Abstract
Nonvisual photosensation enables animals to sense light without sight. However, the cellular and molecular mechanisms of nonvisual photobehaviors are poorly understood, especially in vertebrate animals. Here, we describe the photomotor response (PMR), a robust and reproducible series of motor behaviors in zebrafish that is elicited by visual wavelengths of light but does not require the eyes, pineal gland, or other canonical deep-brain photoreceptive organs. Unlike the relatively slow effects of canonical nonvisual pathways, motor circuits are strongly and quickly (seconds) recruited during the PMR behavior. We find that the hindbrain is both necessary and sufficient to drive these behaviors. Using in vivo calcium imaging, we identify a discrete set of neurons within the hindbrain whose responses to light mirror the PMR behavior. Pharmacological inhibition of the visual cycle blocks PMR behaviors, suggesting that opsin-based photoreceptors control this behavior. These data represent the first known light-sensing circuit in the vertebrate hindbrain.
Introduction
How the nervous system senses and responds to light is a fundamental question in neuroscience.
Photobiology has traditionally focused on visual pathways (Ridge et al., 2003). However, nonvisual photo-sensation also plays an important role in animal physiology and behavior (Lucas et al., 1999; Berson et al., 2002; Hattar et al., 2003; Zaidi et al., 2007; Noseda et al., 2010). The majority of research on nonvisual photic behaviors has been dedicated to understanding circadian rhythms, which are controlled via hormones over relatively long time scales (hours to days) (Reppert and Weaver, 2001). Beyond circadian rhythms, nonvisual pathways can also control motor behaviors on a short time scale (seconds) (Becker and Cone, 1966; Harth and Heaton, 1973; Heaton and Harth, 1974; Peirson et al., 2009). For example, it has been recently shown that deep brain photoreceptors control light-seeking behaviors in zebrafish larvae (Fernandes et al., 2012). Such behaviors are a fundamental aspect of how the vertebrate nervous system responds to light, but remain poorly understood at the cellular and molecular levels.
The retina is the only known light-detecting organ in mammals. However, some birds and reptiles express specialized extraocular photoreceptors in various organs, including the pineal complex, deep brain, and skin (Yoshikawa et al., 1998; Vigh et al., 2002). A number of photopigments have been identified in these extraocular tissues, including pinopsin (Okano et al., 1994; Max et al., 1995), melanopsin (Provencio et al., 1998), parapinopsin (Blackshaw and Snyder, 1997), exo-rhodopsin (Mano et al., 1999), vertebrate-ancient opsin (Val-opsin) (Kojima et al., 2000), and neuropsin (Nakane et al., 2010). These opsins are thought to enable nonvisual photodetection (Vigh et al., 2002). However, their precise roles in physiology and behavior are poorly understood. Here, we have investigated the phenotypic, cellular, and molecular mechanisms of the zebrafish photomotor response (PMR) behavior using a combination of genetic, behavioral, electrophysiological, and calcium imaging techniques. We find an unexpected circuit in the zebrafish hindbrain that is required for nonvisual light-driven motor behaviors. These data implicate a new locus of photosensitive hindbrain neurons controlling nonvisual light detection and motor behaviors in vertebrates.
Materials and Methods
Fish maintenance and aquaculture
Zebrafish embryos were collected from group mating wild-type zebrafish (Ekkwill). Embryos of either sex were raised in HEPES (10 mm) buffered E3 media in a dark incubator at 28°C.
Behavioral recordings and data analysis
Grouped.
The PMR assay was performed as described previously (Kokel et al., 2010). Briefly, groups of 8–10 embryos were distributed into the wells of flat-bottom black 96-well plates. A total of 1000 frames of digital video were recorded at 30 fps. The motion index was calculated by frame differencing. “Excitation scores” are calculated by taking the 75th percentile of the motion index during indicated phases of the PMR behavior. Measurements and analysis were performed using the MetaMorph and Matlab software packages.
Individual.
To quantify individual zebrafish movements, we developed an image-processing pipeline with the following steps: Gaussian deblurring was used to reduce camera noise. Hough circle detection was used to identify a region of interest (ROI) around the chorion surrounding each embryo. The ROI for each animal is dynamically tracked through all video frames. Movement is quantified by frame differencing and normalized relative to the ROI intensity. Manual inspection of the movies revealed that high-magnitude, low-frequency peaks in the motion index correlated with coiling events in the movies, so these peaks were defined as coiling events by the algorithm. Similarly, low-magnitude high-frequency peaks correlated with swimming events, so these peaks were defined as swimming by the algorithm. Coiling events within swimming events were also detected. Videos were processed using the MATLAB software package and the open source MATLAB Toolbox Gait-CAD (Mikut, 2008). The total calculation time of the whole processing pipeline, including fish finding, tracking, and classification, does not exceed 1.5 min for each movie on a usual desktop computer. We also developed a stand-alone graphical user interface providing easy application, batch processing of the video data as well as automated report generation. The software is available from the authors upon request.
