Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Featured ArticleArticles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Apnea-Induced Rapid Eye Movement Sleep Disruption Impairs Human Spatial Navigational Memory

Andrew W. Varga, Akifumi Kishi, Janna Mantua, Jason Lim, Viachaslau Koushyk, David P. Leibert, Ricardo S. Osorio, David M. Rapoport and Indu Ayappa
Journal of Neuroscience 29 October 2014, 34 (44) 14571-14577; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3220-14.2014
Andrew W. Varga
1New York University (NYU) Sleep Disorders Center and
3Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, New York 10003
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Akifumi Kishi
1New York University (NYU) Sleep Disorders Center and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Janna Mantua
1New York University (NYU) Sleep Disorders Center and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jason Lim
1New York University (NYU) Sleep Disorders Center and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Viachaslau Koushyk
1New York University (NYU) Sleep Disorders Center and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
David P. Leibert
1New York University (NYU) Sleep Disorders Center and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Ricardo S. Osorio
2Center for Brain Health, NYU Langone School of Medicine, NYU, New York, New York 10016, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
David M. Rapoport
1New York University (NYU) Sleep Disorders Center and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Indu Ayappa
1New York University (NYU) Sleep Disorders Center and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Hippocampal electrophysiology and behavioral evidence support a role for sleep in spatial navigational memory, but the role of particular sleep stages is less clear. Although rodent models suggest the importance of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep in spatial navigational memory, a similar role for REM sleep has never been examined in humans. We recruited subjects with severe obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) who were well treated and adherent with continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP). Restricting CPAP withdrawal to REM through real-time monitoring of the polysomnogram provides a novel way of addressing the role of REM sleep in spatial navigational memory with a physiologically relevant stimulus. Individuals spent two different nights in the laboratory, during which subjects performed timed trials before and after sleep on one of two unique 3D spatial mazes. One night of sleep was normally consolidated with use of therapeutic CPAP throughout, whereas on the other night, CPAP was reduced only in REM sleep, allowing REM OSA to recur. REM disruption via this method caused REM sleep reduction and significantly fragmented any remaining REM sleep without affecting total sleep time, sleep efficiency, or slow-wave sleep. We observed improvements in maze performance after a night of normal sleep that were significantly attenuated after a night of REM disruption without changes in psychomotor vigilance. Furthermore, the improvement in maze completion time significantly positively correlated with the mean REM run duration across both sleep conditions. In conclusion, we demonstrate a novel role for REM sleep in human memory formation and highlight a significant cognitive consequence of OSA.

  • continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP)
  • obstructive sleep apnea
  • randomized controlled trial
  • REM sleep
  • sleep fragmentation
  • spatial navigational memory

Introduction

Both sleep duration and architecture have been implicated in different types of memories, but the mechanisms by which sleep exerts these actions remain poorly understood (Diekelmann and Born, 2010). Rapid eye movement (REM) and non-REM (NREM) sleep differ in several significant ways, including neurotransmitter tone, degree of neuronal synchrony, and frequency of cortical local field potential oscillations. Understanding the contributions of REM and NREM sleep toward memory processes may help shed light on the underlying mechanisms.

Spatial navigational memory represents an attractive model memory system to study the effects of sleep. In vivo electrophysiological recordings in actively exploring rodents showed that patterns of firing in hippocampal place cells during wake exploration were replayed during sleep, raising the possibility that this phenomenon acted to strengthen the memory trace. Hippocampal replay occurred in both NREM sleep (Wilson and McNaughton, 1994), in which the hippocampal replay happened over timescales much faster than occurred during wake, and in REM sleep (Louie and Wilson, 2001), in which the replay happened with greater temporal fidelity to wake. Behaviorally, hippocampal sharp-wave ripple complex suppression during NREM sleep impaired performance on a radial arm maze (Girardeau et al., 2009). REM sleep suppression impaired spatial memory in the radial arm maze (Smith et al., 1998) and in the Morris water maze (Youngblood et al., 1997).

Contributions of REM and NREM sleep toward spatial memory in human subjects are less well delineated. Overnight sleep in general enhanced spatial navigational memory as assessed via performance on a virtual maze navigation task that was not appreciated after a comparable period of daytime wake in human subjects (Ferrara et al., 2008; Nguyen et al., 2013). In six subjects, the degree of regional cerebral blood flow in the right hippocampus during slow-wave sleep correlated with overnight performance improvement in a virtual spatial maze (Peigneux et al., 2004). The potential contribution of REM sleep toward consolidation of human spatial navigational memories has heretofore not been examined.

