Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Journal Club

Is Feature-Based Attention Always Spatially Global during Visual Search?

Joshua J. Foster and Kirsten C.S. Adam
Journal of Neuroscience 25 June 2014, 34 (26) 8662-8664; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1616-14.2014
Joshua J. Foster
Department of Psychology, University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon 97403-1227
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Joshua J. Foster
Kirsten C.S. Adam
Department of Psychology, University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon 97403-1227
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Kirsten C.S. Adam
  • Article
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

The visual system has a limited capacity, so visual inputs must compete for representation in visual cortex. Attentional mechanisms resolve this competition by selecting a subset of behaviorally relevant information for processing on the basis of location or nonspatial features, such as color (for review, see Carrasco, 2011). One mechanism for attentional selection is modulation of neural gain such that the neuronal response to attended stimuli is amplified relative to unattended inputs (Hillyard et al., 1998). Numerous studies suggest that feature-based attention operates globally, such that neural gain for an attended feature (e.g., the color red or upward motion) is amplified across the entire visual field. For example, Treue and Martínez Trujillo (1999) demonstrated that attending to a feature (direction of motion) in one visual hemifield modulated the response of visual neurons with receptive fields in the opposite hemifield. Additional evidence for global feature-based attention has subsequently come from studies using a range of methodologies, including single-unit recordings in monkeys (for review, see Maunsell and Treue, 2006), psychophysics (Liu and Mance, 2011), functional magnetic resonance imaging (Liu et al., 2007; Serences and Boynton, 2007), and electroencephalography (EEG; Andersen et al., 2013). Although there is strong evidence that global feature-based attention occurs in most experimental settings, it is less clear whether spatial attention can narrow the scope of feature-based attention. If relevant stimuli only appear in one portion of the visual field (e.g., near fixation), it may be advantageous to restrict feature-based attention to the relevant location.

A recent study in The Journal of Neuroscience by Painter et al. (2014) examined whether task demands influence the scope of feature-based attention. Most studies of feature-based attention have used tasks in which participants monitor a single stimulus for a subtle change in a target feature (e.g., a decrement in luminance). Instead of such a simple attentional monitoring task, Painter and colleagues (2014) used a visual search task to examine the effects of feature-based attention. In this task, participants searched sequential arrays of items presented at fixation for targets with a defined feature (a specific color), while task-irrelevant stimuli flickered in the periphery. Given the privileged status of central vision in directing attention within displays (Hollingworth et al., 2001), a fixated central display with task-irrelevant peripheral stimuli may be a prime situation in which feature-based attention can be restricted to a relevant location.

In Painter and colleagues' (2014) Experiment 1, participants performed two different search tasks. During unique-feature search, each array included one item in the target color (e.g., an upright red “T”) among uniformly colored (e.g., green) distractors. During conjunction search, half of the items were in the target color and half were in a distractor color so that participants had to inspect all items in the target color to identify targets. Steady-state visual-evoked potentials (SSVEPs), measured using EEG, were used to assess whether neural responses to the attended color were enhanced globally during visual search. SSVEPs are measured as an enhanced oscillatory response at the frequency of a repetitive, flickering visual stimulus. The SSVEP response is further enhanced when the flickering stimulus is attended. This attentional enhancement is thought to reflect a relative increase in gain for the neurons representing the attended stimulus (Morgan et al., 1996). In Painter and colleagues' (2014) experiments, the central search array was surrounded by checkerboards containing both the target and distractor colors. The target and distractor colors flickered at different frequencies, allowing the authors to examine the SSVEP response to each color separately. If feature-based attention affects processing outside the search array (i.e., is spatially global), the SSVEP response to the target color in the checkerboard surround should be greater than that to the distractor color. Such evidence for global feature selection was found during conjunction search, but not during unique-feature search.

In Experiment 2, Painter and colleagues (2014) examined whether the global effect of feature-based attention seen during conjunction search was the result of a gain increase for the target color (target enhancement), a gain decrease for the distractor color (distractor suppression), or a combination of the two. Participants performed the same conjunction search as in Experiment 1, with the exception that a third color was introduced to the peripheral checkerboard. The three colors in the checkerboard flickered at different frequencies, allowing the authors to examine the SSVEP response to each color separately. Importantly, only two colors were included in the search array (one as the target color and another as the distractor color). The third color therefore provided a neutral baseline. Relative to this baseline, Painter and colleagues (2014) observed a potentiated SSVEP response to the target color, but no attenuation of the SSVEP response to the distractor color, suggesting that feature-based selection was mediated exclusively by target enhancement during conjunction search.

