Abstract
The ability to associate the consumption of a taste with its positive or negative consequences is fundamental to survival and influences the behavior of species ranging from invertebrate to human. As a result, for both research and clinical reasons, there has been a great effort to understand the neuronal circuits, as well as the cellular and molecular mechanisms, underlying taste learning. From a neuroanatomical perspective, the contributions of the cortex and amygdala are well documented; however, the literature is riddled with conflicting results regarding the role of the hippocampus in different facets of taste learning. Here, we use conditional genetics in mice to block NMDA receptor-dependent plasticity individually in each of the three major hippocampal subfields, CA1, CA3, and the dentate gyrus, via deletion of the NR1 subunit. Across the CA1, CA3, and dentate gyrus NR1 knock-out lines, we uncover a pattern of differential deficits that establish the dispensability of hippocampal plasticity in incidental taste learning, the requirement of CA1 plasticity for associative taste learning, and a specific requirement for plasticity in the dentate gyrus when there is a long temporal gap between the taste and its outcome. Together, these data establish that the hippocampus is involved in associative taste learning and suggest an episodic component to this type of memory.
Introduction
Associative taste learning, in which animals, including humans, associate a specific tastant consumed with its postingestive consequences, nausea or safety, is robust and long lasting. This adaptive learning can also prove detrimental, as evidenced by the frequency at which patients receiving chemotherapy struggle with the powerful association between post-treatment sickness and the taste of pretreatment meals (for review, see Reilly and Bornovalova, 2005). In the last 60 years, many of the neural circuits and structures underlying this type of learning have been identified. The gustatory cortex, the amygdala, and the parabrachial nucleus of the pons are all critical; however, the role of the hippocampus in taste learning and memory is still subject to debate (Rosenblum et al., 1993; Yamamoto et al., 1995; Yefet et al., 2006; Ding et al., 2008; Elkobi et al., 2008; Barki-Harrington et al., 2009; Gal-Ben-Ari and Rosenblum, 2012; Gildish et al., 2012; Piette et al., 2012; Inberg et al., 2013; Stern et al., 2013). While biochemical analyses observe novel taste learning-dependent changes in protein expression and phosphorylation in the hippocampus, behavioral studies have reported a wide array of phenotypes following hippocampal damage. Previous studies have reported impaired, normal, or even enhanced conditioned taste aversion (CTA), an associative form of taste learning and memory, following pharmacologically mediated inactivation or hippocampal lesions (Murphy and Brown, 1974; Miller et al., 1975, 1986; Bakner et al., 1991; Reilly et al., 1993; Purves et al., 1995; Yamamoto et al., 1995; Stone et al., 2005; De la Cruz et al., 2008; Ding et al., 2008; Koh et al., 2009; Garcia-Delatorre et al., 2010). However, despite these inconsistencies, current literature continues to suggest that taste learning and memory is considered to be “hippocampus-independent,” even in its associative form (Ding et al., 2008; Ballarini et al., 2009).
To reconcile these differences and better understand the hippocampal contribution to taste learning, we used a conditional genetic approach to allow the function of specific subregions of the hippocampus to be spatially and temporally regulated. Previous behavioral and electrophysiological studies using CA1-, CA3-, and DG-specific NMDAR subunit NR1 knock-out mice (CA1-NR1 KO; CA3-NR1 KO; DG-NR1 KO; Tsien et al., 1996; Nakazawa et al., 2002; McHugh et al., 2007) have delineated the distinct roles of the different regions of the hippocampus in spatial and contextual learning (McHugh et al., 1996, 2007; Tsien et al., 1996; Nakazawa et al., 2002). Using these well characterized transgenic lines as variants of hippocampal circuit function, we investigated the role of the hippocampus in several forms and stages of taste learning and memory.
Materials and Methods
Mice.