Such a contribution of REM sleep has important clinical implications in the most common medical sleep disorder, obstructive sleep apnea (OSA). Because of the skeletal muscle atonia that normally occurs in REM sleep, the frequency of obstructive apneas and the drop in blood oxygen saturation is often greatest in REM sleep. Few subjects have exclusively REM-related OSA, although many with OSA have their worst disease in REM.

The present study was designed to circumvent this limitation in subject availability. To test the effects of REM-only OSA, we recruited subjects with severe underlying OSA who were well treated and highly adherent to therapy with continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP). Transiently discontinuing therapy for OSA by acutely lowering CPAP has caused immediate recurrence of OSA and many of its consequences (Kohler et al., 2011). Restricting this discontinuation of therapy to a specific stage of sleep (e.g., REM) through real-time overnight EEG monitoring provides a unique and novel way of addressing the role of REM in memory in a human model.

Materials and Methods

Participants.

Subjects were recruited from the New York University (NYU) Sleep Disorders Center. Twenty-one adult subjects with previously diagnosed severe OSA who had been on CPAP therapy for at least 2 months and reported good adherence to CPAP were approached for participation in the study. Severe OSA was defined as having an apnea–hypopnea index with 4% oxygen desaturation (AHI4%) >30/h or apnea–hypopnea index with 3% oxygen saturation or arousal (AHI-all) >45/h (see definitions under Polysomnography below). Subjects were included only if CPAP adherence was ≥4 h usage per night for ≥70% of nights as documented by download from the CPAP memory card. Exclusion criteria included use of any psychoactive drugs aside from the use of a single selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) antidepressant, the presence of critical comorbid conditions, the presence of preexisting cognitive or neurological deficits, and pregnancy. One subject was excluded when his CPAP machine data download indicated he had not been using CPAP at all before the first visit. Two other subjects were excluded because their prescribed CPAP was not therapeutic and left them with significant residual apnea during sleep. The number of subjects thus analyzed was 18 (14 men, 4 women; mean age, 54 years). Three of the 18 were taking an SSRI antidepressant. All subjects provided informed consent before their participation. All procedures were approved by the NYU Langone School of Medicine Institutional Review Board.

Virtual maze performance and psychomotor vigilance test.

At ∼8:00 P.M. on each night of study, subjects began training on a virtual maze task: a simple 3D environment designed for this research (Nguyen et al., 2013). After a period of generalized familiarization with joystick controls (Thrustmaster) in a Z-shaped corridor, subjects initially spent 3 min exploring a complex maze designed using “Unreal Tournamant 3 Editor” (Epic Games). Avatar walking speed and turning speed were reduced to minimize motion “cybersickness.” The game was projected onto a screen in a darkened testing room. The viewing area was 67 × 50 inches, and subjects sat 13 feet away. Subjects were instructed to remember the layout of the maze environment as well as possible. Subsequently, subjects navigated through the same maze during three pre-sleep test trials, in which they were instructed to reach a specified goal point as quickly as possible. Time to reach the goal per trial was capped at 600 s.

Subjects were connected to polysomnography equipment at ∼9:00 P.M., and a full night polysomnogram on therapeutic CPAP was performed following standard American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) criteria (Iber et al., 2007). On the REM-disruption nights, a sleep technician lowered CPAP to 4 cm H2O as soon as REM sleep was identified and returned to the therapeutic value after exiting REM sleep. During instances in which additional CPAP withdrawal was necessary to induce apnea, CPAP was reduced to 0 cm H2O using equipment and techniques we developed previously to deliver CPAP of 0 cm H2O without rebreathing (Patel et al., 2011).

To control for attention effects, subjects performed a standardized 20 min psychomotor vigilance test (PVT) beginning 1 h after awakening from each night in the sleep laboratory. Thereafter, subjects performed three final test trials on the 3D virtual maze, again instructed to reach the same specified goal point as quickly as possible.

