Abstract
Oscillatory interference models account for the spatial firing properties of grid cells in terms of neuronal oscillators with frequencies modulated by the animal's movement velocity. The phase of such a “velocity-controlled oscillator” (VCO) relative to a baseline (theta-band) oscillation tracks displacement along a preferred direction. Input from multiple VCOs with appropriate preferred directions causes a grid cell's grid-like firing pattern. However, accumulating phase noise causes the firing pattern to drift and become corrupted. Here we show how multiple redundant VCOs can automatically compensate for phase noise. By entraining the baseline frequency to the mean VCO frequency, VCO phases remain consistent, ensuring a coherent grid pattern and reducing its spatial drift. We show how the spatial stability of grid firing depends on the variability in VCO phases, e.g., a phase SD of 3 ms per 125 ms cycle results in stable grids for 1 min. Finally, coupling N VCOs with similar preferred directions as a ring attractor, so that their relative phases remain constant, produces grid cells with consistently offset grids, and reduces VCO phase variability of the order square root of N. The results suggest a viable functional organization of the grid cell network, and highlight the benefit of integrating displacement along multiple redundant directions for the purpose of path integration.
Introduction
Grid cells recorded in the medial entorhinal cortex (MEC) of freely moving rodents fire whenever the animal occupies any one of an array of locations arranged in a regular triangular grid across the environment (Hafting et al., 2005). These cells are thought to support “path integration,” providing a representation of location updated on the basis of movement-related information (O'Keefe and Burgess, 2005; Fuhs and Touretzky, 2006; McNaughton et al., 2006). The oscillatory interference model (Burgess et al., 2005, 2007) explains grid cell firing patterns in terms of input from neuronal oscillators whose frequency varies as a function of the animal's movement velocity (“velocity-controlled oscillators” or VCOs; Blair et al., 2008; Burgess, 2008; Hasselmo, 2008).
Predictions for the theta-band modulation of grid cell firing rates as a function of running speed and grid scale (Burgess, 2008) have been verified (Jeewajee et al., 2008), stellate cells in MEC exhibit membrane potential resonance at frequencies that vary consistently with changes in grid spacing along the dorsoventral axis of MEC (Giocomo et al., 2007), and septohippocampal “theta cells” appear to function as the predicted VCOs (Welday et al., 2011). However, a key question concerns whether the level of noise in neuronal oscillations is too high to allow the spatial stability observed in grid fields (Zilli et al., 2009; Remme et al., 2010). The observation of theta-phase precession in grid cells (Hafting et al., 2008; Reifenstein et al., 2012) implies that grid cell populations do maintain reliable spatially demarcated phase relationships in vivo. These consistent phase relationships highlight the fact that the interfering oscillations are not independent. For example, the baseline (local field potential) oscillation against which individual neuronal firing precesses may be the mean of the population activity (Burgess et al., 1993; Geisler et al., 2010), and oscillatory activity may be stabilized relative to the environment by location-specific phase-resetting inputs (Burgess et al., 2005, 2007; O'Keefe and Burgess, 2005; Monaco et al., 2011; Sreenivasan and Fiete, 2011; Bush and Burgess, 2014). Zilli and Hasselmo (2010) showed how synaptically or electrically coupling VCOs with the same preferred direction reduces their overall frequency variability. Here we show how coupling multiple VCOs carrying information about displacement along different directions (via entrainment of the baseline oscillation), or with different spatial offsets (via ring-attractor dynamics; Blair et al., 2008), could produce the phase reliability and consistency needed to generate stable and coherent grid-like firing patterns.
Materials and Methods
We use the neuronal-VCO grid cell model described previously (Burgess, 2008; see Results for an overview). VCOs are simulated as neurons with a sinusoidal membrane potential oscillation (MPO) with velocity-dependent frequency described by Equation 15. Three VCOs are used, with preferred directions spaced by 120°. This configuration satisfies the requirement of 60° multiple spacing for producing hexagonal grids, and enables a straightforward mechanism for maintaining VCO consistency (see Results). Each VCO spikes periodically when its phase is zero if the animal is traveling within 90° of its preferred direction (Fig. 1A). The grid cell's VCO inputs are modeled as exponentially decaying EPSPs with a time constant of 25 ms (Fig. 1C; Garden et al., 2008).
The zero-speed VCO frequency f0 was 8 Hz, with velocity-dependent modulation scaled by β = 0.026 cm−1 (which is in the range of typical theta frequency modulation reported in Jeewajee et al., 2008). Phase noise is Normally distributed (SD 0.006 rad) and added to each VCO phase ϕi every 1 ms time step, producing a minimum phase SD of 0.067 rad (i.e., 1.34 ms per 125 ms cycle of an 8 Hz oscillator). The baseline oscillation frequency fb increases from f0 proportional to running speed (Eq. 18) and equals the mean frequency of the VCOs (see Results).