Painter and colleagues' (2014) experiments are among the first to test whether feature-based attention is global during visual search rather than more typical attentional monitoring tasks (see also Bichot et al., 2005). Prominent models of visual search, such as guided search theory (Wolfe, 1994), have long proposed that global feature templates direct attention to potential target features during visual search. During conjunction search in both experiments, Painter and colleagues (2014) found evidence for global enhancement of the SSVEP response to the attended feature. These results suggest that searching for a target color (in a conjunction search task) enhances the response to that color across the entire visual field, even at completely irrelevant locations. This finding provides direct evidence for the role of global feature-based attention during visual search.

Although the conjunction search condition provided strong evidence for global feature-based attention, Painter and colleagues' (2014) found no modulated SSVEP responses to the peripheral checkerboard during unique-feature search. This finding could suggest that feature-based attention can be restricted to the central search array during unique-feature search. However, a second possibility is that participants simply did not use a feature-search strategy (i.e., they did not search for the red item when the target color was red). Instead, participants may have searched for the item that was the “odd color out” in the unique-feature search condition. This strategy, known as singleton search, may be used during visual search when the target is defined by a single unique feature (e.g., the odd color out or the odd shape out; Bacon and Egeth, 1994). If participants used a singleton-search strategy, one would expect that the feature-specific response to the target color would not be enhanced.

Painter and colleagues (2014) acknowledged that singleton search is the most parsimonious explanation for the lack of global feature enhancement during unique-feature search. Nevertheless, they ultimately concluded that their results suggest an interaction between spatial attention and feature-based attention when search demands vary. In other words, feature-based attention is global in some circumstances but is spatially restricted in others. However, this conclusion is undermined by the possibility that participants were not using feature-based attention at all during unique-feature search. Given that participants may have adopted a singleton search strategy in the unique-feature search condition, future research should test whether feature-based attention is global during single-feature search when the possibility of singleton search is precluded. Straightforward modifications of Painter and colleagues' (2014) paradigm should prove useful in this respect. For example, presenting a color-defined target item among heterogeneous-colored distractor items would prevent participants from using a singleton-search strategy and promote search for the target color itself.

Painter and colleagues' (2014) conclusion that feature-based attention can be restricted to a relevant location under certain circumstances is also difficult to reconcile with another recent SSVEP study. Andersen and colleagues (2013) found that participants could not restrict feature-based attention to a visual hemifield when instructed to do so, even though failure to restrict feature-based attention was detrimental to task performance. In a critical condition, subjects were instructed to attend a color in one hemifield and to ignore the same color in the opposite hemifield. The SSVEP response to the attended color was not greater in the relevant hemifield than in the ignored hemifield, indicating that subjects were unable to restrict feature-based attention to the relevant hemifield. In light of these results, the most likely explanation of the lack of global feature enhancement during Painter and colleagues' (2014) unique-feature search is the use of a singleton-search strategy. A singleton search interpretation is consistent both with the visual search literature and with the existing studies on the obligatory global nature of feature-based attention.

Despite ambiguity in the interpretation of the unique-feature search results, the global effect of feature-based attention during conjunction search reported by Painter and colleagues (2014) sheds light on how feature-based attention guides visual search. By recording SSVEPs elicited by checkerboards outside the search array during a visual search task, Painter and colleagues (2014) provide compelling new evidence for models of visual search, which assert that global feature templates direct attention to potential targets. The novel application of SSVEPs to a visual search task highlights the contribution of the basic mechanism of feature-based attention to visual search.

Footnotes

  • Editor's Note: These short, critical reviews of recent papers in the Journal, written exclusively by graduate students or postdoctoral fellows, are intended to summarize the important findings of the paper and provide additional insight and commentary. For more information on the format and purpose of the Journal Club, please see http://www.jneurosci.org/misc/ifa_features.shtml.