Performance metrics on the virtual maze included completion time, distance traveled within the maze, and distance spent backtracking. Overnight change was calculated as the difference between the averages across the three trials before and after sleep, normalized to the average of the three trials before sleep for each metric. Positive values represent improvement, and negative values represent worsening. Performance metrics on the PVT included reaction time (in milliseconds) and number of lapses (no response after 500 ms). A transform of the number of lapses was done to enable parametric testing [lapses transform = sqrt (#lapses) + sqrt (#lapses + 1), where sqrt is square root] (Dinges et al., 1997).

Subjects were counterbalanced for both maze encountered first and sleep condition encountered first in a pseudorandom manner. A 2 week period occurred between conditions, during which any effect of REM sleep fragmentation, including possible REM rebound, should have normalized if REM disruption occurred first. Subjects returned to the laboratory and performed the exact same series of events under the alternate condition. CPAP adherence was evaluated during the period between conditions. All subjects included in our analysis continued to use CPAP for at least 4 h/night for at least 70% of nights (the initial inclusion criterion).

Polysomnography.

All polysomnograms consisted of a full night spent in the NYU Sleep Disorders Center. Respiratory airflow was measured from the CPAP machine. CPAP pressure at the mask was measured using a pressure transducer. Polysomnograms were scored in 30 s epochs according to standard criteria (AASM) for sleep and EEG arousals. Total sleep time and percentage time spent in wake, NREM stage 1 (NREM 1), NREM 2, NREM 3, and REM sleep were determined. Respiratory events were scored from the airflow signal using AASM criteria, and stage specific (REM, NREM, and Total) apnea indices were calculated. Apneas were defined as the absence of airflow for ≥10 s. Hypopnea4% was defined as a reduction in the amplitude of breathing by ≥30% for ≥10 s with ≥4% decline in blood oxygen saturation, regardless of the presence of an arousal. Hypopnea (3% or arousal) was defined as a reduction in the amplitude of breathing by ≥30% for ≥10 s accompanied by ≥3% decline in blood oxygen saturation or an arousal indicated by a sudden increase in the cortical EEG frequency or by sudden increase in the motor tone in the chin or anterior tibealis by EMG.

AHI4% is defined as the sum of all apneas and hypopneas4% divided by the total sleep time in hours. AHI-all is defined as the sum of apneas and hypopneas (3% or arousal) divided by the total sleep time in hours.

Continuity of sleep was assessed as duration of sleep runs, defined as the duration of consecutive 30 s epochs of sleep scored as that stage, terminated by one or more epochs scored as another stage, including wake (Norman et al., 2006; Kishi et al., 2011).

Data analysis.

Data were analyzed using SigmaPlot version 11.0 and MATLAB (R 2013b). Comparison between conditions was performed using paired t tests for normally distributed data, and mean values ± SEM are reported. For data not normally distributed, analysis consisted of Wilcoxon's signed-rank tests, and median values are reported.

For sleep continuity data, a bootstrap-based analysis that accounted for the number of runs contributed by each subject was performed for continuity of each sleep stage (REM, NREM 1, NREM 2, and NREM 3). Nights in which CPAP was withdrawn were likely to result in a significantly higher number of sleep runs versus nights without CPAP withdrawal. Thus, striking a balance between equivalence among subjects and retaining a sufficient number of data points, on those nights with more than the median number of runs, runs were randomly sampled up to the median number. Log rank tests were used to compare REM-disrupted versus normal sleep conditions on the survival curves derived from the sampling procedure. This procedure was repeated 1000 times. At each step of the iteration, a p value was estimated from the history of previous p values, yielding an increasingly stable result as the number of iterations increases. Correlation data were calculated using a Pearson's product moment with mean REM run duration shown on a log scale. Results were considered significant at p < 0.05.

Results

Effects of CPAP withdrawal in REM sleep on sleep architecture and OSA severity

Total sleep time and sleep efficiency were markedly similar between conditions. When REM was disrupted via CPAP withdrawal, there were numerically small, but statistically significant, increases in NREM 1 and NREM 2, without any significant change in NREM 3 (slow-wave sleep). In contrast, when REM was disrupted via CPAP withdrawal, there was a significant reduction in the proportion of REM sleep from 20.5 to 11.5%. CPAP withdrawal in REM sleep also resulted in significant fragmentation of any remaining REM sleep. The mean REM run duration was significantly deceased in the REM-disrupted condition. The percentage of time spent with runs of REM <3 min in duration was significantly increased, and the percentage of time spent with runs of REM >5 min was significantly decreased in subjects when CPAP was withdrawn in REM versus normal sleep. The median number of arousals in REM increased when CPAP was withdrawn in REM. Table 1 summarizes the changes in sleep architecture across conditions. The cumulative duration probability distribution of REM sleep during CPAP withdrawal in REM was significantly left shifted (p < 0.001), indicating that REM sleep occurred in smaller bouts than during normal sleep, without any significant change in the duration probability distribution of NREM stages (Fig. 1). In summary, CPAP withdrawal in REM sleep induced decreased amounts of REM sleep and produced high levels of fragmentation of the remaining REM sleep.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Effects of REM-specific CPAP withdrawal on sleep architecture