Location coding by multiple VCOs
Following Equation 16, the ith VCO's phase ϕi relative to the baseline phase ϕb encodes distance traveled (di) along its preferred direction φi. If distance is represented independently along three or more directions (i.e., with three or more VCOs), the 2D location represented can be inconsistent (i.e., each pair can represent a different location; see Results; Fig. 2). Therefore, when analyzing the spatial drift of a grid due to phase noise, we assume an ongoing mechanism for realigning all phases to indicate the mean location represented by each pair. With a frequent (or continuous) realignment, such that accumulated phase noise between realignments is small (compared with their period), this is a linear transformation. Thus the mapping from phase noise offsets to mean location offset is continuous, i.e., each small phase perturbation produces a small perturbation in grid cell firing locations after realignment. The linear transformation for this realignment is described here, and an example mechanism implementing this is detailed below and in Results.
We define the state of the animal at time t as a vector v(t) comprising its location coordinates vxy(t), and the phase of the baseline oscillation ϕb(t). Then (from Eq. 16) the phases of all the oscillations obey the following: where the first n components of ϕ(t) are the VCO phases and the last component is the baseline phase, e(t) is the phase noise, and A is the (n + 1) by 3 transformation matrix: Considering the VCO phases relative to baseline (i.e., with the baseline phase subtracted), A can be viewed as projecting the location of the animal onto a plane P in the n dimensional space of VCO phases relative to baseline. The points on P correspond exactly to the relative phases in which the location represented by each VCO pair is consistent, and consequently will be inconsistent if phase drift (due to noise) takes the relative phases out of P. The closest consistent point in phase space can be found by projecting onto P along its normal vector. The location corresponding to this point v̂(t) can be found by applying a linear transformation B, such that: which is the Moore–Penrose pseudo-inverse of A. If the phase noise in each oscillation has zero mean, applying B to noisy phases is equivalent to finding the location estimate that minimizes the mean squared distance from the correct (noise-free) location. The consistent set of phases ϕ′(t), corresponding to this location, can be found by applying AB, i.e., ϕ′(t) = ABϕ(t).
Since VCOs encode distance relative to the baseline oscillation, common noise across all oscillations can be ignored. We assume that each oscillation has independent, identically distributed Gaussian noise. Therefore, the covariance matrix of ϕ is given by Σϕ = σϕ2I, where σϕ2 is the phase variance of each oscillation and I is the identity matrix of order n + 1. Consequently, v̂ is Normally distributed with covariance matrix: where the first 2-by-2 element submatrix in Σv is the covariance matrix of v̂xy.
We define the point of unrecoverable error as when 50% of the distribution of v̂xy (which is contained within Mahalanobis distance r ≈ 1.1774, see the cumulative χ2(2) distribution of interval r2) has drifted into adjacent fields (Fig. 4A).
Coupling phase-offset VCOs as ring attractors
To examine noise reduction from coupling phase-offset VCOs, we simulated an oscillating ring-attractor network of N cells (following Blair et al., 2008; for review, see Zhang, 1996). We used a 1D adaption of the 2D continuous attractor model (Burak and Fiete, 2009) with asymmetric recurrent connections making the activity bump rotate around the ring, and rate-coded neurons obeying: where υi is neuron i's synaptic activation, f(x) = x for x > 0, 0 otherwise, and ξt is the noise added at time t. Neurons have positions xi evenly spaced around the ring (xi, increasing clockwise). There are two classes of neurons: clockwise or anticlockwise, in alternating positions around the ring. The recurrent connection to cell i from cell j has weight: where [ ]N indicates modulo N. W0 is a function of the circular distance between the cells δ, shifted by kjl and ω. For clockwise cells ki = 1, for anticlockwise ki = −1, so that the distribution of weights is shifted clockwise or anticlockwise, ω biases the otherwise symmetric distribution of connections so that the bump of activity propagates around the ring at a fixed frequency (see below). The weighting function is a difference of Gaussians: Recurrent inputs are inhibitory, creating a stable bump of activity due to local disinhibition and distal inhibition (i.e., β < λ). The feedforward input to neuron i is as follows: providing differential excitation to clockwise versus anticlockwise cells (due to ki), which modulates the speed of propagation of the activity bump according to running velocity (m(t) = βs(t)cos(θ(t) − φd); see Eq. 15). Parameter values were as follows: a = 25, β = 1/(0.88N)2, λ = 1.05β, l = 0.075N, ω = 0.075N, τ = 10 ms. ξt was drawn from the Normal distribution with zero mean, and SD σξ, at each 0.5 ms time step. Values of the membrane noise level σξ used were 0.025, 0.05, and 0.1. The activity bump had oscillation frequency ∼8 Hz when running speed was zero.