Statistical analysis.
We used one-way ANOVA and the Tukey HSD post hoc test to test for significant differences between groups, generate 95% confidence intervals, and identify groups with significantly different means. For groups with significant differences, we used the two-tailed t test to test the null hypothesis and calculate the p value. Statistical analyses were performed using the anova1, multcompare, and ttest2 functions provided by the MATLAB statistics toolbox. All error bars represent SDs, unless otherwise noted.
Morpholino gene knockdown
All morpholinos in this study were obtained from GeneTools. Morpholinos used for this study were as follows: valA (5′-TTTGT GAAGA CCTTT CTGAG TTTGC-3′), valB (5′-TATAT GACTA ACCTT TCTGA GCTTC-3′), valB2 (5′-GAGTG TTCGA TACCT ATTAA GCATA-3′), exorho (5′-CGGTGTTGTAGTGTGCTCACCGCCG-3′), opn4.1 (CTCTCCATGAAGAGTGATGGCTCAT), opn4b (CAGCCCTGTCCATACACAACACACA), and opn4xb (ACATCCTGAAAACACACACAGAGAA). Morpholino efficacy was verified by PCR identification of splicing defects in the targeted genes, except for the opn4.1 morpholino, which targets the opn4.1 ATG translation start site. The yolks of single-cell, stage-fertilized embryos were injected with 1 nl of the 0.5 mm morpholino solutions (5 ng per animal) or as noted.
Confocal analysis
Live imaging of embryonic zebrafish brain was performed as previously described (Lowery and Sive, 2005). Briefly, zebrafish embryos were embedded in 0.7% low melting agarose. The agarose enabled movements to be visualized in digital videos while preventing animals from swimming away. Images were captured on an inverted microscope (AxioObserver Z1; Zeiss) using the LSM700 scanning system (Zeiss, 488 nm laser lines) and a 20× objective (Zeiss). Embryos globally expressed a GFP transgene (Krovel and Olsen, 2002) to enable their visualization under the 488 nm laser stimulus illumination.
Photic stimuli
Light stimuli were generated with a 300 W xenon bulb housed in a Sutter Lambda LS illuminator and delivered to the well 10 and 20 s (or 13 and 23 s) after the start of each video. A cold mirror (reflectance between 300 and 700 nm) on the Sutter illuminator was used to block wavelengths outside of this range. Stimulus wavelengths were restricted using a quadband beamsplitter (Semrock; DA/FI/TR/Cy5-4X4M-B-ZHE) and four single band exciters (center wavelength/bandwidth): 387/11, 485/20, 560/25, and 650/13 (Semrock; Brightline FF01). Light intensity was measured using a PM100D power meter attached to a S120VC photodiode power sensor (Thorlabs). Intensity of the full strength white light stimulus on the embryos was 67 μW/mm2. The intensities of violet (387 nm) and red (650 nm) wavelengths were 3 μW/mm2. The intensities of blue (485 nm) and green (560 nm) wavelengths were 15 μW/mm2. High intensity violet (405 nm; 600 μW/mm2) and red (650 nm; 23 μW/mm2) stimuli were generated with pen style laser pointers (<5 mW), and their intensities were reduced using neutral density filters. For comparison, ambient light in the laboratory was ∼1 μW/mm2 and direct sunlight on rare sunny day in Boston was 667 μW/mm2.
Dissections and spinalized preparations
Dissections were performed under the same general conditions as described previously (Downes and Granato, 2006). To remove the eyes and pineal gland, the zebrafish hindbrain was severed between the anterior hindbrain ventricle and the otic vesicle, at approximately the level of rhombomere 3–4, completely removing all midbrain and forebrain structures, including the eyes and pineal complex (Asaoka et al., 2002). Data were collected 1–3 h after surgery; however, the decapitated preparations were surprisingly robust, responding to stimuli for >12 h after transection. Surgeries were performed under the microscope with forceps and a sharp razor blade after brief ice anesthesia. Dissected preparations were prepared and maintained in 1× Ringer's solution (116 mm NaCl, 2.9 mm KCl, 1.8 mm CaCl2, and 5 mm HEPES, pH 7.2).