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Withdrawal of CPAP in REM sleep causes significant REM sleep fragmentation. Continuity of sleep for normal sleep and REM-disrupted conditions. Survival curves (cumulative probability distributions) of stage-specific sleep runs in the normal sleep (white) and REM-disrupted (black) conditions. A, The survival curve for the REM-disrupted night showed a significant shift toward shorter runs of REM sleep compared with the normal night (p < 0.001, log-rank test). No significant difference between sleep conditions was observed in the survival curves for NREM 1 (B), NREM 2 (C), or NREM 3 (D).

CPAP withdrawal in REM resulted in small but statistically significant increases in the NREM AHI4% and AHI-all, although all values remained in the normal range, suggesting absence of clinical disease (AHI4% <5/h; AHI-all <15/h). As expected, CPAP withdrawal in REM sleep resulted in large significant increases in the AHI4% and AHI-all during REM to ranges consistent with severe disease (AHI4% >30/h or AHI-all >45/h). Averaged over the entire night, CPAP withdrawal in REM sleep produced only mild OSA overall (AHI4% = 8.1/h and AHI-all = 17.0/h). Table 2 summarizes the changes in OSA severity.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Effects of REM-specific CPAP withdrawal on apnea severity

Effects of CPAP withdrawal in REM sleep on spatial navigational memory

We tracked subjects' average performance before and after sleep by measures of completion time, distance traveled within the maze, and distance spent backtracking. Across a night of normal sleep, there was improvement in completion time (mean of 31.3%), distance traveled (median of 34.3%), and distance spent backtracking (median of 56.5%). During conditions of REM disruption via CPAP withdrawal, these performance gains were not only lost, but on average, subjects worsened on all three measures: completion time (mean of −4.7%, p = 0.016 vs normal sleep, paired t test), distance traveled (median of −0.5%, p = 0.03 vs normal sleep, Wilcoxon's signed-rank test), distance spent backtracking (median of −0.1%, p = 0.034 vs normal sleep, Wilcoxon's signed-rank test; Fig. 2). Importantly, across all study nights regardless of sleep condition, the degree of change in completion time performance significantly positively correlated with the mean REM run duration (r = 0.34, p = 0.043, Pearson's correlation; Fig. 3) but not with amounts of REM sleep, either by total minutes of REM or percentage of REM sleep or by severity of the REM OSA (AHI4% or AHI-all). This suggests then that the consolidation of REM sleep is more important than strict duration for optimizing spatial navigational memory.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Disruption of REM sleep significantly attenuates the gains in virtual maze performance that occur across normal sleep. A, Completion time improved 31.3% after normal sleep but worsened 4.7% after REM disruption. *p = 0.016, paired t test. B, Total distance traveled improved 34.3% after normal sleep but worsened 0.5% after REM disruption. *p = 0.03, Wilcoxon's signed-rank test. C, Distance spent backtracking improved 56.5% after normal sleep but worsened 0.1% after REM disruption. *p = 0.034, Wilcoxon's signed-rank test.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Overnight change in virtual maze completion time positively correlates with the mean REM run duration. Scatter plots of the relationship between REM continuity (mean duration of REM sleep runs) and percentage change in completion time for normal sleep (white circles) and REM-disrupted night (black circles). r = 0.34, p = 0.043, Pearson's correlation coefficient.

Although improved performance could have occurred on the second night as a result of enhanced familiarity with the task, we did not observe any significant differences in subjects' performance between night 1 and night 2 regardless of maze or condition (mean completion time improvement night 1 = 14.4%, night 2 = 12.1%, p = 0.89, paired t test) or between maze 1 and maze 2 regardless of night or condition (mean completion time improvement maze 1 = 18%, maze 2 = 8.6%, p = 0.44, paired t test).