Entraining a baseline oscillator to the mean VCO frequency
To demonstrate that activity in a baseline ring-attractor network could oscillate at a frequency entrained to the mean frequency of three VCO ring attractors, we developed a spiking network whose activity bump can be entrained to rotate at a frequency defined by its inputs. We use the Neural Engineering Framework (NEF; Eliasmith and Anderson, 2003) to derive feedback connection weights facilitating attractor dynamics for a relatively narrow Gaussian bump of activity that naturally oscillates around the ring. The NEF defines neural representations in terms of nonlinear encoding of the variable to be represented (in this case phase) by populations of neurons with tuning curve responses, and weighted linear decoding of their activities to retrieve the represented value.
We assume that neurons in the baseline ring attractor have Gaussian tuning curves in the phase value x represented by their location on the ring, such that the population is able to represent a rotating Gaussian of the form f(x) = exp(− (x − ϕ(t))2/2σ2) with width σ, where ϕ(t) is the phase of the ring oscillator at time t (i.e., the phase indicated by the position of the bump of activity on the ring). Consider estimating the Gaussian activity profile in Fourier space by the vector γ of Fourier coefficients (where γ1,n and γ2,n are the nth frequency cosine and sine coefficients, respectively), using the components F0–F6 (with σ small enough to make F3 significant, as this harmonic will be the target of entrainment by the VCO inputs). The oscillation of the bump around the ring, and the effect of external inputs, can be expressed in terms of the dynamics of these coefficients (Orchard et al., 2013). Noting that rotation of the activity bump corresponds to rotation of the Fourier coefficients, these dynamics are given by the following: where Rω is a 14-by-14 matrix composed of seven 2-by-2 rotation submatrices along the diagonal, each corresponding to coefficient pair n and with rotation angle 2πnf0 (where f0 is the zero-speed VCO frequency, see above). The vector u(t) is the frequency domain representation of the external input (i.e., its first seven sine and cosine Fourier coefficients).
Equation 9 can be implemented in a neural population by applying the NEF (Eliasmith and Anderson, 2003; Conklin and Eliasmith, 2005 for derivation of a similar network). Using linear integrate and fire neurons whose subthreshold voltage dynamics are given by the following:
where τiRC is the membrane time constant, δ(t) is the delta function, and tin is the time of the nth spike of neuron i. The cell spikes when Vi > 1 and subsequently its voltage is held at zero for a refractory period, τref. Each cell i has scalar parameters αi and Jibias, the input gain and bias, respectively. The convolution (*) of each input spike with an exponential decay filter, h(t), with time constant τ, captures postsynaptic current dynamics. The feedback weights from neuron j to neuron i required to implement Equation 9 are given by the following:
where Δt is the simulation time step. The encoding vector for neuron i, φ̃i, specifies its tuning for coefficients in the frequency domain, i.e., the first 14 Fourier coefficients approximating a Gaussian that defines its tuning curve. The vectors φi are optimal linear decoders, such that the population estimate of γ is as follows:
These decoders are found by minimizing the mean square error of the estimate,
In our simulations of this network, 200 neurons were used with σ = 0.45rad, τiRC = 20 ms, τref = 2 ms, τ = 5 ms, and Δt = 0.5 ms. Parameters αi and Jibias were chosen to give each cell a random peak firing rate between 100 and 200 Hz and a random tuning width (both uniformly distributed), to reflect heterogeneity in real neuronal populations (for review, see Eliasmith and Anderson, 2003). The recurrent connectivity of the network is determined as described above. After an initial input to start it spiking, this network settles down to an approximately Gaussian bump of activity that oscillates around the ring at f0.
