Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Active Avoidance Learning Requires Prefrontal Suppression of Amygdala-Mediated Defensive Reactions

Justin M. Moscarello and Joseph E. LeDoux
Journal of Neuroscience 27 February 2013, 33 (9) 3815-3823; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2596-12.2013
Justin M. Moscarello
1Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, New York 10003, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Joseph E. LeDoux
1Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, New York 10003, and
2Emotional Brain Institute, Nathan Kline Institute, Orangeburg, New York 10962
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Published eLetters

Guidelines

As a forum for professional feedback, submissions of letters are open to all. You do not need to be a subscriber. To avoid redundancy, we urge you to read other people's letters before submitting your own. Name, current appointment, place of work, and email address are required to send a letter, and will be published with your review. We also require that you declare any competing financial interests. Unprofessional submissions will not be considered or responded to.

Submit a Response to This Article
Compose eLetter

More information about text formats

Plain text

  • No HTML tags allowed.
  • Web page addresses and e-mail addresses turn into links automatically.
  • Lines and paragraphs break automatically.
Author Information
First or given name, e.g. 'Peter'.
Your last, or family, name, e.g. 'MacMoody'.
Your email address, e.g. higgs-boson@gmail.com
Your role and/or occupation, e.g. 'Orthopedic Surgeon'.
Your organization or institution (if applicable), e.g. 'Royal Free Hospital'.
Statement of Competing Interests
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
14 + 5 =
Solve this simple math problem and enter the result. E.g. for 1+3, enter 4.

Vertical Tabs

Jump to comment:

  • Considering the translational value of signaled active avoidance paradigms: The case of anxiety disorders
    Thomas R. Armstrong
    Submitted on: 12 April 2013
  • Submitted on: (12 April 2013)
    Page navigation anchor for Considering the translational value of signaled active avoidance paradigms: The case of anxiety disorders
    Considering the translational value of signaled active avoidance paradigms: The case of anxiety disorders
    • Thomas R. Armstrong, Doctoral Candidate
    • Other Contributors:
      • Bunmi O. Olatunji

    In this study, Moscarello and LeDoux (2013) provide new insights into the role of infralimbic prefrontal cortex in resolving the conflict between Pavlovian conditioned freezing and instrumentally conditioned avoidance behavior in the process of signaled active avoidance learning. The authors conclude that these findings may have clinical implications for "therapies designed to facilitate adaptive coping strategies," and i...

    Show More

    In this study, Moscarello and LeDoux (2013) provide new insights into the role of infralimbic prefrontal cortex in resolving the conflict between Pavlovian conditioned freezing and instrumentally conditioned avoidance behavior in the process of signaled active avoidance learning. The authors conclude that these findings may have clinical implications for "therapies designed to facilitate adaptive coping strategies," and in a recent commentary, LeDoux (2013) suggests that "proactive avoidance" learned in this paradigm is distinct from the "pathological avoidance" that maintains anxiety disorders, and that future treatments may work by facilitating the transition from bad avoidance (freezing) to good avoidance (escape), perhaps by targeting the primate homolog of the neural substrates identified by Moscarello and LeDoux (2013). However, we contend that an important caveat is needed in considering the translational value of these findings for treating anxiety disorders in humans. Active avoidance can be beneficial when a human has an accurate representation of the contingency between the conditioned stimulus (CS) and the unconditioned stimulus (US). However, active avoidance can be quite harmful if one is wrong about a CS-US contingency, and anxiety disorders are virtually defined by this type of error. Individuals with panic disorder, for example, believe that symptoms of sudden autonomic arousal (CSs such as shortness of breath, rapid heart beat, or dizziness) signal catastrophic outcomes (USs such as a heart attack or psychotic break). These individuals do not freeze in response to CSs; instead, they are quite active in their avoidance, going out of their way to find means of escaping situations that elicit and sustain panic CSs. Such avoidance provides transient relief and perhaps a renewed sense of agency; however, it does not help to resolve panic disorder. Instead, it leads to agoraphobia: marked distress in situations that do not allow escape in the event of a panic attack. Indeed, all anxiety disorders are marked by efforts to prevent USs through active responding to CSs. Proactive avoidance is functionally equivalent to a host of other "safety behaviors" that maintain anxiety by preventing corrective learning about the CS-US contingency. To be precise, individuals with anxiety disorders mistake safety behaviors for negative occasion setters, that is, modulators that determine whether or not the CS will predict the US. In appearing to provide control over the US, safety behaviors may provide short-term benefits, but at the cost of maintaining the distortions that create pathological fear. In fact, the short-term relief provided by safety behaviors is itself problematic, as these behaviors become deeply entrenched due to their negative reinforcement and may become a serious impediment to exposure therapy. In conclusion, while the findings of Moscarello and LeDoux (2013) are intriguing with respect to animal models of normative fear learning, the mechanisms highlighted by this study may be more relevant to the maintenance of pathological fear, rather than its treatment, in humans.

    Moscarello JM & LeDoux JE (2013) Active Avoidance Learning Requires Prefrontal Suppression of Amygdala-Mediated Defensive Reactions. J Neurosci 33: 3815-3823.