Effects of CPAP withdrawal in REM sleep on psychomotor vigilance

To control for possible effects on attention affecting spatial navigation performance in the morning, subjects performed a 20 min PVT immediately before completing the three morning test trials on the virtual maze. We observed no significant differences in mean reaction times (270 ms during normal sleep, 274 ms during REM disruption, p = 0.865, Wilcoxon's signed rank test) or in number of lapses (3.1 ± 0.4 during normal sleep, 3.3 ± 0.6 during REM disruption, p = 0.71, paired t test; Fig. 4).

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Disruption of REM sleep does not affect psychomotor vigilance. There is no difference in mean reaction time between sleep conditions (270 ms during normal sleep, 274 ms during REM disruption, p = 0.865, Wilcoxon's signed-rank test) (A) or in a transform of lapses (3.1 ± 0.4 during normal sleep, 3.3 ± 0.6 during REM disruption, p = 0.71, paired t test) (B).

Discussion

In this study, we demonstrate that overnight sleep acts to consolidate spatial navigational memories. This benefit of overnight sleep is lost when REM sleep is disrupted by OSA induced by CPAP withdrawal exclusively in REM sleep. Our finding of the 31.3% overnight improvement in completion time on the virtual maze in our older population was quite similar to the 34% overnight improvement (Nguyen et al., 2013) and 30% overnight improvement (Ferrara et al., 2008) in completion time found in healthy cohorts of college-aged subjects on related virtual mazes. That these improvements in completion time, as well as improvements in distance traveled and distance spent backtracking, were ablated by REM disruption suggests that REM sleep is necessary for the consolidation of spatial navigational memory. An alternative interpretation is that the deficit in spatial navigational memory consolidation is a nonspecific consequence of sleep disruption in general, regardless of sleep stage. Such an interpretation supposes that an equivalent amount of sleep disruption (e.g., 11.5% of total sleep time) occurring randomly across sleep would cause the same deficit. However, previous work has shown that disruption of NREM sleep significantly reduces psychomotor vigilance (Kribbs et al., 1993), thereby potentially confounding the interpretation of worsened virtual maze performance after such an intervention. Importantly, the disruption of REM sleep in this study caused no change in morning psychomotor vigilance.

The induction of OSA in REM sleep produced in our study shortened and fragmented REM sleep but also induced intermittent hypoxia. Although we cannot rule out the contribution of intermittent hypoxia toward interrupting spatial navigational memory consolidation, intermittent hypoxia in rodent models induced deficits in spatial memory in the Morris water maze under chronic, rather than acute, conditions (Gozal et al., 2001). Additionally, in a direct comparison of acute intermittent hypoxia versus sleep fragmentation 24 h after acquisition of the Morris water maze in rodents, subsequent spatial memory was preserved after intermittent hypoxia but significantly worsened after sleep fragmentation (Ward et al., 2009).

Spatial navigational memory is thought to be subserved by at least two brain systems working in concert: (1) an allocentric system mediated primarily by medial temporal lobe structures in which external landmarks are used to create a mental spatial map; and (2) an egocentric system mediated primarily by the striatum, which incorporates body movement, turn direction, speed, and elapsed time (Iaria et al., 2003; Buzsáki and Moser, 2013). Rodents deprived chronically of either total sleep (Hagewoud et al., 2010) or REM sleep (Bjorness et al., 2005) showed significant inability to engage hippocampal strategies for solving spatial tasks and instead relied on nonspatial, response-based strategies. One night of total sleep deprivation in human subjects resulted in decreased recruitment of bilateral posterior hippocampal regions during encoding of visual scenes measured with functional imaging (Yoo et al., 2007). In contrast, when sleep was completely deprived overnight in human subjects after evening training on a virtual maze, functional imaging during navigation 72 h later showed absence of recruitment of the caudate bilaterally (Orban et al., 2006).