CA1-NR1 (Tg(Camk2a-cre)T29-1Stl; Grin1tm2Stl/Grin1tm2Stl mouse; Research Resource Identifier (RRID): MGI_MGI:3581524), CA3-NR1 (C57BL/6-Tg(Grik4-cre)G32-4Stl/J mouse; RRID: IMSR_JAX:006474 crossed with Grin1tm2Stl/Grin1tm2Stl mouse; RRID: MGI_MGI:2175051), and DG-NR1 KO (Grin1tm2Stl/Grin1tm2Stl; Tg(Pomc-cre)1Stl mouse; RRID: MGI_MGI:4362041) mice were bred and genotyped as previously described (Tsien et al., 1996; Nakazawa et al., 2002; McHugh et al., 2007). Male KO mice from each line were used for experiments at the following ages: CA1-NR1 KO, 6–12 weeks; CA3-NR1 KO, 18–25 weeks; DG-NR1 KO, 20–40 weeks. Male flNR1 homozygous littermates from all three lines were used as age-matched controls (Grin1tm2Stl/Grin1tm2Stl mouse; RRID: MGI_MGI:2175051). c-fos promoter activity was assessed using mice derived from the TetTag mice (B6;DBA-Tg(Fos-tTA,Fos-EGFP)1Mmay/J mouse; RRID: IMSR_JAX:008344; Reijmers et al., 2007) that carried the c-Fos promoter driving a 2 h half-life EGFP (c-fos-shEGFP) and were bred and genotyped as previously described. Arc expression experiments were performed in C57BL/6 male mice bred in-house (12–16 weeks old). All mice were bred and the experiments were performed at the RIKEN Brain Science Institute (Wako-Shi, Saitama, Japan). They were maintained in groups, in a humidity- and temperature-controlled environment, under a 12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 8:00 A.M.) with ad libitum access to food and water. Mice were individually housed before the start of the experiments. Experiments were conducted during the light phase. Efforts were taken to minimize the number of mice used. All the experiments were performed in the home cages of mice by an experimenter blind to the genotypes. All protocols were approved by the RIKEN Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
Conditioned taste aversion.
Mice were habituated to get their daily water ration once a day for 20 min from two pipettes, each containing 5 ml of water, for 3 d. On the fourth (conditioning) day, they were allowed to drink 0.5% sodium saccharin (Sigma) solution from similar pipettes for 20 min, and 0, 40, 100, or 220 min later were injected with lithium chloride [LiCl, a gastric malaise inducer; 0.14 m; 2% body weight (b.w.)] for immediate, 1 h, 2 h, or 4 h intertime interval (ITI)-CTA training, respectively. They were given 20 min access to water on days 5 and 6. On day 7, mice were subjected to a multiple-choice test situation involving two pipettes with 5 ml each of conditioned taste solution (saccharin) and two with 5 ml each of water. The order of the pipettes was counterbalanced and the volume of fluid consumed from each tube was recorded. The behavioral data are expressed in terms of aversion index—the volume of water consumed divided by the total fluid consumed (ml water/ml water plus ml taste).
For reinstatement, 8 d after the last extinction test, the mice were injected with LiCl (0.14 m; 2% b.w.) and 24 h later were subjected to the same multiple-choice tests administered on day 7.
Latent inhibition.
The mice were trained for 3 d to get their daily water ration once a day for 20 min from two pipettes, each containing 5 ml of water. On the preexposure day, the mice were exposed to sodium saccharin solutions for 20 min, in two 5 ml pipettes. The next day, mice were again allowed to get their daily water ration for 20 min. On the conditioning day, they were again allowed to drink the saccharin solution from similar pipettes for 20 min, and 40 min later were injected with LiCl (0.14 m; 2% b.w.). Testing and data analysis were conducted in a manner similar to CTA.
Taste preference, attenuation of neophobia, and long-term safe taste memory.
Mice were trained to drink from pipettes for 20 min per day for 2 d, and on the third day, they underwent multiple-choice tests involving two pipettes with 5 ml each of novel sweet (0.5% sodium saccharin) taste solution or bitter (0.04% quinine) taste solution and two pipettes with 5 ml each of water. For attenuation of taste neophobia, the mice underwent similar multiple-choice tests for the next 2 d. Mice were given water ad libitum for the next 14 d, and on the 15th day they were water restricted. They underwent multiple-choice tests on the 16th day to test long-term safe taste memory. The behavioral data are expressed in terms of preference index (ml taste/ml water plus ml taste).
LiCl-induced gut pain.
An automated tracking system, ANY-maze (Stoelting) was used to see if there was any difference in the exploratory behavior following LiCl injection, to indirectly measure the LiCl-induced gut pain sensation (Ding et al., 2008). A standard rodent housing cage was used as the chamber (floor area of 140 sq in; 11.25 inches wide × 15.5 inches deep × 7.6 inches high). The mice were placed in the chamber, and 10 min after the initial exploration, they were injected with 0.14 m LiCl, 2% b.w. They were monitored using a CCD camera connected to the ANY-maze program. The length they traveled was transferred to MS Excel and analyzed.
In situ hybridization.
For taste learning and tissue preparation, mice (C57BL/6J) were trained to drink from pipettes for 10 min per day for 3 d, and on the fourth day they received either water (control) or novel taste (saccharin) for 10 min. They were killed 3–5 min later. Brains were flash frozen and transferred to −80°C temperature until further processed.
Fluorescence in situ hybridization and cell counting for the immediate early gene Arc were performed according to a previously described protocol (Wintzer et al., 2014).