The activity profile of the external inputs to the ring is estimated by its Fourier components u(t). Each frequency component of the external input can be used to influence the corresponding component of the activity profile of the baseline network. Thus, to entrain the F3 component of the network, the two coefficients, u1,3 and u2,3, should be the cosine and sine amplitudes of the desired control oscillation, with all other components of u set to zero. The desired control oscillation should have a frequency that is three times the mean frequency of the three VCOs, i.e., its phase should be the sum of the phases of the three VCO rings (see Results). The cosine and sine Fourier components of this oscillation are simply derived from consideration of the real and imaginary components of the complex representation of the phases of each VCO ring (the product of these representations represents the desired oscillation: summing their phases), giving: where v1i and v2i are, respectively, the cosine and sine amplitudes of the ith VCO phase. The VCO amplitudes v1i and v2i could readily be decoded from VCO ring networks as inputs to the baseline network with appropriate synaptic weights, similarly as outlined above. Computation of the products of these inputs as in Equation 14 could be provided by multiplicative synapses onto neurons in the entrained network (e.g., the Sigma-pi units of Mel, 1993), or by directly (linearly) decoding the transformation in Equation 14 from a single attractor network representing the phases of all three VCO directions (e.g., by applying the NEF).
Results
The spatial firing pattern of grid cells is produced as an interference pattern between a set of VCOs with different directional tuning (Fig. 1; see Materials and Methods; Burgess, 2008). Each VCO has a preferred direction φd and an “active” frequency fa. This frequency varies according to the animal's running speed s(t) and direction θ(t), relative to a common “baseline” frequency fb(t): The displacement of the animal over a period of time is simply the integral of its velocity during that period, while the phase of an oscillation is the integral of its frequency. Accordingly, the phase of an oscillation whose frequency varies according to velocity along some direction will be proportional to displacement along that direction. Specifically following Equation 15, VCO phase ϕa relative to baseline phase ϕb encodes the animal's displacement along the VCO's preferred direction: where h(t) and ψ(t) are the net displacement distance and angle from the origin, respectively (Fig. 1A, VCOs). Position can be tracked in 2D using two or more VCOs with differing preferred directions (Figs. 1, 2). Furthermore, the interference envelope between such oscillators will generate a periodic pattern across 2D space. With a particular spacing between their preferred directions (60° or noncollinear multiples thereof), this pattern has the same tessellated hexagonal structure as grid cell firing fields (Burgess et al., 2005, 2007; Fig. 1). The development of this structure, from periodic representations of displacement along specific directions, may arise from Hebbian-like unsupervised learning, which favors coincident activity from representations separated by 60° (Burgess et al., 2007; Mhatre et al., 2012), and which may also explain the presence of less regular patterns formed from superposition of these representations (Krupic et al., 2012).
VCOs are neuronal oscillations with frequencies modulated by velocity-dependent synaptic inputs. They were proposed as MPOs occurring in individual neurons that synapse onto the grid cell, or within dendritic subunits of the grid cell itself (Burgess et al., 2005, 2007); see Burgess (2008), Hasselmo (2008), and Welday et al. (2011) for the former implementation, and Remme et al. (2010) for limitations of the latter. Here, we assume that the VCOs and baseline oscillations are external to the grid cell, providing synaptic inputs. The VCOs spike periodically at a particular phase of their oscillation, and might correspond to the velocity-controlled “theta cells” found along the septohippocampal circuit (Welday et al., 2011). Unlike the VCOs, the baseline oscillation does not depend on running direction and is consistent with the “somatic input” described previously (Burgess et al., 2007). See Burgess (2008) for discussion of the relationship of the baseline oscillation to the local field potential.
A grid cell receives inputs from multiple VCOs with preferred directions spaced at multiples of 60° and fires when input spikes arrive at similar phases (Fig. 1; Burgess, 2008; Hasselmo, 2008; i.e., the grid cell performs coincidence detection). Thus a grid cell fires at a grid node because the VCO phases align there, and will fire again when the VCO phases have all changed by a similar amount (modulo 2π), i.e., when the animal has moved similar distances along the VCOs' respective preferred directions (Fig. 2). This arrangement effectively modulates the grid cell's firing rate according to the interference pattern between the VCOs, thus generating their characteristic spatial firing pattern.
We follow Burgess (2008) in which (1) the velocity-dependent inputs to VCOs can only increase their frequency from some minimum, f0; (2) the baseline oscillation takes the mean frequency of the local VCOs; and (3) a VCO only contributes to grid cell firing when the animal is running within 90° of its preferred direction. The first two constraints imply: (without affecting Eq. 15), while the final constraint allows grid cells to show theta-phase precession relative to the baseline (which is identified with the local field potential), as observed in MEC layer II (Hafting et al., 2008).
Phase noise and spatial coherence of grid cell firing
Since VCO phases encode the animal's distance from grid nodes along their preferred directions, spatially coherent grid cell firing requires that VCO phases continue to coincide at the nodes of the grid. In the presence of noise, this requires an ongoing phase-correction mechanism. One solution is for feedback from place cells, driven by location-specific environmental information, to reset the VCO phases for a particular grid cell at a grid node (Burgess et al., 2005, 2007; Monaco et al., 2011). Here we consider local mechanisms for maintaining the phase consistency of VCOs.