Our finding of the importance of REM sleep in spatial navigational memory consolidation in human subjects does not rule out contributions of other sleep stages toward this phenomenon. Slow-wave sleep and its associated sharp-wave ripples have been shown to be important in spatial memory in both humans (Peigneux et al., 2004) and rodents (Girardeau et al., 2009). In fact, the sequential hypothesis (Giuditta et al., 1995) suggests that the maximal benefit of sleep on memory consolidation arises from a period of REM sleep after slow-wave sleep. The strongest correlate of overnight improvement in human visual texture discrimination was the product of the amount of slow-wave sleep in the first quarter of the night and the amount of REM in the last quarter (Stickgold et al., 2000). In any case, a contribution of REM sleep toward spatial navigational memory has important clinical implications in several sleep disorders in which REM fragmentation occurs, including not only OSA as highlighted here, but also in REM behavior disorder and narcolepsy. There may be clinical implications for individuals taking antidepressants known to reduce REM sleep, such as the monoamine oxidase inhibitor, SSRI, and serotonin–norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor (SNRI) classes of medicines, but these medicines have pleiotropic effects that may also act to improve memory.

Several proposals exist for the neural mechanisms by which REM sleep supports memory formation. In a model supported by the sequential hypothesis, the reactivation and redistribution of neural connections occurring during slow-wave sleep require a period of subsequent synaptic consolidation possibly afforded by subsequent REM sleep. Supporting this role for REM sleep, the waking learning of novel object information or the waking induction of hippocampal long-term potentiation (LTP) induced the REM-dependent induction of immediate early genes such as Arc and Zif-268 in primary somatosensory cortex (Ribeiro et al., 2007) and hippocampus (Ribeiro et al., 2002), respectively. REM-preferential sleep deprivation in rats resulted in impaired hippocampal LTP induction via both titanic and theta-burst stimulation protocols (McDermott et al., 2003), impaired LTP maintenance in the hippocampus, but not prefrontal cortex (Romcy-Pereira and Pavlides, 2004), and decreased hippocampal ERK activation (Ravassard et al., 2009).

Plasticity during REM sleep may be bidirectional, for example, strengthening connections encoding novel information and weakening connections encoding familiar information. This idea is supported by the observation that hippocampal place cells fired predominantly at the trough of the local theta oscillation in REM sleep after exploration of a familiar track, whereas a bimodal pattern of firing (at both peaks and troughs) was observed after exploration of a novel track (Poe et al., 2000). Along the same lines, within the hippocampus, CA1 pyramidal neuron and interneuron firing rates decrease across REM, and both these excitability changes and neuronal synchrony during sharp-wave ripples correlate with theta power in REM sleep (Grosmark et al., 2012).

In summary, our results support an important role for REM in the consolidation of spatial navigational memory in human subjects and a negative effect on this process by apnea-induced REM sleep disruption. A particular strength of this study includes the paired design, which helps control for differences in experience with 3D computer games. Studies using tones or other exogenous methods to disrupt REM sleep help answer scientific questions about its role. However, by using stage-specific withdrawal of CPAP to induce OSA, we not only provide mechanistic insight into the consolidation of spatial navigational memory but also describe new cognitive consequences for those left with untreated severe OSA.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by the philanthropy of the James Kuhn Friends of Sleep Medicine, New York University Clinical and Translational Science Awards Grant UL1TR000038 from the National Center for the Advancement of Translational Science (A.W.V.), the American Sleep Medicine Foundation Physician Scientist Training Award (A.W.V.), National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences Training Grant T32ES007267-20 (Principal Investigator William N. Rom), and National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute K24 Grant HL109156 (I.A.). We thank Dr. Omar Burschtin for experimental insight and critical appraisal of this manuscript and Rakhil Kanevskaya and Boris Opancha for expert sleep scoring. We thank Drs. Robert Stickgold, Erin Wamsley, and Matthew Tucker for virtual maze template and performance analysis guidance.

  • I.A. has received support for research from the industry for the past 24 months in the forms of grants and clinical trials from Fisher & Paykel Healthcare. She holds multiple U.S. and foreign patents covering techniques and analysis algorithms for the diagnosis of obstructive sleep apnea/hypopnea syndrome (OSAHS) and techniques for administering continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP). Several of these have been licensed to Fisher & Paykel Healthcare. D.M.R. has received support for research from the industry for the past 24 months in the form of grants and clinical trials from Fisher & Paykel Healthcare and speaking and consulting engagements for Fisher & Paykel Healthcare. He holds multiple U.S. and foreign patents covering techniques and analysis algorithms for the diagnosis of OSAHS and techniques for administering CPAP. Several of these have been licensed to Biologics, Fisher & Paykel Healthcare, and Health C'Aire.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Andrew W. Varga, New York University Sleep Disorders Center, NBV-7N2, 462 First Avenue, New York, NY 10016. andrew.varga{at}nyumc.org