To verify the restriction of the NR1 gene deletion in the CA1-NR1 KO, CA3-NR1 KO, and DG-NR1 KO mice, we conducted in situ hybridization as previously described on two mice of each genotype at the same ages used for behavioral testing (Wintzer et al., 2014) using an NR1 probe (FANTOM clone 4732488C07) labeled with fluorescein. Fluorescent images (5×) of the resulting sections were collected on a Leica DM6000B epifluorescence microscope such that each contained one hemisphere of the entire dorsal hippocampus and overlaying cortex. An experimenter blind to the genotype of the animals processed the images for quantification (n = 3–4 per mouse) by first converting the color image to grayscale and subsequently measuring pixel density in four regions: (1) layer IV of the cortex, (2) CA1 and (3) CA3 pyramidal cell layers, and (4) the DG granule cell layer. Two independent measurements were made in each region in each section and averaged. All hippocampal regional values were then normalized by dividing the average cortical density, resulting in a single normalized value for each subregion in each section.
c-fos promoter activity.
c-fos-shEFGP mice were trained to drink from pipettes for 10 min per day for 3 d, and on the fourth day they received either water (control) or novel taste (saccharin) for 10 min. Two hours later, they were perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde, and the brain was postfixed with the same fixative for 72 h at 4°C. Brain slices (50 μm) were washed three times with PBS, mounted on slides, and coverslipped with Vectashield mounting medium containing DAPI (Vector Laboratories) for nuclear counterstaining. Quantification of the EGFP signal was performed using ImageJ software by an experimenter blind to the treatment groups.
Statistical analysis.
All grouped data are expressed as means ± SEM. Data analysis was performed with SPSS version 19 or GraphPad Prism (version 5.04 for Windows). Statistical analyses were conducted using two-tailed paired or independent-sample Student's t test, Pearson's correlation, one-way ANOVA, two-way ANOVA, multivariate ANOVA (MANOVA), and repeated-measures ANOVA, as appropriate, with Dunnett's post hoc test (when one group served as a control) or Bonferroni's post hoc test. Significance for all analyses was defined as p < 0.05.
Results
To examine the role of hippocampal plasticity in taste learning, we first confirmed the layer-specific deletion of NR1 (Grin1) gene by in situ hybridization in the conditional knock-out mice lines (Fig. 1A). Consistent with previous reports (Tsien et al., 1996; Nakazawa et al., 2002; McHugh et al., 2007), CA1-, CA3-, and DG-NR1 KOs showed clear deletion of NR1 gene restricted to the appropriate layer of the hippocampus with a faint in situ signal remaining in the deleted subfields due to the expression of the NR1 message in interneurons located in the cell body layer (Fig. 1B; two-way ANOVA: NR1 KO lines × NR1 expression in hippocampus subregion interaction, F(6,63) = 42.57, p < 0.0001; Bonferroni post hoc test, CA1-NR1 KOs: CA1 p < 0.0001, CA3 p > 0.05, DG p > 0.05; CA3-NR1 KOs: CA1 p > 0.05, CA3 p < 0.0001, DG p > 0.05; DG-NR1 KOs: CA1 p > 0.05, CA3 p > 0.05, DG p < 0.0001; 2 mice for each genotype, n = 6–7 images per group).
Incidental taste learning
Mice spontaneously exhibit taste neophobia, the reluctance to consume a novel taste due to a lack of knowledge regarding its postingestive consequences. This behavior is a vitally important feeding activity, as it increases the probability of survival by promoting avoidance of novel, and perhaps fatal, tastes (Reilly and Bornovalova, 2005). As outlined in Figure 1C, we conducted a series of behavioral experiments to test whether NMDA receptor-dependent hippocampal plasticity is required for taste neophobia. CA1-NR1, CA3-NR1, and DG-NR1 KO mice all exhibited normal taste neophobia to both an unpalatable bitter taste (quinine; Fig. 1D; control mice, n = 7; CA1-NR1 KOs, n = 6; CA3-NR1 KOs, n = 7; DG-NR1 KOs, n = 7; one-way ANOVA, F(3,24) = 0.887, p = 0.462) and a palatable sweet taste (saccharin; Fig. 1E; n = 5 mice per group, one-way ANOVA, F(3,16) = 0.592, p = 0.629). Taste neophobia is modified by experience; the sampling of a taste stimulus without aversive consequences leads to an establishment of a “safe taste” memory trace which favors an increase in the consumption of the taste when encountered in the future, a phenomenon called attenuation of neophobia. Therefore, we subjected CA1-NR1, CA3-NR1, and DG-NR1 KOs to experimental attenuation of neophobia tests and again found their behavior no different from controls (Fig. 1F; mixed-model repeated-measures ANOVA: there was a main effect of test, F(2,15) = 123.514, p < 0.0001, but there was no interaction between tests and genotypes, F(6,32) = 0.297, p = 0.934). Moreover, CA1-, CA3-, and DG-NR1 KOs also exhibited normal safe taste memory when tested 16 d later (Fig. 1G; n = 5 mice per group, one-way ANOVA, F(3,16) = 0.901, p = 0.463), indicating that hippocampal plasticity is not critical for the acquisition or consolidation of incidental taste memory.