If VCO phases are aligned with the baseline oscillation at the origin, then the relative phase of the ith VCO encodes displacement along its preferred direction φi with distance di(t) given by the following (from Eq. 16): Two (noncollinear) VCOs encode an unambiguous 2D location at the intersection of orthogonal lines from the endpoints of their displacement vectors (Fig. 2A). However, the orthogonals corresponding to any additional VCOs must intersect the same point to provide a consistent representation (Fig. 2B). Any errors in their phases will cause each pair to intersect at different points (Fig. 2C). Such inconsistencies will change their interference pattern, producing smaller sub-peaks in firing around each (error-free) grid node of the downstream grid cell. Therefore, variable inconsistency will lead to unstable and unpredictable firing fields (Fig. 3, top row; Burgess et al. 2005, 2007; Giocomo and Zilli, 2007).
If three VCOs with 120° preferred direction spacing together represent a consistent location, their encoded distances will sum to zero, i.e., d1 + d2 + d3 = 0, and (Eq. 16) their relative phases will too, modulo 2π. This can be seen by applying any three 120° spaced preferred directions to Equation 20, e.g., the preferred directions φ1 = 0°, φ2 = 120° and φ3 = 240° shown in Figure 2B. An error e in one of the VCO phases can abolish this zero sum property, causing the three orthogonals to intersect at the vertices of an equilateral triangle with altitude e/2πβ (Fig. 2C). If the source of error is unknown, the triangle's center best estimates location (Fig. 2D). Subtracting e/3 from all VCO phases will make their orthogonals coincide at this point, restoring consistency (and their zero sum). The situation for independent errors in all three VCOs is similar: the orthogonals intersect at the vertices of an equilateral triangle whose center best estimates location, and subtracting one-third of the total phase error from each VCO will realign them there.
Since VCO phases encode distances relative to baseline, the above error correction (specifically, subtracting one-third of the total phase error from each VCO) will happen automatically if the baseline oscillation is coupled to the VCOs to include their mean phase noise, i.e., if the baseline frequency takes the mean of the VCO frequencies (Fig. 3, bottom row). This arrangement was originally proposed in Burgess (2008), to enable the model to function with only positive VCO frequency modulation, rather than for any contribution to noise reduction. Following this, the baseline phase at time t is as follows: where ei(t) is the ith VCO's phase error. This maintains the zero sum of relative phases (and therefore their consistency) despite the presence of noise: Tracking the mean VCO frequency could be implemented by entrainment of the baseline oscillation by the VCOs. A network mechanism to implement this is described below.
The ability to bring VCO phases into consistency by a common phase shift (e.g., by altering the baseline phase) is unique to the 3 × 120° configuration. This can be seen by inspecting Figure 2D: any additional VCOs (in a multiple of 60° configuration) would be oriented in the opposite direction to one of the 120° triples. It would thus require a correctional phase shift of the same magnitude but opposite sign, leading to a nonzero sum of phases. Furthermore, arrangements of this configuration can have a zero sum of phases with an inconsistent representation. For example, adding equal but opposite offsets to the phases of oppositely oriented VCOs makes them inconsistent with the remaining VCOs, but leaves the sum of phases unchanged. Therefore, no common baseline shift can by itself make six 60° spaced VCOs consistent. However, the principle can be extended to this configuration by combining the baseline shift (as above) with a subtraction of the mean phase between each opposing VCO from their respective phases.
For any symmetrical configuration of VCO directions, the single best estimate to use for maintaining coherent grid firing is the average location (i.e., the mean vector) indicated by all unique pairwise combinations of VCOs. Realigning every VCO to indicate this location is equivalent to setting their position in relative phase space to the nearest point on the plane of consistent phases. We discuss this further in Materials and Methods, where we derive the linear transformation for computing this estimate from the phases of an arbitrary configuration of VCOs. Note that with any configuration, noise common to all VCOs and the baseline will not affect the represented location.