References

  1. ↵
    1. Bjorness TE,
    2. Riley BT,
    3. Tysor MK,
    4. Poe GR
    (2005) REM restriction persistently alters strategy used to solve a spatial task. Learn Mem 12:352–359, doi:10.1101/lm.84805, pmid:15897251.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    1. Buzsáki G,
    2. Moser EI
    (2013) Memory, navigation and theta rhythm in the hippocampal-entorhinal system. Nat Neurosci 16:130–138, doi:10.1038/nn.3304, pmid:23354386.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Diekelmann S,
    2. Born J
    (2010) The memory function of sleep. Nat Rev Neurosci 11:114–126, doi:10.1038/nrn2762, pmid:20046194.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Dinges DF,
    2. Pack F,
    3. Williams K,
    4. Gillen KA,
    5. Powell JW,
    6. Ott GE,
    7. Aptowicz C,
    8. Pack AI
    (1997) Cumulative sleepiness, mood disturbance, and psychomotor vigilance performance decrements during a week of sleep restricted to 4–5 hours per night. Sleep 20:267–277, pmid:9231952.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Ferrara M,
    2. Iaria G,
    3. Tempesta D,
    4. Curcio G,
    5. Moroni F,
    6. Marzano C,
    7. De Gennaro L,
    8. Pacitti C
    (2008) Sleep to find your way: the role of sleep in the consolidation of memory for navigation in humans. Hippocampus 18:844–851, doi:10.1002/hipo.20444, pmid:18493970.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Girardeau G,
    2. Benchenane K,
    3. Wiener SI,
    4. Buzsáki G,
    5. Zugaro MB
    (2009) Selective suppression of hippocampal ripples impairs spatial memory. Nat Neurosci 12:1222–1223, doi:10.1038/nn.2384, pmid:19749750.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Giuditta A,
    2. Ambrosini MV,
    3. Montagnese P,
    4. Mandile P,
    5. Cotugno M,
    6. Grassi Zucconi G,
    7. Vescia S
    (1995) The sequential hypothesis of the function of sleep. Behav Brain Res 69:157–166, doi:10.1016/0166-4328(95)00012-I.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Gozal D,
    2. Daniel JM,
    3. Dohanich GP
    (2001) Behavioral and anatomical correlates of chronic episodic hypoxia during sleep in the rat. J Neurosci 21:2442–2450, pmid:11264318.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    1. Grosmark AD,
    2. Mizuseki K,
    3. Pastalkova E,
    4. Diba K,
    5. Buzsáki G
    (2012) REM sleep reorganizes hippocampal excitability. Neuron 75:1001–1007, doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.015, pmid:22998869.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Hagewoud R,
    2. Havekes R,
    3. Tiba PA,
    4. Novati A,
    5. Hogenelst K,
    6. Weinreder P,
    7. Van der Zee EA,
    8. Meerlo P
    (2010) Coping with sleep deprivation: shifts in regional brain activity and learning strategy. Sleep 33:1465–1473, pmid:21102988.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Iaria G,
    2. Petrides M,
    3. Dagher A,
    4. Pike B,
    5. Bohbot VD
    (2003) Cognitive strategies dependent on the hippocampus and caudate nucleus in human navigation: variability and change with practice. J Neurosci 23:5945–5952, pmid:12843299.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. ↵
    1. Iber C,
    2. Ancoli-Israel S,
    3. Chesson AL,
    4. Quan SF
    (2007) The AASM manual for the scoring of sleep and associated events: rules, terminology, and technical specifications (American Academy of Sleep Medicine, Westchester, IL), Ed 1.
  13. ↵
    1. Kishi A,
    2. Natelson BH,
    3. Togo F,
    4. Struzik ZR,
    5. Rapoport DM,
    6. Yamamoto Y
    (2011) Sleep-stage dynamics in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome with or without fibromyalgia. Sleep 34:1551–1560, pmid:22043126.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Kohler M,
    2. Stoewhas AC,
    3. Ayers L,
    4. Senn O,
    5. Bloch KE,
    6. Russi EW,
    7. Stradling JR
    (2011) Effects of continuous positive airway pressure therapy withdrawal in patients with obstructive sleep apnea: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 184:1192–1199, doi:10.1164/rccm.201106-0964OC, pmid:21836134.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Kribbs NB,
    2. Pack AI,
