Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Journal Club

A Role for Short-Lived Synapses in Adult Cortex?

Samuel J. Barnes and Claire E. J. Cheetham
Journal of Neuroscience 21 May 2014, 34 (21) 7044-7046; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1108-14.2014
Samuel J. Barnes
1MRC Centre for Developmental Neurobiology, King's College London, London SE1 1UL, United Kingdom and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Claire E. J. Cheetham
2National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, Bethesda, Maryland 20892
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Longitudinal imaging studies of individual neurons in vivo have greatly enhanced our understanding of the rewiring that the adult brain is capable of, both during normal experience and in response to altered sensory input or learning. Most of these studies have focused on dendritic spines, tiny femtoliter-sized protrusions from the dendritic shaft that receive the majority of excitatory synapses onto pyramidal neurons. Electron microscopy (EM) studies have established a strong positive correlation between spine and postsynaptic density (PSD) size. PSD size, which is activity-dependent and varies between synapses, in turn provides a good measure of synaptic strength. Hence, dendritic spines have become a well established and frequently used proxy for the number and strength of cortical excitatory synapses.

In vivo two-photon imaging studies have shown that while most dendritic spines persist for weeks or months, ongoing formation and elimination of spines occurs even in the adult cortex (Trachtenberg et al., 2002). This spine turnover is thought to underpin experience-dependent reorganization of cortical circuitry as well as some forms of learning and memory (Holtmaat et al., 2006; Keck et al., 2008; Xu et al., 2009; Yang et al., 2009). However, fundamental questions remain: How frequently do new spines form synapses? What is the temporal relationship between spine and synapse formation, and how does the presence of a PSD affect spine survival? Finally, and most tantalizing: what is the function of transient spines that live for only hours or days?

In a recent study published in The Journal of Neuroscience, Cane et al. (2014) set out to address many of these questions. They used in utero electroporation to label both PSDs and neuronal morphology, including dendritic spines of mouse layer 2/3 cortical pyramidal neurons, by coexpressing PSD-95-eGFP and DSRedExpress. They then implanted cranial windows over somatosensory cortex and used chronic in vivo two-photon microscopy to repeatedly image the same dendritic structures in layer 1 over hours and days. This enabled a systematic comparison of the temporal dynamics of spines and PSDs. PSDs were detected using a method based on localized accumulation of PSD-95-eGFP fluorescence. By reconstructing a stretch of dendrite imaged in vivo using Focused Ion Beam Scanning EM, the authors provided compelling evidence that they could detect in vivo the smallest PSDs discernable with EM.

Determining the relationship between spine and synapse formation is an important task that is not without technical difficulties. Previous correlative EM studies have established that not all spines have a PSD, at least not all of the time: whereas spines ≥4-d-old always possessed PSDs, younger spines frequently did not (Knott et al., 2006). However, EM by necessity provides a “snapshot” assessment of PSD presence. By incorporating an in vivo PSD marker and using short (6–24 h) intervals between imaging sessions, Cane et al. (2014) were able to gain considerable new insight into this relationship. They confirmed that both spines and PSDs appear and disappear over time, even without overt manipulation of sensory experience. Most new spines lacked a PSD when first observed. However, the proportion of new spines carrying a PSD increased with spine lifetime, from 19% at ≤6 h old to 30% at ≤24 h old and 42% at ≤96 h old. Therefore, while PSDs can form within 6 h, delayed or more gradual PSD acquisition is also possible.

One intriguing point not discussed by the authors is that late PSD acquisition was rarely seen. Indeed, 32% of spines lacking a PSD at <6 h old acquired one over the ensuing 18 h, whereas none of the spines lacking a PSD at 24 h old acquired one over the subsequent 24 h. Therefore, PSD acquisition may be predominantly confined to the first 24 h after spine formation, matching the time course for PSD insertion at new spines in hippocampal slice cultures (De Roo et al., 2008). How the temporal window for PSD formation is regulated is unknown, although it appears that the pattern of cortical activity is the key determinant of spine stability in vivo (Wyatt et al., 2012).

It has been widely thought that PSD formation is required for spine survival, such that persistent spines always contain a PSD. As described above, Cane et al. (2014) show that the proportion of spines carrying a PSD increases with spine age, indicating that the presence of a PSD does indeed promote spine stability. However, PSD formation was not sufficient to render a spine persistent: 25% of spines present at only a single time point had a PSD, while 70% of new spines that bore a PSD at least once still disappeared within 4 d. This suggests that there is not a simple binary relationship between PSD formation and spine survival. In further support of this idea, only 83% of new spines that survived for ≥4 d displayed a PSD once or more. Although nonsynaptic spines have been described previously, they have been widely thought of as “intermediates,” i.e., new spines yet to form a synapse or disappearing spines that have lost their synapses (Arellano et al., 2007). Data in Cane et al. (2014) suggest that some PSD-95-lacking spines are in fact persistent, although what role they play remains to be determined.

