkhalter A
    (1997) Nasotemporal asymmetries in V1: ocular dominance columns of infant, adult, and strabismic macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol 388:32–46.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Tychsen L,
    2. Wong AM,
    3. Burkhalter A
    (2004) Paucity of horizontal connections for binocular vision in V1 of naturally strabismic macaques: cytochrome oxidase compartment specificity. J Comp Neurol 474:261–275.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Van Essen DC,
    2. Newsome WT,
    3. Maunsell JH
    (1984) The visual field representation in striate cortex of the macaque monkey: asymmetries, anisotropies, and individual variability. Vision Res 24:429–448.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. von Noorden GK,
    2. Campos EC
    (2002) Binocular vision and ocular motility: theory and management of strabismus (Mosby, St. Louis, MO), Ed 6.
  42. ↵
    1. Wiesel TN,
    2. Hubel DH,
    3. Lam DM
    (1974) Autoradiographic demonstration of ocular-dominance columns in the monkey striate cortex by means of transneuronal transport. Brain Res 79:273–279.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Wong-Riley M,
    2. Carroll EW
    (1984) Effect of impulse blockage on cytochrome oxidase activity in monkey visual system. Nature 307:262–264.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Wong-Riley M
    (1979) Changes in the visual system of monocularly sutured or enucleated cats demonstrable with cytochrome oxidase histochemistry. Brain Res 171:11–28.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Yoshioka T,
    2. Blasdel GG,
    3. Levitt JB,
    4. Lund JS
    (1996) Relation between patterns of intrinsic lateral connectivity, ocular dominance, and cytochrome oxidase-reactive regions in macaque monkey striate cortex. Cereb Cortex 6:297–310.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 33 (9)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 33, Issue 9
27 Feb 2013
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Cortical Metabolic Activity Matches the Pattern of Visual Suppression in Strabismus
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Cortical Metabolic Activity Matches the Pattern of Visual Suppression in Strabismus
Daniel L. Adams, John R. Economides, Lawrence C. Sincich, Jonathan C. Horton
Journal of Neuroscience 27 February 2013, 33 (9) 3752-3759; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3228-12.2013

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Cortical Metabolic Activity Matches the Pattern of Visual Suppression in Strabismus
Daniel L. Adams, John R. Economides, Lawrence C. Sincich, Jonathan C. Horton
Journal of Neuroscience 27 February 2013, 33 (9) 3752-3759; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3228-12.2013
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Articles

  • Choice Behavior Guided by Learned, But Not Innate, Taste Aversion Recruits the Orbitofrontal Cortex
  • Maturation of Spontaneous Firing Properties after Hearing Onset in Rat Auditory Nerve Fibers: Spontaneous Rates, Refractoriness, and Interfiber Correlations
  • Insulin Treatment Prevents Neuroinflammation and Neuronal Injury with Restored Neurobehavioral Function in Models of HIV/AIDS Neurodegeneration
Show more Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • The Role of Visual Experience in Individual Differences of Brain Connectivity
  • Spontaneous Alpha-Band Oscillations Bias Subjective Contrast Perception
  • Cortico-Striatal Activity Characterizes Human Safety Learning via Pavlovian Conditioned Inhibition
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2022 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.