Abstract
Human neuroimaging studies indicate that the anterior midcingulate cortex (AMC) and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) play important roles in the expression and extinction of fear, respectively. Electrophysiological rodent studies further indicate that oscillatory neuronal activity in homolog regions (i.e., prelimbic and infralimbic cortices) changes during fear expression and fear extinction recall. Whether similar processes occur in humans remains largely unexplored. By assessing scalp surface EEG in conjunction with LORETA source estimation of CS-related theta and gamma activity, we tested whether a priori defined ROIs in the human AMC and vmPFC similarly modulate their oscillatory activity during fear expression and extinction recall, respectively. To this end, 42 healthy individuals underwent a differential conditioning/differential extinction protocol with a Recall Test on the next day. In the Recall Test, nonextinguished versus extinguished stimuli evoked an increased differential (CS+ vs CS−) response with regard to skin conductance and AMC-localized theta power. Conversely, extinguished versus nonextinguished stimuli evoked an increased differential response with regard to vmPFC-localized gamma power. Finally, individuals who failed to show a suppressed skin conductance response to the extinguished versus nonextinguished CS+ also failed to show the otherwise observed alterations in vmPFC gamma power to extinguished CS+. These results indicate that fear expression is associated with AMC theta activity, whereas successful fear extinction recall relates to changes in vmPFC gamma activity. The present work thereby bridges findings from prior rodent electrophysiological research and human neuroimaging studies and indicates that EEG is a valuable tool for future fear extinction research.
Introduction
Investigating how human brains retrieve previously conditioned and extinguished fear is critical for understanding anxiety (Myers and Davis, 2007; Norberg et al., 2008). Human neuroimaging studies suggest that the recall of conditioned and extinguished fear involves the anterior midcingulate cortex (AMC) and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), respectively (Etkin et al., 2011; Milad and Quirk, 2012). Specifically, the AMC shows stronger activation to fear-conditioned versus nonconditioned stimuli (Büchel et al., 1998; Knight et al., 2004; Phelps et al., 2004; Milad et al., 2007a). Conversely, the vmPFC is more active when previously extinguished versus nonextinguished fear stimuli are presented after extinction (e.g. 24 h later) (Kalisch et al., 2006; Milad et al., 2007b), consistent with a critical role of vmPFC in fear extinction recall (Phelps et al., 2004; Quirk and Mueller, 2008).
In rodents, neurons in prelimbic cortex (a homolog region of human AMC) (Milad and Quirk, 2012) respond to previously fear-conditioned, nonextinguished stimuli with a sustained shift from 2 Hz to ∼4–8 Hz (Burgos-Robles et al., 2009), whereas neurons in the infralimbic cortex (a homolog region of vmPFC) (Milad and Quirk, 2012) switch to even higher frequencies during the presentation of previously extinguished fear stimuli (Milad and Quirk, 2002). Moreover, electrical stimulations of prelimbic and infralimbic neurons increase and decrease conditioned fear, respectively, suggesting that high-frequency activity in medial frontal cortices plays a causal role in the expression and suppression of fear (Milad and Quirk, 2002; Vidal-Gonzalez et al., 2006).
Whether prefrontal cortex oscillations change during the recall of conditioned and extinguished fear in humans is currently unknown. Notably, whereas the human AMC generates theta oscillations (e.g., 4–8 Hz) (Mueller et al., 2011), a relevant frequency range for fear expression in animals (Seidenbecher et al., 2003; Adhikari et al., 2010; Lesting et al., 2011), the vmPFC has been linked to the gamma rhythm (e.g., 36.5–44 Hz) (Pizzagalli et al., 2006; Mantini et al., 2007). Thus, we expect that retrieval of conditioned fear will relate to AMC theta activity (Hypothesis 1), whereas fear-extinction recall will relate to vmPFC gamma activity (Hypothesis 2). Although prior EEG studies on fear learning have focused on event-related potentials (Pizzagalli et al., 2003), fear-enhanced sensory processes (Miskovic and Keil, 2012), and single-session learning (Flor et al., 1996), none has probed oscillatory activity during retrieval of previously conditioned/extinguished fear.