    LeDoux J (2013) For the anxious, avoidance can have an upside. The New York Times (online only). Retrieved from http://opinionator.blogs.nytimes.com/2013/04/07/for-the-anxious-avoidance- can-have-an-upside/

    Conflict of Interest:

    None declared

    Show Less
    Competing Interests: None declared.
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 33 (9)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 33, Issue 9
27 Feb 2013
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Active Avoidance Learning Requires Prefrontal Suppression of Amygdala-Mediated Defensive Reactions
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Active Avoidance Learning Requires Prefrontal Suppression of Amygdala-Mediated Defensive Reactions
Justin M. Moscarello, Joseph E. LeDoux
Journal of Neuroscience 27 February 2013, 33 (9) 3815-3823; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2596-12.2013

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Active Avoidance Learning Requires Prefrontal Suppression of Amygdala-Mediated Defensive Reactions
Justin M. Moscarello, Joseph E. LeDoux
Journal of Neuroscience 27 February 2013, 33 (9) 3815-3823; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2596-12.2013
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

  • Considering the translational value of signaled active avoidance paradigms: The case of anxiety disorders
    Thomas R. Armstrong
    Published on: 12 April 2013
  • Published on: (12 April 2013)
    Page navigation anchor for Considering the translational value of signaled active avoidance paradigms: The case of anxiety disorders
    Considering the translational value of signaled active avoidance paradigms: The case of anxiety disorders
    • Thomas R. Armstrong, Doctoral Candidate
    • Other Contributors:
      • Bunmi O. Olatunji

    In this study, Moscarello and LeDoux (2013) provide new insights into the role of infralimbic prefrontal cortex in resolving the conflict between Pavlovian conditioned freezing and instrumentally conditioned avoidance behavior in the process of signaled active avoidance learning. The authors conclude that these findings may have clinical implications for "therapies designed to facilitate adaptive coping strategies," and i...

    Show More

    In this study, Moscarello and LeDoux (2013) provide new insights into the role of infralimbic prefrontal cortex in resolving the conflict between Pavlovian conditioned freezing and instrumentally conditioned avoidance behavior in the process of signaled active avoidance learning. The authors conclude that these findings may have clinical implications for "therapies designed to facilitate adaptive coping strategies," and in a recent commentary, LeDoux (2013) suggests that "proactive avoidance" learned in this paradigm is distinct from the "pathological avoidance" that maintains anxiety disorders, and that future treatments may work by facilitating the transition from bad avoidance (freezing) to good avoidance (escape), perhaps by targeting the primate homolog of the neural substrates identified by Moscarello and LeDoux (2013). However, we contend that an important caveat is needed in considering the translational value of these findings for treating anxiety disorders in humans. Active avoidance can be beneficial when a human has an accurate representation of the contingency between the conditioned stimulus (CS) and the unconditioned stimulus (US). However, active avoidance can be quite harmful if one is wrong about a CS-US contingency, and anxiety disorders are virtually defined by this type of error. Individuals with panic disorder, for example, believe that symptoms of sudden autonomic arousal (CSs such as shortness of breath, rapid heart beat, or dizziness) signal catastrophic outcomes (USs such as a heart attack or psychotic break). These individuals do not freeze in response to CSs; instead, they are quite active in their avoidance, going out of their way to find means of escaping situations that elicit and sustain panic CSs. Such avoidance provides transient relief and perhaps a renewed sense of agency; however, it does not help to resolve panic disorder. Instead, it leads to agoraphobia: marked distress in situations that do not allow escape in the event of a panic attack. Indeed, all anxiety disorders are marked by efforts to prevent USs through active responding to CSs. Proactive avoidance is functionally equivalent to a host of other "safety behaviors" that maintain anxiety by preventing corrective learning about the CS-US contingency. To be precise, individuals with anxiety disorders mistake safety behaviors for negative occasion setters, that is, modulators that determine whether or not the CS will predict the US. In appearing to provide control over the US, safety behaviors may provide short-term benefits, but at the cost of maintaining the distortions that create pathological fear. In fact, the short-term relief provided by safety behaviors is itself problematic, as these behaviors become deeply entrenched due to their negative reinforcement and may become a serious impediment to exposure therapy. In conclusion, while the findings of Moscarello and LeDoux (2013) are intriguing with respect to animal models of normative fear learning, the mechanisms highlighted by this study may be more relevant to the maintenance of pathological fear, rather than its treatment, in humans.

    Moscarello JM & LeDoux JE (2013) Active Avoidance Learning Requires Prefrontal Suppression of Amygdala-Mediated Defensive Reactions. J Neurosci 33: 3815-3823.

    LeDoux J (2013) For the anxious, avoidance can have an upside. The New York Times (online only). Retrieved from http://opinionator.blogs.nytimes.com/2013/04/07/for-the-anxious-avoidance- can-have-an-upside/

    Conflict of Interest:

    None declared

    Show Less
    Competing Interests: None declared.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Articles

  • Memory Retrieval Has a Dynamic Influence on the Maintenance Mechanisms That Are Sensitive to ζ-Inhibitory Peptide (ZIP)
  • Neurophysiological Evidence for a Cortical Contribution to the Wakefulness-Related Drive to Breathe Explaining Hypocapnia-Resistant Ventilation in Humans
  • Monomeric Alpha-Synuclein Exerts a Physiological Role on Brain ATP Synthase
Show more Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Memory Retrieval Has a Dynamic Influence on the Maintenance Mechanisms That Are Sensitive to ζ-Inhibitory Peptide (ZIP)
  • Neurophysiological Evidence for a Cortical Contribution to the Wakefulness-Related Drive to Breathe Explaining Hypocapnia-Resistant Ventilation in Humans
  • Monomeric Alpha-Synuclein Exerts a Physiological Role on Brain ATP Synthase
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.