Electrophysiology
We devised a 14 min recording protocol in which we recorded from a muscle cell in partially paralyzed embryos aged 32–36 h post fertilization (hpf). Lights were turned off at the beginning of the recording, and the responsiveness of the embryos to photo and tactile stimuli was assessed. Most of the embryos (11 of 15) also showed spontaneous fictive motor activity during the period of dark adaptation.
Myocyte recordings were obtained from embryos aged 32–36 h at room temperature using methods previously described (Hamill et al., 1981; Ribera and Nüsslein-Volhard, 1998; Drapeau et al., 1999). Embryos were anesthetized by adding Tricaine (0.02% w/v) to our Evans extracellular recording solution: 134 mm NaCl, 2.9 mm KCl, 2.1 mm CaCl2, 1.2 mm MgCl2, 10 mm glucose, and 10 mm HEPES, adjusted to pH 7.8 with NaOH. The embryos were pinned to a 35 mm Sylgard-coated dish, and the skin overlying several somites was removed. The solution was replaced by gravity perfusion (1–2 ml/s) of anesthetic-free Evans, which contained 6 μm d-tubocurarine to partially paralyze the preparation and enable recording from muscle cells under the whole-cell patch-clamp configuration. The internal recording solution contained the following: 116 mm K-gluconate, 16 mm KCl, 2 mm MgCl2, 10 mm HEPES, 10 mm EGTA, at pH 7.2 with KOH to which we added 0.1% SulforhodamineB for cell identification. Borosilicate glass electrodes had resistances of 4–6 MΩ when filled with internal recording solution. Recordings were made with an Axopatch 200B amplifier (Molecular Devices) low pass filtered at 2 kHz and sampled at 10 kHz. The microscope used was a Zeiss examiner A1, and fine pipette movement was controlled with a Sutter MPC-200 manipulator system. Tactile stimuli were delivered by ejecting solution from a pipette with a 25 μm opening where the pressure and duration were set via a picospritzer III (Parker). Photo stimuli were delivered using the built-in illumination from the microscope (100 W) where the duration was limited at 1 s using a uniblitz shutter (Optikon) triggered by a TTL pulse, this illumination was enhanced with a camera flash (Vivitar, 600 CR) occurring at the beginning of the 1 s TTL triggered time period. Data acquisition and TTL triggering of stimuli were achieved with pClamp 10 software using a Digidata 1440A interface. The initial data analysis was done with Clampfit 10, and figures were prepared using Adobe Illustrator.
Calcium imaging
A custom-built two-photon microscope (Denk et al., 1990) was used to monitor neuronal activity in the hindbrain of 32–38 hpf zebrafish. The embryos expressed the genetically encoded calcium indicator GCaMP2 under the pan-neuronal HuC promoter, and were homozygous nacre in a WIK wild-type background. Embryos were treated in phenylthiourea to remove pigmentation and were prescreened before imaging for robust responses to photic stimuli. Embryos were then dechorionated and paralyzed by bathing them in 1 mg/ml bungarotoxin (Invitrogen) solution and creating a small lesion at the tip of the tail with a forceps for better access to the internal tissues. Paralyzed embryos were side-mounted in 2% agarose and covered in standard E3 fish water in preparation for imaging. During imaging experiments, one second of blue light was delivered by an LED (Thorlabs) 3 times at 30 s intervals. This was repeated for the next imaging plane with a 12 min delay to allow the PMR to recover. Because of leakage of the blue light to the photomultiplier tube, the photomultiplier tube was switched off during blue light stimulation, so that measurement of GCaMP2 fluorescence commenced upon offset of the stimulus.