Here, participants underwent a two-day differential fear conditioning and extinction paradigm (see Fig. 1). During acquisition, two stimuli were fear conditioned (CS + E and CS + N) and two were not (CS − E and CS − N). During subsequent extinction, CS + E and CS − E were extinguished (i.e., presented without the US). To dissociate recall of conditioned and extinguished fear, CS + N and CS − N were not extinguished. Finally, all four CS were presented and EEG was recorded during a Recall Test on the next day. Low Resolution Electromagnetic Tomography (LORETA) (Pascual-Marqui et al., 1999) analysis was performed on day 2 CS-related EEG to probe whether AMC theta and vmPFC gamma during CS presentation differed as a function of day 1 learning and extinction history.
Materials and Methods
Participants.
A total of N = 42 subjects participated in the present study. Data were initially collected in two cohorts (A and B) with slightly varying experimental procedures (see below). Cohorts A and B consisted of N = 22 (mean age: 21.1 years, SD: 1.4 years, 11 males) and N = 20 (mean age: 25.0 years, SD: 3.7 years; 9 males; all right-handed and white) psychology students, respectively, who participated for partial fulfillment of course credit. After confirming that the central effects of the present study were not modulated by cohorts, both datasets were combined to increase statistical power. The study was approved by the local ethics committee of Marburg University Psychology Department.
Experimental paradigm.
The two-day paradigm consisted of the four stages: Habituation (day 1), Acquisition (day 1), Extinction (day 1), and Extinction Recall (day 2). During Habituation, each CS was presented alone 5 times for 4 s with a jittered interstimulus interval of 6–8 s. The Acquisition consisted of three blocks in each of which every CS was presented 20 times in random order. The CS+ co-terminated with the US in 50% of the trials. In the Extinction phase (day 1), only one CS+ (“CS + E”) and one CS− (“CS − E”) were presented 40 times and a novel face (“Dummy Stimulus”) was presented 20 times to maintain some variability in the presented stimuli. One CS+ (“CS + N”), one CS− (“CS − N”), and the US were never presented during the Extinction phase. Finally, Recall Test (day 2) was identical to the Acquisition stage with the exception that the US was not presented (i.e., all CS were presented again). In Cohort B, the Extinction phase differed such that CS + E, CS − E, and the dummy stimulus were only presented 30, 30, and 15 times, respectively. In addition, throughout all phases, participants of Cohort B had to respond to randomly occurring arrows pointing to the left or right with an associated button press. This secondary task was included to ensure that participants attended to the screen throughout the experiment.
The CS stimuli consisted of four male faces with a neutral expression (Ekman and Friesen, 1976). The faces were black and white and presented in front of a black background with a size of 8.5 × 11.5 cm (Cohort A) and 17 × 22 cm (Cohort B). The specific assignment (i.e., CS + E, CS + N, CS − E, CS − N; see below) of the CS was counterbalanced across face stimuli. The US consisted of a loud 85 dB 1000 ms white noise burst delivered over room speakers. If participants felt that the 85 dB burst was too loud, the US intensity was adapted such that it was perceived as “highly annoying but not painful.”
Affective ratings.
Participants rated each CS with respect to its associated valence and arousal on a 5 point Likert scale. In the Acquisition and Recall Test stage, all four CS were rated; in the Extinction stage, only CS + E and CS − E were rated. Ratings were given after each block (Cohort A) or after each stage (Cohorts A and B). To test for effective acquisition on day 1, the ratings after the Acquisition phase were analyzed. Effective extinction was probed by comparing CS + E versus CS − E ratings after the Acquisition phase with CS + E versus CS − E ratings after the Extinction phase. Extinction and conditioning recall were assessed with Block 1 ratings of the day 2 Recall Test.
Skin conductance.
Skin conductance was measured with an extra channel of the BioSemi ActiveTwo EEG system (see below) using two electrodes applied to the middle phalanges of the nondominant hand. Skin conductance was manually screened for artifacts, and the peak response in a window from 1 to 5 s relative to the CS onset was automatically determined for each trial. Skin conductance responses were then normalized by dividing by the subject's maximum overall response across conditions (Lykken and Venables, 1971) and averaged across trials by CS type. For two participants, skin conductance recordings were unavailable because of malfunctioning electrodes.
EEG recording and source localization.