To further explore hippocampal activation during sensory learning, we examined whether incidental taste learning leads to the activation of the immediate early genes Arc and c-fos in the individual subfields (Fig. 2A,C). Consistent with our behavioral results, we did not find any significant difference in nuclear Arc mRNA expression (Fig. 2B) in the CA1 or CA3 between the water and novel taste learning groups (n = 11 mice per group, unpaired t test; CA1, t20 = −0.102, p = 0.919; CA3, t20 = 0.672, p = 0.509). We did not include the DG subregion in this analysis due to the sparse signal in the DG (2–3% of cells express arc), which makes statistically significant observations difficult and prone to noise. To examine DG activation, we used a complementary approach, a transgenic mouse expressing EGFP under the control of the c-Fos promoter (Reijmers et al., 2007). Again, we found no significant difference in c-Fos promoter-driven EGFP expression (Fig. 2D; Koh and Bernstein, 2005) in the CA3 or DG subregions of hippocampi of mice exposed to water and those exposed to a novel taste (unpaired t test; CA3, t6 = 0.919, p = 0.394; DG, t6 = −0.971, p = 0.369).
Associative taste learning and memory
We next addressed the role of the hippocampus in associative taste learning and memory using the standard CTA paradigm (Fig. 3A). In CTA, a novel and palatable taste (conditioned stimulus, CS) becomes aversive following its association with gastric discomfort (unconditioned stimulus, US). The control group in these experiments included age-matched male flNR1 homozygous littermates from all three mutant lines; therefore, we first examined whether there was any difference in CTA strength between the three types of littermates and found no significant effect (n = 10 flNR1 control littermates from each of the three lines; one-way ANOVA, F(2,27) = 1.467, p = 0.248) and thus combined these results to represent a single control group. Compared with these controls, we observed a significant attenuation of CTA memory in CA1-NR1 KO mice, but not in the CA3-NR1 or DG- NR1 KO lines (Fig. 3B; controls, n = 30; CA1-NR1 KOs, n = 24; CA3-NR1 KOs, n = 14; DG-NR1 KOs, n = 18; one-way ANOVA, F(3,82) = 6.767, p = 0.0003; Bonferroni's post hoc test, compared with control group: CA1-NR1 KOs, p = 0.004; CA3-NR1 KOs, p = 1.0; DG-NR1 KOs, p = 0.163; compared with CA1-NR1 KOs: CA3-NR1 KOs, p = 0.002; DG-NR1 KOs, p = 1.0; compared with DG-NR1 KOs: CA3-NR1 KOs, p = 0.058). The attenuation of CTA memory in CA1-NR1 KOs could not be explained by differences in the taste perception, neophobic responses, or sensitivity to the US, as the CA1-NR1 KO mice, similar to CA3-NR1 KOs and DG-NR1 KOs, exhibited normal responses to both taste (Fig. 1D–G) and LiCl-induced suppression of locomotory behavior [gut pain; Fig. 3F; control plus NR1 KO lines × LiCl-induced locomotion (10 min before and 10 min after LiCl injection) interaction, MANOVA, Pillai's trace, F(6,44) = 2.062, p = 0.077; there was also no interaction between groups (control and NR1 KO lines) before LiCl injection, F(3,22) = 2.754, p = 0.067, or after LiCl injection, F(3,22) = 1.755, p = 0.185; paired t test between 10 min before and 10 min after LiCl: controls, t6 = 29.39, p < 0.0001; CA1-NR1 KOs, t6 = 9.675, p < 0.0001; CA3-NR1 KOs, t6 = 9.484, p < 0.0001; DG-NR1 KOs, t4 = 6.537, p = 0.003]. Further, there were no differences in the total fluid consumption among all groups of mice; thus, this factor also cannot account for any CTA memory attenuation in the CA1-NR1 KOs [control, 1.57 ± 0.05 ml; CA1, 1.52 ± 0.05 ml; CA3, 1.82 ± 0.11 ml; DG,1.63 ± 0.06 ml; there was a general interaction—one-way ANOVA, F(3,82) = 2.991, p = 0.036; however, no group differed significantly from others, compared with controls (n = 30): CA1-NR1 KOs (n = 24) p = 1.0, CA3-NR1 KOs (n = 14) p = 0.09, DG-NR1 KOs (n = 18) p = 1.0].