Fluorescence movies were analyzed with custom written Matlab software as described previously (Ahrens et al., 2012). First, a square ROI, half the size of a neuron, is swept over all locations of the imaging plane. At each location, a fluorescence time-series, averaged over the ROI, is extracted and converted to a statistic for the “peaky-ness” of the fluorescence signal at that point. If f(x,y,t) is the fluorescence of the pixel at x, y at time t, and fsqu(x,y,t) is the average fluorescence of the box centered at pixel x, y, and favg is the average fluorescence over space and time for the entire image sequence, then the statistic was defined to be as follows: where <…> denotes the average over t. This measure was chosen because it bears resemblance to the usual dF/F but contains an offset to counteract the undesired amplification of noise; the third power was chosen because it nonlinearly converts peaks in the fluorescence signal to larger values of the statistic. This measure (one of several tested) yielded spatial signal maps m(x,y), which tended to be at least as sensitive as sets of ROIs selected manually from observation of the raw and dF/F movies. These signal maps were thresholded, and points of interest were found at the approximate centers of the peaks of m(x,y) by first smoothing with a 2D Gaussian and then finding the local maxima of the smoothed map. These formed the centers of the final ROIs (see Fig. 5e). These ROIs were then checked manually for any obvious artifacts. For the representative slice shown in Figure 5b, automatically detected ROIs were hand segmented to match the shape of each neuron associated with a given ROI. The regions indicated by dotted red lines contain many detected ROIs that were associated with fiber tracts rather than individual somata. For constructing the standardized ROI spatial distribution map, automatically detected ROIs from 4 fish with complete image stacks through at least one half of the brain were assigned coordinates relative to the location of the first occipital nerve in rhombomere 8. In this 3D coordinate system, each ROI was assigned a rostrocaudal distance along the ventral surface of the brain relative to the nerve, a dorsoventral distance along the line normal to its position on the previously defined rostrocaudal axis, and a mediolateral distance from the lateral edge of the brain. Each ROI was then plotted onto an average intensity projection of a standard 36 hpf brain. The density metric used in Figure 5d was calculated by averaging ROI densities across all sampled imaging planes from three additional fish that were exposed to all three conditions. For each plane, ROI density was calculated by dividing the number of detected ROIs by the volume of labeled neurons in the plane (area of labeled neurons × 1 μm).
Results
The PMR is a stereotyped behavior of reproducible kinematic events
Recently, we discovered the PMR, a robust series of motor behaviors triggered by photic stimuli in embryonic zebrafish (Kokel et al., 2010). In the PMR behavior, dark-adapted zebrafish embryos (30 hpf) respond to a bright pulse of light with a striking series of stereotyped motor behaviors within the chorion (Fig. 1a). The PMR can be divided into different phases, including background, excitation, and inactive/refractory phases (Fig. 1a,b). Before the stimulus, individual animals move infrequently with spontaneous coiling movements (Fig. 1a,b). Then, the stimulus elicits vigorous motor excitation lasting for ∼5–7 s. After excitation, the animals enter an inactive/refractory phase, characterized by less than background levels of activity and unresponsiveness to a second pulse of light (Fig. 1a,b). Interestingly, it takes ∼10 min of dark readaptation between stimuli for animals to respond to a second pulse of light (Fig. 1b).
We find that individual animals undergoing the PMR exhibit at least two types of motor events (coiling and swimming), which can be discriminated based on visual inspection of behavioral recordings. Coiling describes large alternating contractions that are likely to be analogous to turning events in older animals. By contrast, swimming describes high-frequency contractions that serve to propel the animal in the forward direction. Using a custom image-processing algorithm to analyze the behavior of individual animals, we find that swimming and coiling events can be discriminated via the amplitude and duration of peaks in the motion. Coiling events correspond to large-magnitude, low-frequency peaks, whereas swimming events correspond to low-magnitude, high-frequency peaks (Fig. 1c).
Overall, we identified 1540 coiling events and 19,775 swimming events in an analysis of 425 animals during the PMR behavior. The distribution of these events shows that motor activity increases dramatically after stimulus presentation (Fig. 1d). After a median of 2.2 s after the start of the stimulus presentation, coiling frequency increases ∼10-fold, from 0.07 coils/s to 0.82 coils/s, for a median duration of 0.8 s. Swimming behaviors, which are only rarely observed before the stimulus in only 0.5% of animals, are observed in 38% of animals after the stimulus and last for a median duration of 3.3 s (Fig. 1e,f). Interestingly, the peak of swimming behaviors occurs 1.75 s after the peak of coiling behaviors, indicating that the PMR excitation phase can be subdivided into an early period of rapid coiling, followed by a later period of prolonged swimming, although coils are not always followed by swimming (Fig. 1e,f). By the end of this excitation phase, coiling behaviors decrease to 0.02 coils/s, ∼3-fold lower than background levels, and animals do not respond to a second pulse of light (Fig. 1b–d). These results indicate that the PMR behavior is a stereotyped series of reproducible coiling and swimming events followed by a long period of inactivity.