EEG was recorded during the day 2 Recall Test using a 64-channel BioSemi ActiveTwo EEG system with Driving Right Leg and Common Mode Sense as reference electrodes (for Cohort A, EEG was also recorded on day 1). EEG was rereferenced to the average reference, filtered using a 0.5 Hz low-pass filter, eye-blink corrected using Independent Component Analysis with manual rejection of clear eye-blink related components, manually screened for artifacts, and segmented into epochs ranging from 0 to 2 s relative to CS onset (Milad and Quirk, 2002). Using the LORETA software, these epochs were then subjected to standard spectral analyses via Discrete Fourier Transform using a boxcar windowing (Brillinger, 1981), yielding cross spectra with a resolution of 0.5 Hz.
Next, for each frequency bin within the theta (4–8 Hz) (Mueller et al., 2011) and gamma (36.5–44 Hz) (Pizzagalli et al., 2006) range, intracerebral current density was computed using the LORETA algorithm across 2394 voxels constrained to cortical gray matter and hippocampi (Pascual-Marqui et al., 1994, 1999). LORETA finds a unique inverse solution for the cortical sources of the current pattern at the scalp level by assuming that neighboring neurons are activated synchronously and therefore produce smooth spatial distributions of intracerebral electrical activity. Although the validity of this distributed source localization technique has been supported from studies combining LORETA with fMRI (Vitacco et al., 2002; Mulert et al., 2004), PET (Pizzagalli et al., 2004), and intracranial recordings (Zumsteg et al., 2005), it is important to acknowledge the limitations of this method, including its limited spatial resolution. Next, using custom-made MATLAB (MathWorks) scripts, the estimated single-trial current density at each voxel was normalized such that the total activation across all voxels amounted to 1 for both EEG bands and averaged across all trials of one CS type to achieve CS-associated brain activation maps.
AMC and vmPFC ROIs were identified as the peak activation reported by Phelps et al. (2004) for the CS+ versus CS− contrast on day 2 fear expression (AMC Talairach coordinates: 0, 19, 30) and the peak activation in vmPFC reported by Milad et al. (2007b) for the CS + E versus CS + N contrast (vmPFC Talairach coordinates: 2, 35, −8). For statistical analyses, current density for AMC and vmPFC was extracted from the nearest corresponding voxels in LORETA space (AMC Talairach coordinates: −3, 17, 29; LORETA voxel #1645; vmPFC Talairach coordinates: 4, 38, −6; LORETA voxel #778). For display purposes, activation values in the ROIs were scaled by the factor 1,000,000.
In addition to analyses at the source level, scalp EEG spectral power was also assessed by FFT of the first and the second hamming-windowed 1000 ms relative to CS onset. These separate 1-s epochs allowed us to further explore whether CS-related oscillatory activity changed throughout the 2-s analysis window. The same theta and gamma ranges as in the source analyses were considered for the scalp data. In addition, at a reviewer's request, the frequency ranges from 30 to 43 Hz and from 65 to 80 Hz were explored. As shown in Figure 2, CS-related θ and γ activity (collapsed across conditions) was maximal at frontal midline channel Fz and right frontopolar channel AF8, respectively. Scalp θ was therefore measured at channel Fz and neighboring channels FCz and AFz, whereas gamma was measured at AF8 and neighboring channels Fp2 and F8.
Statistical analysis.
The central hypotheses were all tested using repeated-measures ANOVAs with contingency (CS+ vs CS−) and extinction (E vs N) as repeated measure factors; the Greenhouse–Geisser correction was used where applicable. To confirm day 1 extinction of the differential response to CS + E vs CS − E, a repeated-measures ANOVA with contingency (CS + E vs CS − E) and stage (before vs after extinction) was conducted on the affective ratings. Tests on brain activity focused on LORETA-estimated current density in two a priori defined ROIs, and the α-level was set to 0.05. For tests on whole-brain LORETA activation, tetrad extinction recall (ER) scores were computed for each voxel (ER = [CS + N − CS − N] − [CS + E − CS − E]). If this score deviates from 0, the differential fear responses (i.e., CS+ vs CS−) are different for stimuli that had or had not been extinguished 24 h before. However, more positive ER values may result from both increased activity to CS + N versus CS − N and/or reduced activity to CS + E versus CS − E. Because the main purpose of the whole-brain LORETA analyses was to show that the reported effects were specific to the predefined ROIs, the threshold was set to p < 0.05 without correcting for multiple comparisons. Finally, in addition to extinction and contingency, ANOVAs on scalp EEG spectral power also included the factors electrode and time window (0–1000 vs 1000–2000 ms).