  • J.J.F. and K.C.S.A. are supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant R01-MH087214 awarded to Edward Awh and Edward K. Vogel. J.J.F. is also supported by a Fulbright Science and Innovation Graduate Award awarded by Fulbright New Zealand.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to either Joshua J. Foster or Kirsten C. S. Adam, Department of Psychology, 1227 University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon 97403-1227. jfoster2{at}uoregon.edu or kadam{at}uoregon.edu

References

  1. ↵
    1. Andersen SK,
    2. Hillyard SA,
    3. Müller MM
    (2013) Global facilitation of attended features is obligatory and restricts divided attention. J Neurosci 33:18200–18207, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1913-13.2013, pmid:24227728.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    1. Bacon WF,
    2. Egeth HE
    (1994) Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Percept Psychophys 55:485–496, doi:10.3758/BF03205306, pmid:8008550.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Bichot NP,
    2. Rossi AF,
    3. Desimone R
    (2005) Parallel and serial neural mechanisms for visual search in macaque area V4. Science 308:529–534, doi:10.1126/science.1109676, pmid:15845848.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Carrasco M
    (2011) Visual attention: the past 25 years. Vision Res 51:1484–1525, doi:10.1016/j.visres.2011.04.012, pmid:21549742.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Hillyard SA,
    2. Vogel EK,
    3. Luck SJ
    (1998) Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 353:1257–1270, doi:10.1098/rstb.1998.0281, pmid:9770220.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    1. Hollingworth A,
    2. Schrock G,
    3. Henderson JM
    (2001) Change detection in the flicker paradigm: the role of fixation position within the scene. Mem Cognit 29:296–304, doi:10.3758/BF03194923, pmid:11352212.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Liu T,
    2. Mance I
    (2011) Constant spread of feature-based attention across the visual field. Vision Res 51:26–33, doi:10.1016/j.visres.2010.09.023, pmid:20887745.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Liu T,
    2. Larsson J,
    3. Carrasco M
    (2007) Feature-based attention modulates orientation-selective responses in human visual cortex. Neuron 55:313–323, doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.030, pmid:17640531.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Maunsell JH,
    2. Treue S
    (2006) Feature-based attention in visual cortex. Trends Neurosci 29:317–322, doi:10.1016/j.tins.2006.04.001, pmid:16697058.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Morgan ST,
    2. Hansen JC,
    3. Hillyard SA
    (1996) Selective attention to stimulus location modulates the steady-state visual evoked potential. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 93:4770–4774, doi:10.1073/pnas.93.10.4770, pmid:8643478.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    1. Painter DR,
    2. Dux PE,
    3. Travis SL,
    4. Mattingley JB
    (2014) Neural responses to target features outside a search array are enhanced during conjunction but not unique-feature search. J Neurosci 34:3390–3401, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3630-13.2014, pmid:24573295.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. ↵
    1. Serences JT,
    2. Boynton GM
    (2007) Feature-based attentional modulations in the absence of direct visual stimulation. Neuron 55:301–312, doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.015, pmid:17640530.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Treue S,
    2. Martínez Trujillo JC
    (1999) Feature-based attention influences motion processing gain in macaque visual cortex. Nature 399:575–579, doi:10.1038/21176, pmid:10376597.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Wolfe JM
    (1994) Guided search 2.0: a revised model of visual search. Psychon Bull Rev 1:202–238, doi:10.3758/BF03200774, pmid:24203471.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 34 (26)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 34, Issue 26
25 Jun 2014
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Is Feature-Based Attention Always Spatially Global during Visual Search?
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Is Feature-Based Attention Always Spatially Global during Visual Search?
Joshua J. Foster, Kirsten C.S. Adam
Journal of Neuroscience 25 June 2014, 34 (26) 8662-8664; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1616-14.2014

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Is Feature-Based Attention Always Spatially Global during Visual Search?
Joshua J. Foster, Kirsten C.S. Adam
Journal of Neuroscience 25 June 2014, 34 (26) 8662-8664; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1616-14.2014
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Parabrachial Projections to PAG-RVM Axis May Promote Placebo Hypoalgesia and Nocebo Hyperalgesia
  • Differential Dopamine Dynamics in Adolescents and Adults
  • Complementing Neuroregeneration: Deciphering the Role of Neuro-Immune Interactions in CNS Repair
Show more Journal Club
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2022 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.