Motor circuits are strongly recruited during PMR excitation
To understand the mechanism underlying the series of coiling and swimming events that occur during the PMR, we devised a 14 min electrophysiological recording protocol in which we recorded responses from a muscle cell in partially paralyzed embryos aged 32–36 hpf. Lights were turned off at the beginning of the recording, and the responsiveness of the embryos to photo and tactile stimuli (tail or head) was assessed. Embryos were dark adapted for 10 min and then tested again with photo and tactile stimuli (Fig. 2). Consistent with our video analysis, we find two types of electrophysiological events during head touch, tail touch, and PMR assays (low-frequency, large amplitude events and high-frequency low amplitude events), consistent with fictive coiling and swimming, respectively (Fig. 2A). None of the embryos responded to the light stimuli before dark adaptation (n = 15), whereas they all responded to tactile stimulation. Most of the embryos showed spontaneous fictive motor excitation during the period of dark adaptation (11 of 15). The mean latency to tail and head tactile stimulus responses were significantly different from each other at 27 ± 2 ms (n = 18) and 38 ± 4 ms (n = 16), respectively (p < 0.01), whereas the mean latency of responses to photo stimulation in the same animals was a full two orders of magnitude greater than either head or tail latencies at 3988 ± 612 ms (n = 14, p < 0.001) (Fig. 2B). This massive increase in latency suggests that the recruitment of the motor system by light stimulation may be dependent on a very slow cellular and/or molecular mechanism that is different from the mechanisms controlling the touch response. The PMR included a high percentage of fictive coils (92%, n = 18), whereas spontaneous motor output consisted of 82% (n = 11) fictive coils; head touch elicited fictive coils in 72% (n = 18) of trials, and tail touch only 11% (n = 18) (Fig. 2D). The mean duration of fictive photomotor events at 4907 ± 337 ms was significantly longer than all other forms of motor activity with 675 ± 179, 818 ± 64, and 1301 ± 168 ms, respectively, for head, tail, and spontaneous motor events (p < 0.001; Fig. 2C). These long duration activity patterns, which consistently include struggling coils, suggest a very strong recruitment of the motor circuits during PMR excitation.
The PMR is a nonvisual motor behavior
The developmental time course of several motor behaviors in the zebrafish embryo have been well characterized. For example, spontaneous contractions develop at 17 h after fertilization (Saint-Amant and Drapeau, 1998). In dechorionated animals, the touch response develops at 21 hpf and touch-evoked swimming develops at 26 hpf (Saint-Amant and Drapeau, 1998). Zebrafish embryos hatch between 48 and 60 hpf and develop their first visual behaviors, including the optokinetic reflex and the visual startle response, between 68 and 79 hpf (Easter and Nicola, 1996, 1997).
To determine the time course of PMR development, we examined zebrafish behavior between 21 hpf and 50 hpf. We find that the magnitude of PMR excitation develops rapidly between 28 and 30 hpf: after the start of touch-evoked coiling and before the optokinetic reflex (Fig. 3a). The PMR excitation phase lasts for ∼10 h between 30 and 40 hpf. At 40 hpf, the magnitude of PMR excitation begins a gradual decline and eventually disappears by 50 hpf (Fig. 3a). Interestingly, we find evidence of photo-sensation even before 30 hpf. Although light does not trigger PMR excitation at 27 hpf, it does trigger the PMR inactive phase (Fig. 3a,b). For example, even in the absence of PMR excitation, light significantly reduces motor activity (Fig. 3b). These are the earliest reported behavioral responses to light in the zebrafish. Because zebrafish develop their first signs of vision at 73 hpf (Easter and Nicola, 1997), these data suggest that the PMR behavior is a nonvisual response to light.
The PMR develops >40 h before the first functional visual pathways (Brockerhoff et al., 1995; Schmitt and Dowling, 1999; Morris and Fadool, 2005). To determine whether the PMR requires visual photoreceptors, we analyzed the behavior of zebrafish preparations with and without their eyes at 30 hpf. Remarkably, blinded preparations, also lacking the pineal organs and other canonical deep brain photosensitive tissues, continued to exhibit robust PMR excitation for many hours after transection (Fig. 3c). Thus, the PMR is a nonvisual photic behavior.
Because the PMR does not require canonical photoreceptive organs, such as the eyes and pineal, we sought to determine where PMR photoreceptors are located anatomically. Dermal melanocytes are intrinsically photosensitive in some amphibians and fish (Wakamatsu et al., 1980). To determine whether melanocytes are necessary to trigger the PMR, we analyzed the behavior of casper mutant zebrafish that fail to develop all melanocytes and iridophores (White et al., 2008). We find that casper mutant embryos exhibit a robust PMR (data not shown), indicating that dermal melanophores are not necessary for the PMR.
Previous reports suggest that nonvisual photic behaviors in lampreys and snakes are triggered by cutaneous photoreceptors in the tail (Young, 1935; Zimmerman and Heatwole, 1990). To determine whether tail photoreceptors are necessary for the zebrafish PMR, we analyzed the PMR in animals lacking the caudal half of their bodies. Tailless animals exhibit a robust PMR, indicating that the tail is not necessary for PMR photo-sensation and that PMR photoreceptors are not located exclusively in the tail (Fig. 3c).