Results
Affective ratings
As shown in Figure 1C, D, ratings collected directly after acquisition demonstrated successful conditioning, with the CS+ rated as more arousing (main effect contingency: F(1,41) = 24.64, p < 0.0001) and unpleasant (F(1,41) = 9.16, p < 0.005) than the CS−. There was no main effect or interaction involving extinction, confirming that CS + E and CS + N were associated with equal levels of arousal (CS + E: 2.98 ± 1.26; CS + N: 2.93 ± 1.23) and negative versus positive valence (CS + E: 2.24 ± 0.93; CS + N: 2.14 ± 1.09) before extinction (p > 0.5). After the Extinction stage, the extinguished CS + E (compared with the CS − E) was rated as relatively less arousing and negatively valenced than before the Extinction stage, thereby confirming effective day 1 extinction (contingency × time interaction, arousal: F(1,41) = 10.03, p < 0.005; valence: F(1,41) = 15.71, p < 0.0005).
Most importantly, the day 2 Recall Test arousal ratings revealed that day 1 conditioning and extinction were successfully recalled (contingency × extinction interaction: F(1,21) = 6.72, p < 0.018). As expected, ratings were significantly higher for CS + N (2.45 ± 0.22) versus CS − N (1.72 ± 0.16; t(21) = 3.17, p < 0.005), which is consistent with successful recall of conditioned, nonextinguished fear. Conversely, arousal ratings were not significantly different between CS + E (2.18 ± 0.20) and CS − E (1.8 ± 0.19; t(21) = 1.89, p = 0.07), consistent with successful recall of extinguished fear. Main effects of contingency further indicate that CS+ were generally perceived as more arousing (F(1,21) = 7.45, p < 0.02) and unpleasant (F(1,21) = 6.25, p < 0.02) than CS−.
Physiological assessment of fear conditioning recall and extinction recall
Like the arousal ratings, day 2 SCR amplitudes confirmed successful recall of both fear conditioning and extinction (contingency × extinction interaction: (F(1,39) = 8.66, p < 0.006). Specifically, CS + N (0.051 ± 0.006) produced significantly larger SCRs than the CS − N (0.038 ± 0.005; t(38) = 3.11, p < 0.005), consistent with successful fear conditioning consolidation and recall. Critically, the CS + E (0.047 ± 0.007) did not show a larger SCR compared with the CS − E (0.050 ± 0.006; p > 0.6; Fig. 3A), indicating effective fear extinction recall.
Prefrontal oscillations during fear conditioning recall and extinction recall
Theta.
AMC-localized theta power during the day 2 Recall Test was modulated by a significant contingency × extinction interaction (F(1,41) = 8.87; p < 0.005; Fig. 3B). Providing important evidence for Hypothesis 1 (involvement of AMC theta in fear expression), AMC theta was significantly larger for CS + N (22.7± 0.95) relative to CS − N (22.3 ± 0.93; t(41) = 2.50, p < 0.02), although there were no significant differences between CS + E (22.4 ± 0.96) and CS − E (22.6 ± 0.95; t(41) = 1.00, p > 0.3). Highlighting the specificity of these findings, there were no significant main effects or interactions for the vmPFC ROI in the theta frequency band (p > 0.1).
Converging with the findings at the AMC, analyses of frontal midline scalp channel EEG also revealed a significant contingency × extinction interaction (F(1,41) = 5.22; p < 0.03). Corroborating our choice of investigating 2-s epochs for the LORETA analyses, the contingency × extinction interaction in the scalp analyses was not further modulated by time window (i.e., whether theta was recorded from 0 to 1000 ms or 1000 to 2000 ms relative to CS onset: time window × contingency × extinction: p > 0.4, partial eta squared: 0.01). As for AMC-localized theta, CS + N minus CS − N was larger than CS + E minus CS − E.