Analysis of spines that were destined to disappear adds further weight to the argument that the presence of a PSD does not always result in a persistent spine. Although PSD loss and spine elimination were temporally matched for the majority of disappearing spines, 20% of disappearing spines survived for ≥4 d after PSD loss. In marked contrast, only 5% of new spines that never acquired a PSD lived for ≥4 d (Cane et al., 2014). Hence, spines that once had a PSD disappear at a slower rate than those that never contained a PSD. Perhaps some echo or trace of that PSD remains in the spine to promote stability. This could take the form of a weak complement of AMPA or NMDA receptors, meaning that the spine retains some level of neurotransmission. Alternatively, although the authors verified that spines lacking PSD-95 in vivo also lacked a detectable PSD by EM, other residual scaffolding or synaptic adhesion proteins may be disassembled more slowly than the PSD itself. Similarly, it is possible that low levels of extrasynaptic receptors could support neurotransmission at new spines before PSD insertion.

Nevertheless, any discussion of the role of new spines is transformed by the finding that some transient spines do acquire PSDs (Cane et al., 2014), and may therefore function as bona fide short-lived synapses. It is possible that new spines, and even PSDs, are generated by an entirely postsynaptic process that has little consequence for synaptic connectivity (Fig. 1A). However, increased spinogenesis following either motor learning (Xu et al., 2009; Yang et al., 2009) or sensory deprivation (Holtmaat et al., 2006) is testament to the role that new spines likely play in memory acquisition and cortical circuit reorganization. Given the sparse connectivity of excitatory intracortical networks, only a small number of new spines need to be stabilized to dramatically alter network architecture (Chklovskii et al., 2004). In keeping with this idea, new spine stabilization increases after sensory deprivation, but these new persistent spines comprise only 13% of the total spine population (Holtmaat et al., 2006). It is possible that the transient PSD-carrying spines described by Cane et al. (2014) have the potential to stabilize but do not receive appropriate reinforcement in terms of the pattern or frequency of presynaptic stimulation (Fig. 1B). An alternative hypothesis arises from the fact that two-thirds of the boutons contacted by new spines already have a synapse (Knott et al., 2006); hence, new spine addition may result in a competitive interaction that destabilizes the original synapse at these multisynapse boutons. Transient spines may therefore be more than just spines that fail to stabilize, and instead play an active role in cortical reorganization by destabilizing existing networks (Fig. 1C). Finally, transient spines could temporarily increase correlated activity in connected neurons, promoting strengthening and/or stabilization of existing synapses between those neurons (Fig. 1D). A key next step will be to confirm that transient PSD-carrying spines do form functional synaptic contacts.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Possible roles of transient PSD-carrying spines. A, The formation of a transient spine (center) is an entirely postsynaptic process and although it contains a PSD (red) there is no presynaptic partner and thus no functional connectivity. B, Transient spines fail to stabilize: after contacting a presynaptic bouton (black), the presynaptic activity pattern is not sufficient to maintain the spine. C, Transient spines destabilize existing connections by forming a competing synapse at multisynapse boutons. D, Transient spines temporarily increase correlated activity between connected neurons and strengthen preexisting synapses.

Cane et al. (2014) also describe PSDs that were not located in a discernable spine. These represented 20% of imaged structures and although spine synapses that protrude predominantly in the lower resolution axial (z) plane may contribute, the authors suggest that these are likely shaft synapses. Approximately 25% of excitatory inputs to cortical pyramidal neurons are in fact formed on dendritic shafts (Knott et al., 2006), yet the structural dynamics and even strength of these shaft synapses are poorly understood. This is in part because they lack a clear structural proxy, rendering longitudinal tracking studies challenging. The strategy used by Cane et al. (2014) provides a solution to this technical problem, and indeed, the authors clearly show that shaft synapses do turn over, and furthermore, that shaft synapse turnover rate is comparable to that of the spine population as a whole. Hence, shaft synapse plasticity is an underappreciated mechanism that could contribute to cortical rewiring.

In conclusion, the data described in Cane et al. (2014) represent an important advance in our understanding of synapse structural dynamics. In particular, the finding that transient spines can carry a PSD hints at a more instructive role for transient spines in cortical reorganization than previously thought. However, interesting questions remain, such as how the temporal progression of synapse formation is affected by sensory deprivation or task-specific repetitive activation. Furthermore, identification and tracking of the presynaptic partners of both new and persistent spines remains an important future goal toward understanding adult plasticity.