Supraspinal input from the brain is not necessary for spontaneous coiling or touch-evoked coiling behaviors in the zebrafish (Downes and Granato, 2006). To determine whether the hindbrain is required for the PMR, we analyzed the behavior of spinalized animals. We find that spinalized preparations lacking the hindbrain did not respond to photic stimuli (Fig. 3c). Thus, unlike the touch response, supraspinal input from the hindbrain is necessary for the PMR.
Because the hindbrain is necessary for the PMR, we wondered whether it might also be sufficient for sensing photic stimuli. To test this hypothesis, we used confocal microscopy to restrict photic stimulation to the column of cells above and below the eye, hindbrain, trunk, or tail. We find that motor activity is not elicited by photic stimuli restricted to the eye, trunk, or tail (Fig. 3d). By contrast, PMR excitation is strongly and reproducibly elicited by photic stimuli directed at the hindbrain (Fig. 3d). Together, these data suggest that hindbrain neurons are both necessary and sufficient to elicit PMR behaviors.
The PMR is a response to visible light
To determine the relationship between stimulus intensity and response magnitude, we exposed dark-adapted animals to white light stimuli (300–700 nm) from a xenon lamp at various intensities. Whereas 1 μW/mm2 is too dim to trigger PMR excitation, a 13 μW/mm2 stimulus elicits a significant increase in motor activity (p < 0.001; Fig. 4a). Thus, the minimum intensity needed to trigger PMR excitation is between 1 and 13 μW/mm2. Stimulus intensities ≥33 μW/mm2 trigger significantly more excitation than the minimum effective intensity (p < 0.005; Fig. 4a). These data indicate that PMR behaviors are triggered by stimuli ∼50× less intense than direct sunlight (667 μW/mm2). Furthermore, these data suggest that PMR behaviors could be triggered by natural stimuli outside the laboratory.
To determine which wavelengths of light are sufficient to trigger the PMR, we exposed animals to violet (387 nm), blue (480 nm), green (560 nm), and red (650 nm) stimuli. We find that blue and green wavelengths (both ∼15 μW/mm2) reproducibly trigger robust PMR excitation (Fig. 4b). By contrast, neither violet (405 nm) nor red (650 nm) stimuli (15 μW/mm2 and 23 μW/mm2 respectively) triggers PMR excitation (Fig. 4b). These data suggest that the PMR is triggered by blue and green light stimuli, but not by shorter or longer wavelengths.
To determine the duration of light necessary to trigger PMR excitation, we analyzed the behavior of animals exposed to blue light stimuli (480 nm, 15 μW/mm2) for various durations. We find that the response magnitudes are the same for stimuli lasting 1 or 20 s (Fig. 4c). Similarly, the maximum response duration is 5–7 s, for stimuli lasting either 1 or 20 s. A 5–7 s response duration is also observed for bright (67μW/mm2) white stimuli lasting 1 or 20 s (data not shown). These data indicate that PMR excitation has a finite magnitude and duration and does not continue under constant light (Fig. 4d). Together, these data suggest that the minimum stimulus duration for maximum effect is ∼1 s.
In larval zebrafish, visual phototaxis behavior is triggered by changes in the relative intensities of field and target stimuli (Burgess et al., 2010). To determine whether PMR behaviors are also triggered by relative changes in light intensity, we preadapted zebrafish embryos to low levels of ambient light (1 μW/mm2) for 10 min before PMR analysis. Next, we exposed these light-adapted animals to a large relative increase in brightness. Surprisingly, even very intense stimuli (67 μW/mm2) do not trigger PMR excitation in light-adapted animals (Fig. 4a). Thus, unlike visual phototaxis, the PMR depends on dark adaptation, rather than relative changes in light intensity.