Additional exploratory analyses were performed with the scalp channel EEG: First, analysis of scalp channel theta power from 2000 to 3000 ms after CS onset during the Recall Test revealed that the observed effect did not extend beyond the 2000 ms analysis window contingency × extinction interaction (p > 0.5). Second, we analyzed EEG data collected during acquisition (only available for Cohort A) and confirmed the absence of a significant contingency × extinction interaction before the Recall Test (F(1,20) = 0.11, p > 0.7, partial eta squared = 0.005). Moreover, to test whether increased theta to nonextinguished CS+ versus CS− is specific for retrieval of conditioned fear (Hypothesis 1) and not related to general fear expression, we also compared CS+ and CS− during the Acquisition and Extinction phase. Consistent with Hypothesis 1, none of these analyses revealed significant differences between CS+ and CS− (p > 0.07).
Gamma.
In line with Hypothesis 2 (involvement of vmPFC γ in fear extinction recall), a contingency × extinction ANOVA on vmPFC-localized gamma current density also revealed a significant two-way interaction (F(1,41) = 5.45, p < 0.025; Fig. 3). Unlike AMC theta, post hoc tests revealed that vmPFC gamma did not differ between CS + N (25.5 ± 1.5) and CS − N (25.3 ± 1.5; t(41) = 0.86, p > 0.3) but rather was reduced during CS + E (25.3 ± 1.1) versus CS − E presentation (25.8 ± 1.6; t(41) = 2.45, p < 0.02), which is consistent with the hypothesized role of vmPFC in extinction recall rather than fear expression. There were no significant main effects or interactions for the AMC ROI in the gamma frequency band (p > 0.3).
Gamma power at the scalp level was not significantly modulated by contingency or extinction. Exploratory analyses for different time windows (i.e., 2–3 s after stimulus presentation) or gamma ranges (i.e., 30–43 Hz; 65–80 Hz) did not yield significant main effects or interactions (p > 0.3).
Regional specificity of the ROI findings
To confirm that the reported effects of theta and gamma activity were specific to AMC and vmPFC, respectively, we also tested for ER effects at the whole-brain level. For each voxel, frequency band, and participant, an ER score was computed and tested for significance across participants using an uncorrected p value of 0.05 (see Experimental procedures). Despite the liberal criterion, significant ER scores were largely restricted to the AMC in the theta band (Fig. 3D) and vmPFC in the gamma band (Fig. 3E), thereby supporting the regional specificity of the ROI findings (Fig. 3). Additional regions with significant voxels are provided in Table 1. Of note, gamma ER scores significantly differed from 0 within the left hippocampus, a region previously implicated in fear extinction recall (Kalisch et al., 2006; Milad et al., 2007b).
Relationships between electrodermal and electrocortical correlates of fear extinction
Although the differential SCR response was reliably extinguished at the group level, it should be noted that only n = 23 participants (58%) showed a reduced SCR when directly comparing CS + E vs CS + N responses, whereas this was not the case for a considerable number of participants (n = 17; 42%). This converges with prior reports of substantial between-subject variation in successful fear extinction recall and has potential relevance for the treatment of anxiety (Milad et al., 2009).
To further test whether fear extinction success was related to vmPFC activity (Milad et al., 2007b), we compared the 23 participants with successful SCR extinction with the 17 participants with failed SCR extinction. As shown in Figure 4, the successful SCR extinction group showed a very robust contingency × extinction interaction on vmPFC gamma current density (F(1,22) = 11.78, p < 0.003, partial eta square = 0.35), whereas this interaction was absent in the failed SCR extinction group (F(1,16) = 0.40, p > 0.5, partial eta square = 0.02; group × contingency × extinction interaction: F(1,38) = 3.82, p < 0.06).
Discussion
The overarching goal of this study was to elucidate neurophysiological mechanisms associated with the recall of conditioned and extinguished fear in humans using noninvasive EEG. Inspired by prior fMRI and rodent studies, we hypothesized that changes in AMC and vmPFC oscillatory activity would be associated with recall of conditioned and extinguished fear, respectively. We tested our hypotheses by using a two-day differential conditioning and differential extinction paradigm in conjunction with EEG and source localization. Consistent with our first hypothesis, we found that stimuli that had been fear conditioned and not extinguished on the previous day (i.e., CS + N) evoked significantly more AMC-theta activity than not fear conditioned stimuli (i.e., CS − N) in the day 2 Recall Test. Moreover, confirming our second hypothesis, fear stimuli that had been extinguished on the previous day (i.e., CS + E) elicited lower vmPFC gamma activity relative to nonfear stimuli that had been presented during extinction (i.e., CS − E). Thus, the present study shows, for the first time, that the retrieval of long-term conditioned and extinguished fear in humans is associated with changes in prefrontal cortex theta and gamma oscillatory activity.