Following the initial CTA test, the conditioned mice were reexposed to the conditioned taste daily for the next 3 d to study extinction of this association. Although previous studies have found accelerated CTA extinction following hippocampal damage (Yamamoto et al., 1995; Garcia-Delatorre et al., 2010), in our hands all the mutant strains exhibited extinction curves identical to the controls (Fig. 3C; mixed-model repeated-measures ANOVA: there was an interaction between tests, F(3,21) = 44.071, p < 0.0001; however, there was no interaction between tests and genotypes, F(9,69) = 0.259, p = 0.983). To explore this further, following extinction, we again presented the mice with the US (LiCl), this time in the absence of any taste, to assess reinstatement of the initially learned association. When we analyzed the group responses, they revealed a significant effect of genotype (Fig. 3D), and further post hoc analysis revealed that the CA1-NR1 KO mice again exhibited significantly less aversion to the conditioned taste than controls (one-way ANOVA, F(3,26) = 4.982, p = 0.008; Dunnett's post hoc test, compared with controls: CA1-NR1 KOs, p = 0.036; CA3-NR1 KOs, p = 0.88; DG-NR1 KOs, p = 0.11). Reflecting their initial slightly lower aversion following CTA, the DG-NR1 KOs also showed less aversion, albeit not significantly lower than control mice, following reinstatement. To examine this more carefully, we compared the aversion expressed by individual mice during the last extinction test (T4 in Fig. 3C) with their aversion following reinstatement. Similar to what we observed in the group averages, the CA1-NR1 KO mice demonstrated no significant increase in aversion following the unpaired US, but in this analysis, neither did the DG-NR1 KOs, whereas control and CA3-NR1 KO mice exhibited robust reinstatement (Fig. 3E; control mice, t6 = −3.207, p = 0.018; CA1-NR1 KOs, t5 = −0.155, p = 0.883; CA3-NR1 KOs, t6 = −5.874, p = 0.001; and DG-NR1 KOs, t6 = −1.702, p = 0.140). Consistent with the idea that the CTA extinction is not unlearning (Berman and Dudai, 2001; Berman et al., 2003) our reinstatement results demonstrate that the presentation of US 7 d after extinction leads to the robust reappearance of the aversive memory in control mice and further suggest that the loss of NMDA receptor-dependent plasticity in CA1 eliminated the original associative trace, whereas NR loss in the DG may lead to an initially weaker or more modifiable memory.
Temporal modulation of CTA memory
A critical parameter in any associative learning paradigm is the interval (ITI) or trace between the presentations of the CS and US. One interesting property of CTA is that it can be established with CS–US intervals measured in hours which, while reasonable from physiological, ecological, and evolutionary perspectives, is unique among traditional associative learning paradigms (Koh et al., 2009). In mice, both Pavlovian trace fear conditioning and trace eye-blink conditioning engage plasticity in the hippocampus, but with ITIs on the timescales of tens of seconds and hundreds of milliseconds, respectively (McEchron and Disterhoft, 1999; Weitemier and Ryabinin, 2004; Kitamura et al., 2014). Further, recent lesion work has suggested that CTA performed with an interval of 3 h between the CS and US is sensitive to hippocampal damage (Koh et al., 2009). To examine whether the requirements for hippocampal NR-dependent plasticity changed with differing ITIs in the CTA paradigm, we varied the time between taste and LiCl injection. We subjected the two lines with quantitatively lower CTA under our standard protocol, the CA1-NR1 KOs and the DG-NR1 KOs (Fig. 3B), to “immediate CTA” in which the mice were injected with LiCl immediately after consuming the novel taste to assess whether (1) the lower CTA memory that was observed after regular 1 h ITI-CTA conditioning (which typically involves a 40 min trace between taste and malaise) was related to trace length or (2) whether CA1 is required for the taste-malaise association independent of the CS–US interval. As shown in Figure 4A, CA1-NR1 KOs exhibited a clear and significant attenuation of CTA memory after immediate CTA training (one-way ANOVA, F(2,27) = 2.49, p < 0.05; control mice, n = 13, Dunnett's post hoc test, compared with controls: CA1-NR1 KOs, n = 7, p = 0.044; DG-NR1 KOs, n = 8, p = 0.679), suggesting that the CA1 NMDA receptors are always required for CTA, even without a trace interval. Conversely, whereas DG-NR1 KOs exhibited a trend toward impairment following regular 1 h ITI-CTA conditioning (Fig. 3B; p = 0.142; also reflected in reinstatement, Fig. 3D,E), they did not show any deficits in CTA memory following immediate CTA training. Therefore, we next subjected the DG-NR1 KOs, along with the CA3-NR1 KOs, to CTA training with trace intervals of either 2 or 4 h (2 h ITI-CTA or 4 h ITI-CTA), to examine whether NMDA receptor in these regions is required at longer intervals. As expected, in the control, CA3-NR1, and DG-NR1 KO mice, the ability to relate the taste stimulus with bodily status (“taste-value”) weakened with increasing ITI, as demonstrated by the strong negative correlation between the ITI and memory in all three groups (Fig. 4D; controls, Pearson's r = −0.533, p < 0.0001; CA3-NR1 KOs, Pearson's r = −0.544, p = 0.002; DG-NR1-KOs, Pearson's r = −0.654, p < 0.0001). Interestingly, compared with the controls, we observed a significant CTA memory impairment in DG-NR1 KOs when we increased the time between CS and US to 2 or 4 h (Fig. 4B,C; 2 h ITI-CTA, one-way ANOVA, F(2,29) = 7.366, p = 0.003; control mice, n = 16, Dunnett's post hoc test, compared with controls: DG-NR1 KOs, n = 8, p = 0.012; CA3-NR1 KOs, n = 8, p = 0.342; 4 h ITI-CTA, one-way ANOVA, F(2,29) = 6.430, p = 0.005, control mice. n = 15, Dunnett's post hoc test, compared with controls: DG-NR1 KOs, n = 8, p = 0.012; CA3-NR1 KOs, n = 8, p = 0.648). These data distinguish the DG-NR1 KO mice from the CA1-NR1 KO mice, and this progressive phenotype, which becomes significant at longer trace intervals, suggests that NMDA receptor-dependent plasticity in the DG plays a critical role in the modulation of temporal context in CTA (Fig. 4D; two-way ANOVA comparing controls and DG-NR1 KOs revealed that there was a significant effect of trace interval, F(3,109) = 21.097, p < 0.0001 and genotype, F(1,109) = 14.967, p = 0.0001, but no significant interaction F(3,109) = 1.944, p = 0.113). Further, the actual magnitude of the difference of the mean aversion index between the control and DG-NR1 KO mice highlights the progressive nature of DG-NR1 KOs phenotype: immediate CTA, −0.002 (0.19% of the control mean), 1 h ITI-CTA, −0.091 (10.58% of the control mean), 2 h ITI-CTA, −0.105 (13.04% of the control mean), 4 h ITI-CTA: −0.180 (25.93% of the control mean). Although CA3-NR1 KOs did not show any defects, they did exhibit a statistically insignificant trend toward higher aversive memory compared with control mice across all the ITI-CTA conditions examined (Fig. 4D). Therefore, in our last CTA experiment, we subjected CA3-NR1 KO mice to weak CTA training (0.05 m LiCl; a weaker US) to look for enhanced CTA learning that may have been masked in our earlier experiments by the ceiling effect of strong conditioning. However, we observed no differences between mutant and control mice under this protocol (Fig. 4E; n = 11 mice per group, unpaired t test, t20 = 0.472, p = 0.642), indicating that NMDA receptor-dependent plasticity in CA3 is not required for associative CTA memory formation.
Latent inhibition of CTA
Finally, we tested whether NMDA receptor-dependent synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus is required for another form of taste memory, latent inhibition (LI; Fig. 5A). In LI, preexposure of a novel taste stimulus diminishes the efficacy of the same taste to serve as a conditioned stimulus in a subsequent associative learning trial; a positive tag of the learned taste is established and thus the weaker association leads to a weaker aversive memory formation (Fig. 5B; Rosenblum et al., 1993; Stone et al., 2005; Stern et al., 2013; CTA, n = 6; LI, n = 8; unpaired t test, t12 = 3.654, p = 0.003). CA1-NR1 KOs exhibited weaker aversive memory compared with control mice in LI of CTA (Fig. 5C; one-way ANOVA, F(3,114) = 4.210, p = 0.007; control mice, n = 44; Dunnett's post hoc test, compared with controls: CA1-NR1 KOs, n = 21, p = 0.009; CA3-NR1 KOs, n = 30, p = 0.891; and DG-NR1 KOs, n = 23, p = 0.9). There are two possible interpretations that can be made for the reduced aversive memory exhibited by CA1-NR1 KOs in LI. First, it may be due to the increased safety learning/memory regarding the taste because of the first preexposure, and hence this result can be seen as an enhanced LI in CA1-NR1 KOs. However, given that CA1-NR1 KOs clearly exhibited an attenuation of CTA memory in both immediate CTA and 1 h ITI-CTA training, but normal attenuation of neophobia (Fig. 1E), it is very reasonable to interpret these data as indicating that the CA1-NR1 KO line has difficulty associating a taste, be it novel (CTA) or familiar (LI), with the negative outcome.