A discrete set of hindbrain neurons drive PMR behaviors
To understand which neurons in the brain may be activated during the PMR, we measured neuronal activity in transgenic zebrafish expressing the genetically encoded fluorescent calcium indicator GCaMP2 (Diez-Garcia et al., 2007) under the pan-neuronal HuC promoter (Park et al., 2000). By monitoring the neuronal fluorescence changes elicited by photic stimuli with two-photon laser scanning microscopy, we were able to identify the specific location of neurons involved in the PMR (Fig. 5a). In 7 responsive 32–38 hpf fish, neurons in the caudal hindbrain, but not in the forebrain or midbrain, showed robust excitation to the photic stimulus, consistent with the hypothesis that supraspinal input is necessary to drive PMR behaviors. Figure 5b shows that, in one representative animal, 21 neurons and 2 distinct fiber tracts fire synchronously during the PMR, illustrating the common pattern of excitation in response to the photic stimulus (Fig. 5b). By pooling the normalized positions of identified ROIs across responsive fish, we show that active neurons are distributed throughout the caudal hindbrain (Fig. 5e), with the highest frequency of active neurons in Ro8, ∼45 μm caudal to the first occipital nerve (Fig. 5f, bottom, blue). ROIs also cluster with greatest frequency 35 μm from the ventral surface on the brain (Fig. 5f, top right, orange) and 10 μm from the lateral surface of the brain (Fig. 5f, top left, cyan). Some PMR-correlated neuronal activity was also observed in the anterior spinal cord (Fig. 5e, hyphenated line). All observed neuronal responses are likely to correspond to the PMR because activity is consistently locked to stimulus onset with a fixed latency and can only be observed after the first stimulus after a long refractory period. Across all identified ROIs, the median time to fluorescence onset was 2.5 s, and the median duration of the calcium response was 6 s (Fig. 5c), in close agreement with the latencies and durations of the coiling and swimming events that occur during PMR excitation (Figs. 1 and 2). When hindbrain activity is compared with forebrain activity during stimulus trials, a pronounced difference in both the density of identified ROIs and the amplitude of detected responses emerges (Fig. 5d). These data support the conclusion that the PMR stimulus specifically recruits neurons in the hindbrain. Furthermore, the density of responsive ROIs in the hindbrain during stimulus trials is significantly different from the density during no stimulus trials. These data reiterate that hindbrain responses during stimulus presentation are stimulus-evoked and not artifacts of the experimental preparation (e.g., two-photon scanning). The few hindbrain responses detected during no stimulus trials most likely reflect baseline spontaneous activity in hindbrain circuits. Because of limitations of the calcium sensor, it remains possible that we are underestimating the number and distribution of responsive neurons. Nonetheless, the anatomical clustering of identified ROIs, coupled with the similarity of neuronal response and behavioral response parameters, suggests that a discrete set of hindbrain neurons drive PMR motor activity.
Opsins are necessary for the PMR
Vertebrate photosensation requires the opsins, a large family of retinal-based G-protein-coupled receptors (Peirson et al., 2009). To determine whether neurons in the zebrafish hindbrain sense photic stimuli via opsin-based phototransduction, we analyzed the effects on opsin impairment on PMR behavior. The zebrafish genome encodes >10 extra-retinal opsins with different expression patterns and functions, including at least 5 melanopsin isoforms (Matos-Cruz et al., 2011). We chose 3 candidate opsin genes for further analysis, including exo-rhodopsin, valopsinA, and valopsinB (Kojima et al., 2008). To determine whether these candidate opsins are necessary for the PMR, we used antisense morpholinos to knock-down their expression. We find that PMR excitation is not reduced in the morphant animals, suggesting that these opsins are not necessary for PMR excitation (Fig. 6a).
The visual retinoid cycle depends on 11-cis-retinal. To determine whether the visual cycle is necessary for the PMR, we treated animals with the small-molecule Ret-NH2, which inhibits synthesis of 11-cis-retinal in the zebrafish (Schonthaler et al., 2007). We find that Ret-NH2-treated animals fail to exhibit PMR behaviors, including the PMR excitation and inactive phases (Fig. 6b–d). Importantly, Ret-NH2-treated animals appear otherwise normal, showing normal background motion and touch response. Melanopsin-based photoreception is independent of the visual retinoid cycle (Tu et al., 2006), suggesting that melanopsin is not necessary for PMR excitation. Because PMR excitation appears to depend on the visual cycle, these data suggest that the PMR is mediated by one or more opsins in the nervous system.
Discussion
Here, we have described the molecular, cellular, and behavioral mechanisms of the zebrafish PMR. The PMR behavior is the first photic behavior linked to light-sensitive neurons in the vertebrate hindbrain and the earliest known photic behavior to develop in the zebrafish. At least three attributes distinguish the PMR from other previously reported behaviors in the zebrafish. First, the PMR latency and refractory phases are orders of magnitude longer than those observed for other stimuli. For example, the previously reported latency and refractory periods for responses to acoustic, tactile, and photic stimuli are milliseconds in duration (Saint-Amant and Drapeau, 1998; Burgess and Granato, 2007a, b; Best et al., 2008), whereas the PMR latency and refractory phases last for seconds and minutes, respectively. Second, the neuroanatomical requirements of the PMR are distinct from other behaviors that occur at this time in development. For example, supraspinal input is necessary for the PMR, unlike spontaneous coiling, touch-evoked coiling, and touch-evoked swimming (Downes and Granato, 2006). Finally, the PMR depends on nonocular photoreceptors, unlike other photic behaviors, such as the light adaptation response (Burgess and Granato, 2007a), optokinetic response (Rick et al., 2000), and the visual-motor response (Emran et al., 2007).