Findings emerging in the context of the first hypothesis suggest that fear-evoking stimuli lead to increased theta activity in the AMC and thus converge with the following: (1) EEG studies showing that frontomedial theta is predominantly generated in the AMC (Asada et al., 1999; Ishii et al., 1999); (2) fMRI studies showing a predominant role of AMC in fear expression (for review, see: Vogt, 2005; Etkin et al., 2011; Milad and Quirk, 2012); and (3) animal studies indicating that (medial prefrontal) theta activity is highly relevant for fear expression (Seidenbecher et al., 2003; Pape et al., 2005; Burgos-Robles et al., 2009; Lesting et al., 2011; Narayanan et al., 2011). Together, these findings suggest that AMC theta may play an important role in human fear expression. This interpretation is further supported by the absence of AMC theta differences for extinguished CS+ versus CS−, mirroring the attenuation of prelimbic theta enhancements after successful fear extinction in rats (Burgos-Robles et al., 2009).
The AMC receives afferents from the amygdala (Vogt and Pandya, 1987; Vogt, 2005), and theta oscillations may serve to retrieve episodic memories and communicate between brain regions (Klimesch, 1996), including medial prefrontal cortex and amygdala during fear processing (Lesting et al., 2011). As the rodent prelimbic cortex is important for sustaining an increased level of fear during ongoing threat presentation, which only evokes a transient amygdala response directly after stimulus onset (Burgos-Robles et al., 2009), the observed AMC theta response might reflect an extended state of anxious apprehension during the retrieval of threat-related memories. The human AMC is involved in a number of relevant processes (Shackman et al., 2011), including central control of the autonomic nervous systems (Critchley et al., 2003; Panitz et al., 2013) and may thereby regulate physiological fear responses. Consistent with this interpretation, SCR findings closely mirrored the pattern of AMC theta: Both were increased in response to CS + N versus CS − N but not to CS + E versus CS − E. More generally, evoked frontomedial theta has been related to the processing of events that are worse than expected (Mueller et al., 2011; Cavanagh et al., 2012) and dispositional anxiety (Mueller et al., 2013). Together, CS + N-evoked AMC theta enhancements may indicate a number of mechanisms, including the retrieval of fear memories, regulation of physiological fear responses, or a general processing of any unpleasant events requiring adaptive control (Shackman et al., 2011). The functional relevance of the relationship between AMC theta and fear-related processing should be further investigated in future studies.
In contrast to AMC theta, vmPFC gamma was not affected by the presentation of nonextinguished CS+ versus CS−, which is consistent with studies indicating that the vmPFC in humans and the infralimbic cortex in rodents are not responsive to fear stimuli per se (Etkin et al., 2011; Milad and Quirk, 2012). Instead, vmPFC gamma activity differed between extinguished CS+ and CS−, which supports the following hypotheses: (1) a particular safety signal becomes associated with the CS+ during extinction (Myers and Davis, 2007); (2) the retention and recall of this safety signal involve the vmPFC or infralimbic cortex (Milad and Quirk, 2012); and (3) the retention and recall of this safety signal involve fast oscillatory processes in the vmPFC/infralimbic cortex (Milad and Quirk, 2002). Gamma activity can only be measured on the scalp surface EEG when large numbers of neurons show coordinated activities (Jensen et al., 2007). Therefore, the current findings may reflect that larger cell assemblies change their gamma-range oscillation patterns during fear extinction recall, perhaps because more specific neuron populations in the vmPFC increase their firing rates in response to the CS + E (Milad and Quirk, 2002; Santini et al., 2008).