Discussion
Conditional genetics has allowed us to address how changing the quality of hippocampal information impacts behavior, and our results demonstrate that the hippocampal circuit is involved in associative, but not incidental, taste learning. The pattern of deficits we observed across three strains of mutant mice clarifies a longstanding inconsistency and suggests that the hippocampus may underlie an episodic component to associative taste learning.
We demonstrated that hippocampal plasticity is not involved in the sensory perception of taste and LiCl-induced pain (Figs. 1, 3) or in incidental taste learning (Fig. 1), but rather is engaged when learning to associate a taste and its consequence (Figs. 3B, 4A–C, 5C). Further, by contrasting the behavior of the CA1-NR1, CA3-NR1, and DG-NR1 KO mice, we can suggest specific circuit contributions the hippocampus may provide. In the CA1-NR1 KO mice, the loss of plasticity leads to unorganized hippocampal output during spatial exploration and a myriad of learning deficits (McHugh et al., 1996; Tsien et al., 1996). One possibility is that impaired CTA learning in these mice could be attributed to their contextual representational difficulties. Changes in context between CTA training and testing can attenuate memory (León et al., 2012); thus, it may be that a poor or unreliable contextual representation in the CA1-NR1 KO mice mimics this effect. Additionally, studies point to the relevance of context specificity, specifically in LI, in generating a safe or aversive taste memory trace. It has been proposed that before LI conditioning, the safe taste memory trace is enhanced with the context specificity (i.e., the more the animals spend time in their home cage, the better their safe taste memory), whereas, in contrast, the safe component of the taste is reduced with time spent in the home cage after LI conditioning (Bakner et al., 1991; De la Casa and Díaz, 2013; Molero-Chamizo, 2013). Although we conducted the LI experiment in the home cages (during preexposure, conditioning, and test), CA1-NR1 KOs exhibited increased preference to conditioned taste, suggesting that the impaired association in CA1-NR1 KOs may be attributed to the poor representation of the context.
Plasticity in the DG was necessary for learning at long (Fig. 4B,C), but not short (Fig. 4A), trace intervals, with the magnitude of the learning deficit increasing with the length of the trace (Fig. 4D). Temporal memory has been shown to be intact in CA1-NR1 and CA3-NR1 KOs (Place et al., 2012). Although this has not been examined in the DG-NR1 KOs, these mice have previously been shown to be impaired in context discrimination, which results in the mice treating novel contexts as more familiar than do controls (McHugh et al., 2007; Wintzer et al., 2014). It is possible that increasing the ITI leads to the loss of “episodic discrimination” in DG-NR1 KOs. During habituation, these mice had experienced several similar drinking “episodes” (Fig. 3A), with the key difference being that they received water, not saccharin. On the day of conditioning, after receiving the CS, the longer trace may lead to a decrease in the novelty of the episode due to its similarity with previous experiences. This would result in a decrease in the novelty of the CS and, hence, in a weaker CTA. The reinstatement deficit we observed in these mice (Fig. 3E) suggests that despite not having a significantly lower aversion following standard CTA training, the memory formed in the DG-NR1 KOs is, in fact, altered, perhaps by these same mechanisms. The DG-NR1 KOs exhibited normal LI; thus, the “safe” taste association can be formed in a manner similar to that by control mice, as this was the typical outcome of their experiences drinking water in the same manner. It may be that temporal contiguity is the major factor in longer ITI-CTA, and the defect in NR-dependent plasticity in the DG led to a decrease in the taste novelty, whereas the same NMDA receptor-dependent plasticity in the DG may not necessarily be required during the retrieval-dependent LI (which typically is 1 h ITI-CTA) conditioning. Although our results indicate the dispensable role of CA3-NMDA receptors in both the associative and nonassociative forms of taste learning and memory, our experiments did not test for any possible contribution of non-NMDA receptor-dependent plasticity mechanisms in CA3. For example, plasticity at the mossy fiber inputs from the DG to CA3 does not require NMDA receptors, but can strongly influence CA3 activity; it is hoped that future experiments with mice specifically lacking plasticity at these inputs can address these possibilities.