Phase noise and locational drift of grid cell firing
A mechanism for maintaining VCO phase consistency (e.g., following the principle described above) will ensure coherent grid cell firing. However, accumulating phase noise in any number of VCOs will still cause grid fields to drift (Fig. 3). We now characterize the drift rate for different configurations of VCOs: two VCOs with 60° spaced preferred directions, three 120° VCOs, and six 60° VCOs. In the latter two cases assuming that VCO phases are continuously aligned to the vector mean of the locations indicated by all pairwise combinations of VCOs to maintain coherent grid firing, as discussed above. The distribution of represented locations can be calculated from the distributions of oscillation phases (see Materials and Methods). Two main factors determine its spread: the number of VCOs and the configuration of preferred directions (Fig. 4A). Increasing numbers of VCOs result in more independent estimates of location to average over, tightening the distribution of estimated location. Increasing from two VCOs with 60° spaced preferred directions to three 120° VCOs decreases the area containing 50% of the distribution by ∼66%, and increasing from two to six 60° VCOs decreases it by ∼83%.
We also calculated the relationship between VCO phase variance and the duration of spatially stable firing (Fig. 4B; see Materials and Methods for derivation). Using three 120° VCOs extends the stable duration ∼200% compared with two 60° VCOs, and increasing from three to six extends this by a further ∼100%.
Coupling VCOs with offset phases
Finally, we considered the noise-reducing effects of coupling the VCOs required to drive a population of grid cells with different spatial phases. VCOs with the same preferred directions can be configured as a ring-attractor network (Blair et al., 2008), in which a localized bump of activity oscillates around the ring at theta frequency. The bump should oscillate faster when driven by velocity-dependent inputs according to Equation 15. Such a network enforces the phase offsets between VCOs and, given a common baseline oscillation as described above, would maintain consistent spatial offsets between grids. The maintenance of fixed phase differences between each VCO in a ring means that the activity bump of each ring effectively encodes a single oscillation (with a single phase error).
We simulated such a ring-attractor network of N cells, with noisy synaptic inputs, to examine phase stability. Networks were simulated with three different levels of noise, and with N ranging from 20 to 200 (see Materials and Methods). We found that the phase variability of the network decreased with increasing N, with SD approximately of the order
A population of grid cells can be driven by two or more ring-attractor networks containing phase-offset VCOs: one ring for each preferred direction (Blair et al., 2008). Grid cells with grid-like firing patterns of different spatial offsets can be generated by combining inputs from VCOs in each ring with different phase offsets. Furthermore, the baseline oscillation modulating a grid cell's firing can also be implemented as a ring-attractor network. This ring could be entrained by the VCO rings to follow their mean frequency. We developed a spiking network model to demonstrate this entrainment using the NEF (Eliasmith and Anderson, 2003), see Materials and Methods and below.
Entrainment of oscillator networks
To demonstrate the feasibility of the error correction scheme outlined above, we demonstrate that the frequency of a baseline ring attractor can follow fluctuations in the mean frequency of three VCO ring attractors, which might be caused by phase noise in the VCOs. This requires using the mean of the three phases of each VCO ring as an ongoing target to entrain the phase of the activity bump in the baseline ring. However, there are three possible solutions for the mean of the three phases, spaced 120° apart (the phase of each VCO ring is only determined modulo 360°, so the mean phase is only determined modulo 120°). Since modulation of grid cell firing by the baseline oscillation restricts firing to positive baseline phases, each solution corresponds to three different (shifted) grid patterns (Burgess, 2008, their Fig. 7a). To ensure stability, the baseline phase should track a single solution and not switch between different solutions. This will be achieved if the three VCO rings start with coherent phases (e.g., following reset by place cell feedback), and baseline entrainment occurs faster than the accumulation of phase error. This entrainment can be implemented by the appropriate connectivity between the VCO rings and the baseline ring attractor, as described below (see Materials and Methods for details).
We specifically targeted entrainment of the third harmonic of the baseline oscillation. As long as the oscillation of the activity bump around the ring remains coherent (i.e., the phases of the other significant harmonics are locked with the third), this achieves the desired result. Moreover, since this component is three times the base frequency, it allows us to use the sum of the phases of the three VCO rings for entraining the baseline ring, which is easier to implement than using the mean of the phases of the three VCO rings. To do this, we constructed an oscillating ring-attractor network with dynamics able to stabilize a relatively narrow Gaussian bump of activity, by calculating the appropriate feedback connectivity. Since narrower Gaussians have larger components at high frequencies, a narrow bump of activity is easier to entrain via its third harmonic. We show that, with the appropriate connection weights, the baseline oscillatory frequency can be entrained by an external input representing the sum of the phases of the three VCO rings, and indicate how this sum of phases could be derived from the VCO ring attractors (see Materials and Methods). We applied the NEF (Eliasmith and Anderson, 2003) to derive the weights necessary for feedback connections to stabilize an appropriately sized Gaussian bump in the baseline ring attractor, and for external inputs to modulate its third harmonic (Fig. 5A, schematic; Materials and Methods; Conklin and Eliasmith, 2005).