During extinction recall, the vmPFC presumably receives inputs from the hippocampus (Kalisch et al., 2006; Milad et al., 2007b). Notably, gamma ER scores at the whole-brain level were not only significant for the vmPFC but also for the hippocampus, indicating that both regions may be relevant for fear extinction recall. Both regions project to the amygdala (Milad et al., 2007b; Beckmann et al., 2009; Orsini et al., 2011) and may reduce amygdala-driven fear expression by activating inhibitory regions of the amygdala during extinction recall (McDonald, 1998; Orsini et al., 2011; Milad and Quirk, 2012). Moreover, the hippocampus may confer vmPFC-dependent fear extinction recall by coding for contextual information (Delamater, 2004; Kalisch et al., 2006) or directly gate fear expression after extinction by inhibiting fear signaling in AMC or rodent prelimbic cortex (Sotres-Bayon et al., 2012). Together, prior rodent and fMRI studies are thus consistent with the current finding of hippocampal gamma alterations during fear extinction recall, which, however, should be replicated in future EEG studies on hippocampal fear extinction recall mechanisms.
Further consistent with a role of vmPFC in fear reduction, participants who showed lower SCR responses to extinguished versus nonextinguished CS+ were characterized by altered vmPFC gamma activity to the extinguished CSs, whereas this effect was absent in participants who failed to show reduced SCR to the CS + E versus CS + N. This finding converges with human (Milad et al., 2007b) and animal (Milad and Quirk, 2002; Burgos-Robles et al., 2007; Santini et al., 2008) studies suggesting that difficulties in successful fear extinction recall may be related to blunted vmPFC/infralimbic cortex recruitment. By further probing vmPFC EEG or fMRI activity in anxiety disorders (Milad et al., 2009), these findings might ultimately be related to moderators of long-term exposure therapy outcome.
The present study has some limitations. First, more trials than in most fear conditioning/extinction studies were used to achieve sufficient signal-to-noise ratio for EEG (but see Flor et al., 1996; Wessa and Flor, 2007). This may have facilitated habituation to the US and CS throughout the Acquisition, Extinction, and Recall phases. Moreover, because the US was no longer presented during the Recall phase, further within-session extinction presumably occurred on day 2. However, ongoing habituation and extinction would have reduced conditioning- and/or extinction-related stimulus differences. Because the hypothesized contingency × extinction interactions were nevertheless significant, the reported effects seem particularly strong. As a further limitation, amygdala activity cannot be probed using EEG and was therefore not investigated in the present study. In addition, because of the limited spatial resolution of EEG and LORETA, the current density extracted from the AMC and vmPFC ROIs was likely influenced by activity from neighboring regions. To more precisely localize the sources of the current AMC theta and vmPFC gamma effects and to test how they relate to amygdala activity, future studies that combine the present approach with fMRI will be needed. Finally, the signal of frontal gamma is relatively weak and can be affected by ocular activity to complex visual stimuli or muscular activity to noise bursts. Because the US was never presented during the Recall Test and because visual stimuli were counterbalanced across conditions, it is unlikely that such artifacts influenced the present findings. However, effects for gamma activity were weaker than for theta activity and did not emerge at frontopolar electrodes but only in the LORETA analyses. Although the precise localization of the gamma effect to the vmPFC supports its validity, future replications of the present effects in the gamma range are warranted.
Despite these limitations, the present study provides novel evidence that electrophysiological correlates of human fear conditioning and extinction recall can be probed noninvasively using EEG. More importantly, the present EEG findings clarify that human recall of conditioned and extinguished fear 24 h after a fear conditioning and extinction procedure involves the AMC and vmPFC, respectively, which contribute to integrate prior animal and fMRI studies. The present results further indicate that AMC-associated fear conditioning recall relates to theta oscillations, whereas vmPFC-associated extinction recall is linked to gamma activity. Future fear extinction studies may further exploit the advantages of EEG, which include the following: (1) high temporal resolution, which allows to adequately capture fear-related oscillatory processes and cross-region interactions; (2) economic data collection of large samples required for molecular genetic research of anxiety risk factors; and (3) data collection in real life environments, including fear exposure therapy.
Footnotes
This work was supported by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft Grant DFG MU3535/2-1 to E.M.M. and a Gustav A. Lienert stipend to E.M.M. We thank Hannah Münch, Anika Schulze, and Wiebke Schröter who collected the data as part of their theses.
Over the last 3 years, D.A.P. has received consulting fees from AstraZeneca, Pfizer, Shire, Servier, and Ono Pharma for studies unrelated to this project. The remaining authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Erik M. Mueller, Department of Clinical Psychology, Justus-Liebig University Giessen, Otto-Behaghel Str. 10F, 39354 Giessen, Germany. erik.mueller{at}psychol.uni-giessen.de