Footnotes

  • Editor's Note: These short, critical reviews of recent papers in the Journal, written exclusively by graduate students or postdoctoral fellows, are intended to summarize the important findings of the paper and provide additional insight and commentary. For more information on the format and purpose of the Journal Club, please see http://www.jneurosci.org/misc/ifa_features.shtml.

  • C.E.J.C. is funded by a Long-Term Fellowship from the Human Frontier Science Program.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Claire E. J. Cheetham, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, 35 Convent Drive, Bethesda, MD 20892. claire.cheetham{at}nih.gov

References

  1. ↵
    1. Arellano JI,
    2. Espinosa A,
    3. Fairén A,
    4. Yuste R,
    5. DeFelipe J
    (2007) Non-synaptic dendritic spines in neocortex. Neuroscience 145:464–469, doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.12.015, pmid:17240073.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Cane M,
    2. Maco B,
    3. Knott G,
    4. Holtmaat A
    (2014) The relationship between PSD-95 clustering and spine stability in vivo. J Neurosci 34:2075–2086, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3353-13.2014, pmid:24501349.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    1. Chklovskii DB,
    2. Mel BW,
    3. Svoboda K
    (2004) Cortical rewiring and information storage. Nature 431:782–788, doi:10.1038/nature03012, pmid:15483599.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. De Roo M,
    2. Klauser P,
    3. Mendez P,
    4. Poglia L,
    5. Muller D
    (2008) Activity-dependent PSD formation and stabilization of newly formed spines in hippocampal slice cultures. Cereb Cortex 18:151–161, doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm041, pmid:17517683.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    1. Holtmaat A,
    2. Wilbrecht L,
    3. Knott GW,
    4. Welker E,
    5. Svoboda K
    (2006) Experience-dependent and cell-type-specific spine growth in the neocortex. Nature 441:979–983, doi:10.1038/nature04783, pmid:16791195.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Keck T,
    2. Mrsic-Flogel TD,
    3. Vaz Afonso M,
    4. Eysel UT,
    5. Bonhoeffer T,
    6. Hübener M
    (2008) Massive restructuring of neuronal circuits during functional reorganization of adult visual cortex. Nat Neurosci 11:1162–1167, doi:10.1038/nn.2181, pmid:18758460.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Knott GW,
    2. Holtmaat A,
    3. Wilbrecht L,
    4. Welker E,
    5. Svoboda K
    (2006) Spine growth precedes synapse formation in the adult neocortex in vivo. Nat Neurosci 9:1117–1124, doi:10.1038/nn1747, pmid:16892056.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Trachtenberg JT,
    2. Chen BE,
    3. Knott GW,
    4. Feng G,
    5. Sanes JR,
    6. Welker E,
    7. Svoboda K
    (2002) Long-term in vivo imaging of experience-dependent synaptic plasticity in adult cortex. Nature 420:788–794, doi:10.1038/nature01273, pmid:12490942.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Wyatt RM,
    2. Tring E,
    3. Trachtenberg JT
    (2012) Pattern and not magnitude of neural activity determines dendritic spine stability in awake mice. Nat Neurosci 15:949–951, doi:10.1038/nn.3134, pmid:22706266.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Xu T,
    2. Yu X,
    3. Perlik AJ,
    4. Tobin WF,
    5. Zweig JA,
    6. Tennant K,
    7. Jones T,
    8. Zuo Y
    (2009) Rapid formation and selective stabilization of synapses for enduring motor memories. Nature 462:915–919, doi:10.1038/nature08389, pmid:19946267.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Yang G,
    2. Pan F,
    3. Gan WB
    (2009) Stably maintained dendritic spines are associated with lifelong memories. Nature 462:920–924, doi:10.1038/nature08577, pmid:19946265.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 34 (21)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 34, Issue 21
21 May 2014
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
A Role for Short-Lived Synapses in Adult Cortex?
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
A Role for Short-Lived Synapses in Adult Cortex?
Samuel J. Barnes, Claire E. J. Cheetham
Journal of Neuroscience 21 May 2014, 34 (21) 7044-7046; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1108-14.2014

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
A Role for Short-Lived Synapses in Adult Cortex?
Samuel J. Barnes, Claire E. J. Cheetham
Journal of Neuroscience 21 May 2014, 34 (21) 7044-7046; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1108-14.2014
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Adult-Born Granule Cells Contribute to Dentate Gyrus Circuit Reorganization after Traumatic Brain Injury
  • The Oscillatory Nature of Movement Initiation
  • Lateral Preoptic Hypothalamus: A Window to Understanding Insomnia
Show more Journal Club
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.