We ran simulations with the network modulated by inputs oscillating with the sum of the phases of three VCO rings (Fig. 5B,C). For this demonstration, we used VCO ring frequencies starting at 5, 6, and 7 Hz and all increasing by 3.5 Hz over 1 s. This network was very reliably entrained to the mean phase of the VCO rings nearest to its activity bump (given the 120° ambiguity in mean phase), and thus tightly followed the mean frequency of the VCO rings.
Discussion
According to the oscillatory interference model, grid cell firing reflects interference between multiple VCOs whose phases, relative to a baseline oscillation, represent the animal's distance traveled along the VCOs' “preferred directions.” Consequently, the model is sensitive to accumulating phase noise in the VCOs. When using three or more VCOs with different preferred directions, phase noise additionally corrupts grid cell firing due to inconsistency in the locations represented by pairs of VCOs (Fig. 3).
Phase noise must be controlled sufficiently for the spatial representation to be stable over short distances and durations between resets by environmental information (Etienne et al., 1996). We addressed the control of VCO phase noise in two ways: (1) coupling the baseline oscillation to VCOs with three different preferred directions to eliminate phase inconsistency between them and (2) coupling VCOs with the same preferred direction to reduce the effects of phase noise in individual VCOs. We then showed how both approaches could work together.
Coupling VCOs with three different preferred directions
Under the configuration of three VCOs with 120° spacing of preferred directions, coupling a modulatory baseline oscillation (Burgess, 2008) to the VCOs so that it follows their mean frequency enforces consistent phases corresponding to the mean location represented by pairs of VCOs. With three VCOs, phase noise with SD ∼2–3 ms per 125 ms cycle allows location to be stably represented for 1–2 min (Fig. 4B).
This approach follows from the observation that, as the animal moves around, changes in the phases of VCOs with symmetrically distributed preferred directions should always sum to zero. Conversely, phase changes that sum to zero are always coherent: corresponding to a grid cell representation of a shifted location. In the face of independent identically distributed phase noise in three VCOs with 120° spacing, we can optimally mitigate the effect of noise and ensure continued phase coherence, by subtracting one-third of the summed phase change in the VCOs from each (Fig. 2). This is achieved automatically by making the phase of the baseline oscillation track the mean phase of the VCOs, so that its frequency tracks their mean frequency. The zero sum of phase changes can be understood intuitively, reflecting the symmetry of the VCO's preferred directions, which ensures that the net translation along these directions sum to zero. It is also consistent with the Fourier shift theorem, if the VCO rings are identified as the Fourier components of the 2D grid firing pattern (Orchard et al., 2013).
Coupling VCOs with the same preferred direction
Oscillatory MPOs measured in MEC slices have a phase SD of ∼15 ms per cycle (Zilli et al., 2009), which would give stable grid firing for only ∼3 s. However, the slice preparation may well disconnect grid cells from the ascending theta system that may contain coherent VCO networks (Welday et al., 2011). Furthermore, coupling neuronal oscillators to entrain one another to the same phase (Zilli and Hasselmo, 2010), or arranging them as a ring attractor such that they fire with sequentially offset phases (Blair et al., 2008; Welday et al., 2011), would achieve much lower variance and might correspond to the extended theta network operating in vivo. We show that in the latter case, a ring-attractor network of N VCOs maintains fixed offsets between their phases and increases their phase reliability by the order of √N (Fig. 4C) for a range of neuronal noise levels.
Combining both approaches
A population of grid cells with spatially offset grids can be driven by ring attractors containing phase-offset VCOs (Blair et al., 2008; Welday et al., 2011; Bush and Burgess, 2014). The baseline oscillation could also be implemented in a ring attractor, entrained to track the mean frequency of the VCO rings. We showed one way this entrainment could be implemented using connections between different ring-attractor networks (Fig. 5). The baseline ring attractor will precisely track the mean frequency of the VCO rings if its third harmonic is entrained to the sum of the VCO phases. We demonstrated network connectivity between ring attractors that would be able to implement this.
We also discussed how configurations with additional VCO directions could be implemented, such as the 6 × 60° configuration in which each VCO ring could be phase coupled to the VCO ring with the opposing preferred direction, without compromising the baseline entrainment mechanism. Note that Figure 4 shows increases in performance relating to both phase noise cancellation by an entrained baseline (e.g., when going from the 2 × 60° to the 3 × 120° configuration) and from increasing the number of coupled VCOs (e.g., the √N increase in stability in a ring attractor when going from the 3 × 120° to the 6 × 60° configuration).