It appears that the early zebrafish embryo has a limited repertoire of motor responses consisting of coils and swims and that these motor responses can be differentially recruited by specific upstream circuits (Saint-Amant and Drapeau, 1998, 2000; Brustein et al., 2003). Whereas touch stimuli elicit brief coiling and swimming events, the PMR evokes coils and swims with long latency and long duration. Because hindbrain neurons link sensory input to motor output, they provide an excellent starting point for studying the underlying circuitry of this complex series of behaviors.
Most behavioral reflexes in the zebrafish have very short refractory periods (milliseconds). By contrast, the minimum interstimulus interval for maximum PMR excitation is several orders of magnitude longer (10 min). One possible explanation for this long refractory phase is opsin photobleaching. Most opsins use cis-to-trans isomerization of the retinaldehyde chromophore as the primary photoreceptive event and must regenerate the cis-chromophore to regain photosensitivity. Recovery from photobleaching occurs over a similar time scale as the PMR refractory phase (10 min) (Sexton et al., 2012), suggesting that photobleaching may explain the 10 min interstimulus interval between PMR excitation phases.
Different mechanisms may control the PMR excitation and inactive/refractory phases. We show that light stimuli inhibit spontaneous coiling activity at 27 hpf without eliciting PMR excitation. At 27 hpf, spontaneous coils are generated and propagated by an extensive network of neurons interconnected in the spinal cord by gap junctions (Saint-Amant and Drapeau, 2001). Spontaneous coils are independent of chemical synaptic transmission and are dependent on electrical coupling of the neuronal network within the spinal cord (Saint-Amant and Drapeau, 2001). At 27 hpf, inhibitory signals from the PMR cells may be sufficient to block spontaneous coiling. By 32 hpf, the nervous system may develop additional synaptic inputs that drive the PMR excitation phase. One possibility is that each phase may be controlled by independent photoreceptors.
Opsin-based phototransduction is based on photoisomerization of the chromophore 11-cis-retinal to all-trans-retinal. Chromophore regeneration, via the retinoid cycle, is necessary to restore photoreceptors to their dark-adapted state. The small-molecule Ret-NH2 is a potent and selective inhibitor of chromophore regeneration (Golczak et al., 2005) that reduces levels of 11-cis-retinal in wild-type zebrafish (Schonthaler et al., 2007). We find that Ret-NH2 blocks PMR behaviors, suggesting that opsin-based phototransduction is necessary for these nonvisual photic responses. However, exactly which opsin or opsins are necessary for PMR behaviors remains unclear. Morpholino knockdown of six candidate genes did not block the PMR, suggesting that an untested opsin or multiple redundant opsins may control PMR behaviors.
What adaptive purpose might the PMR serve for fish in the wild? Similar nonvisual photic behaviors have been previously described in lampreys (Young, 1935), C. elegans (Edwards et al., 2008; Liu et al., 2010), and Drosophila (Xiang et al., 2010). In these animals, nonvisual light-induced motor behaviors appear to function as an escape response to keep animals hidden from dangerous UV radiation or predators (Xiang et al., 2010). The PMR is likely to serve a different purpose in zebrafish because it only occurs before hatching, when the chorion prevents animals from swimming away. One possibility is that the PMR is a consequence of irradiance detection mechanisms with alternative primary functions. For example, it may be useful for animals to modulate neuronal excitability based on environmental light levels. Alternatively, the PMR may be an evolutionarily vestige with important ancestral functions in other organisms. Overall, the PMR presents new opportunities to understand the development and function of vertebrate nonvisual photobiology and may improve our understanding of how the nervous system responds to light.
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants K01MH091449 to D. Kokel, R01MH086867 and R21MH085205 to R.T.P., and DP1 NS082121 and R01DA030304 to T.W.D. and F.E. This work was supported in part by Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research from Japan Society for the Promotion of Science to Y.F. and D. Kojima, T.W.D. was supported by the National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship. M.B.A. was supported by a Sir Henry Wellcome Postdoctoral Fellowship from the Wellcome Trust. R.T.P. was supported by the Charles and Ann Sanders MGH Research Scholar award. We thank John Dowling and members of our research groups for encouragement and advice.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to either Dr. David Kokel or Dr. Randall T. Peterson, Cardiovascular Research Center and Division of Cardiology, Department of Medicine, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, 149 13th Street, Charlestown, MA 02129, dkokel{at}cvrc.mgh.harvard.edu or peterson{at}cvrc.mgh.harvard.edu