Previous studies addressing the role of the hippocampus in taste learning reported an array of conflicting phenotypes following hippocampal damage, including deficits, enhancements, and no change (Murphy and Brown, 1974; Miller et al., 1975, 1986; Bakner et al., 1991; Reilly et al., 1993; Purves et al., 1995; Yamamoto et al., 1995; Stone et al., 2005; De la Cruz et al., 2008; Ding et al., 2008; Koh et al., 2009; Garcia-Delatorre et al., 2010; Molero-Chamizo, 2013). It remains difficult to untangle these reports because of the varied conditions, species, and interventions used; various types of intervention (ranging from pharmacological blockade to permanent lesion), different target areas of the hippocampus (from unspecific to multiple subregions), and various types of training (trace intervals ranging from 0 to 30 min and 10 to 30 min of CS presentation) and testing (only CS—one-bottle test, multiple-choice test). However, these studies share a common aim: to understand how taste learning proceeds in the absence of all or a part of hippocampal output. This is in contrast to the current work, which examines taste learning in three unique lines of mice with intact, but altered, hippocampal processing. Nonetheless, to put our data in context, we would like to attempt a succinct review of some of these earlier papers. Anisomycin, a memory consolidation blocker, microinfused into the dorsal hippocampus (male Wistar rats; coordinates: P 3.6 mm, L ±3 mm, V 3.3 mm; mainly targeting but not restricted to CA1 region) during CTA conditioning did not alter CTA memory (De la Cruz et al., 2008). However, muscimol, an agonist of the GABAA receptor, microinfused into dorsal hippocampus (Long–Evans rats; coordinates relative to bregma: AP −3.8 mm, ML ±2.5 mm, DV −1.8 mm from dura; mainly targeting but not essentially restricted to CA1 region), resulted in enhanced CTA memory (Stone et al., 2005). Thus, it seems that the hippocampus may not be essentially involved in the consolidation of CTA memory but, instead, its output to other parts of the gustatory circuit (muscimol essentially changes circuit properties when applied locally into the brain; Piette et al., 2012) may be vital during the CS–US association. The absence of this output may actually lead to an initially stronger associative memory. These interpretations are further supported by hippocampal lesion studies which report that regardless of the magnitude of the lesion, rats demonstrated normal CTA (taste-LiCl conditioning protocol; Reilly et al., 1993; Yamamoto et al., 1995); however, lesioned rats elicited accelerated CTA extinction, and this acceleration was correlated with the magnitude of the lesion in the hippocampus (dorsal or ventral hippocampus lesion did not change the CTA extinction pattern, but the whole hippocampus lesion enhanced the extinction; Yamamoto et al., 1995). These data again suggest that CTA learning can proceed in the absence of the hippocampus; however, it is qualitatively different as assessed by its ability to withstand extinction.
Despite the clear CTA phenotypes we observed in CA1-NR1 and DG-NR1 KOs, the question remains whether the hippocampus is modulating the CS–US association or actually is associated directly with the US itself. Although our data do not distinguish these possibilities, it is tempting to speculate that the episodic memory of sampling the novel taste, even in the highly familiar context of the home cage, can itself be associated with the nausea occurring minutes or hours later. The multiple memory systems theory suggests that learning engages multiple discrete memory circuits in the brain, which then can both cooperate and compete to control the behavioral outcome of an animal (White and McDonald, 2002; Wang and Morris, 2010; Packard and Goodman, 2013). In light of this, our results indicate that although the hippocampus may not be necessary for the nonassociative form of taste learning, when a taste is associated with a US, the hippocampus participates and can perhaps compete for this association. In addition, recent studies demonstrated the necessity of incorporating hippocampally encoded information into existing activated cortical frameworks in associative (schema) learning (Tse et al., 2007, 2011). Thus, it is possible that the demand for the information from CA1 to the gustatory network/circuitry is high in shorter time intervals when the novelty of the episode is strong, before it is paired with US, whereas DG-encoded information to the gustatory network/circuitry is critical for the episodic novelty to be maintained for longer durations.
In conclusion, our results shed light on both the hippocampal and classic taste learning circuits and suggest that conditional intervention in specific sublayers of hippocampus can be useful in understanding the role of hippocampus in taste learning and memory formation.
Footnotes
↵*T.J.M. and K.R. jointly directed this work.
This work was supported by European Union Seventh Framework Program EUROSPIN (Contract HEALTH-F2-2009-241498) and the German-Israeli Foundation DIP (RO3971/1-1; K.R.) and by the RIKEN Brain Science Institute (T.J.M.). We thank members of the laboratory of K.R. and T.J.M. for their support and encouragement.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Thomas J. McHugh, RIKEN Brain Science Institute, Laboratory for Circuit and Behavioral Physiology, 2-1 Hirosawa, Wako-shi, Saitama, Japan, 351-0198. tjmchugh{at}brain.riken.jp