General implications
The oscillatory interference model can be seen as mapping location in 2D space onto the phase differences between VCO rings (Blair et al., 2014). From this perspective, our configuration of three VCO rings with 120° spacing represents location by the phase differences between the VCO rings and the baseline. The dimensionality of the phase difference code (3) is reduced to match that of 2D space by the constraint that the sum of phase differences is constant, ensuring a continuous mapping between phase and location. The baseline in this configuration can be thought of as a fourth VCO ring, which could also be velocity modulated similarly to other VCO rings. As long as the other three VCOs are symmetrically arranged around the “baseline” in terms of their modulation by the direction and speed of movement, then entraining the baseline to the mean phase of the other VCO rings will have the desired effect. We note that our analyses concern mitigating the effects of phase noise within a “module” of grid cells and VCOs with the same grid scale. The coherence of representations across modules of different scales is not considered here, but does have interesting implications for decoding error (for review, see Sreenivasan and Fiete, 2011; Blair et al., 2014; Towse et al., 2014).
Our implementation of baseline phase entrainment suggests a functional role for harmonics of theta frequency oscillations, which may, for example, manifest as theta–gamma frequency coupling (Belluscio et al. 2012; Lisman and Jensen, 2013). This may reflect an important aspect of oscillatory interference models: that the proposed oscillations are tightly coupled, rather than independent oscillations, which would rapidly decohere. Such coupling suggests a role for local circuits, and for phase coordination across the septohippocampal theta network (Blair et al., 2008; Welday et al., 2011). Failure to detect theta rhythmicity in the firing of grid cells in bats (Yartsev et al., 2011) argues against oscillatory models; however, rhythmicity may be undetectable due to the very low firing rates and movement speeds in this study (Barry et al., 2012). Additionally, the absolute frequency band of oscillations in the model is unimportant, with some suggesting that delta-band rhythmicity plays the role of rodent theta in bats (Heys et al., 2013) and humans (Watrous et al., 2013), while a baseline frequency of zero corresponds to a nonoscillatory network that integrates self-motion along multiple preferred directions (Mhatre et al., 2012).
Without our scheme to maintain phase consistency between VCOs, the amount of inconsistency between three or more VCOs (e.g., the area of the triangle of pairwise intersections; Fig. 2C) will increase as noise accumulates, potentially signaling the uncertainty in represented location. This measure of uncertainty is another advantage of tracking distance redundantly along more than two directions (Burgess, 2008). Similarly, sailors obtain a position fix using multiple “lines of position” from landmarks, enabling them to gauge uncertainty: using three lines produces a triangular region (a “cocked-hat”) whose size reflects measurement inconsistency (Cort, 2009). Common noise in VCO phases will not affect the spatial firing pattern, although common errors in self-motion information will cause drift in any path integration system, and can only be corrected by environmental input (Cheung et al., 2008).
In summary, the oscillatory interference model (Burgess, 2008) produces coherent grid cell firing patterns that remain stable for seconds in the face of reasonable assumptions regarding phase noise in an isolated system. Extending this scheme to the in vivo situation, in which location-specific information (perhaps mediated by place cells) might reset VCO phases (Burgess et al., 2007; Monaco et al., 2011; Sreenivasan and Fiete, 2011; Bush and Burgess, 2014) and VCOs with constant phase offsets could be arranged as ring attractors (Blair et al., 2008), potentially provides unlimited additional stability. This situation is more consistent with the spatially stable theta-phase of firing observed in grid cells (Hafting et al., 2008; Reifenstein et al., 2012) and large-scale coherence of the septohippocampal theta system (Buzsáki, 2002) than analyses of MPOs in vitro (Zilli et al., 2009; Remme et al., 2010). The provision of a local baseline oscillation might be a role for local circuits in MEC, while the septohippocampal theta system may contain the proposed coherent sets of VCOs (Welday et al., 2011).
Footnotes
This work is supported by the Medical Research Council (UCL Biomedicine studentship to C.B., program grant to N.B.), The Wellcome Trust, EU SpaceBrain Project, and Office of Naval Research MURI N00014-10-1-0936. We thank an anonymous referee for insightful comments.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.
- Correspondence should be addressed to either of the following: Christopher P. Burgess, UCL Institute of Ophthalmology, 11-43 Bath Street, London EC1V 9EL, UK, christopher.burgess.10{at}ucl.ac.uk; or Neil Burgess, UCL Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17 Queens Square, London WC1N 3AR, UK, n.burgess{at}ucl.ac.uk
This article is freely available online through the J Neurosci Author Open Choice option.