    3. Kline LR,
    4. Getsy JE,
    5. Schuett JS,
    6. Henry JN,
    7. Maislin G,
    8. Dinges DF
    (1993) Effects of one night without nasal CPAP treatment on sleep and sleepiness in patients with obstructive sleep apnea. Am Rev Respir Dis 147:1162–1168, doi:10.1164/ajrccm/147.5.1162, pmid:8484626.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Louie K,
    2. Wilson MA
    (2001) Temporally structured replay of awake hippocampal ensemble activity during rapid eye movement sleep. Neuron 29:145–156, doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00186-6, pmid:11182087.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. McDermott CM,
    2. LaHoste GJ,
    3. Chen C,
    4. Musto A,
    5. Bazan NG,
    6. Magee JC
    (2003) Sleep deprivation causes behavioral, synaptic, and membrane excitability alterations in hippocampal neurons. J Neurosci 23:9687–9695, pmid:14573548.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    1. Nguyen ND,
    2. Tucker MA,
    3. Stickgold R,
    4. Wamsley EJ
    (2013) Overnight sleep enhances hippocampus-dependent aspects of spatial memory. Sleep 36:1051–1057, doi:10.5665/sleep.2808, pmid:23814342.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Norman RG,
    2. Scott MA,
    3. Ayappa I,
    4. Walsleben JA,
    5. Rapoport DM
    (2006) Sleep continuity measured by survival curve analysis. Sleep 29:1625–1631, pmid:17252894.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Orban P,
    2. Rauchs G,
    3. Balteau E,
    4. Degueldre C,
    5. Luxen A,
    6. Maquet P,
    7. Peigneux P
    (2006) Sleep after spatial learning promotes covert reorganization of brain activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103:7124–7129, doi:10.1073/pnas.0510198103, pmid:16636288.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    1. Patel AV,
    2. Hwang D,
    3. Masdeu MJ,
    4. Chen GM,
    5. Rapoport DM,
    6. Ayappa I
    (2011) Predictors of response to a nasal expiratory resistor device and its potential mechanisms of action for treatment of obstructive sleep apnea. J Clin Sleep Med 7:13–22, pmid:21344051.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Peigneux P,
    2. Laureys S,
    3. Fuchs S,
    4. Collette F,
    5. Perrin F,
    6. Reggers J,
    7. Phillips C,
    8. Degueldre C,
    9. Del Fiore G,
    10. Aerts J,
    11. Luxen A,
    12. Maquet P
    (2004) Are spatial memories strengthened in the human hippocampus during slow wave sleep? Neuron 44:535–545, doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2004.10.007, pmid:15504332.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Poe GR,
    2. Nitz DA,
    3. McNaughton BL,
    4. Barnes CA
    (2000) Experience-dependent phase-reversal of hippocampal neuron firing during REM sleep. Brain Res 855:176–180, doi:10.1016/S0006-8993(99)02310-0, pmid:10650147.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Ravassard P,
    2. Pachoud B,
    3. Comte JC,
    4. Mejia-Perez C,
    5. Scoté-Blachon C,
    6. Gay N,
    7. Claustrat B,
    8. Touret M,
    9. Luppi PH,
    10. Salin PA
    (2009) Paradoxical (REM) sleep deprivation causes a large and rapidly reversible decrease in long-term potentiation, synaptic transmission, glutamate receptor protein levels, and ERK/MAPK activation in the dorsal hippocampus. Sleep 32:227–240, pmid:19238810.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Ribeiro S,
    2. Mello CV,
    3. Velho T,
    4. Gardner TJ,
    5. Jarvis ED,
    6. Pavlides C
    (2002) Induction of hippocampal long-term potentiation during waking leads to increased extrahippocampal zif-268 expression during ensuing rapid-eye-movement sleep. J Neurosci 22:10914–10923, pmid:12486186.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    1. Ribeiro S,
    2. Shi X,
    3. Engelhard M,
    4. Zhou Y,
    5. Zhang H,
    6. Gervasoni D,
    7. Lin SC,
    8. Wada K,
    9. Lemos NA,
    10. Nicolelis MA
    (2007) Novel experience induces persistent sleep-dependent plasticity in the cortex but not in the hippocampus. Front Neurosci 1:43–55, doi:10.3389/neuro.01.1.1.003.2007, pmid:18982118.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Romcy-Pereira R,
    2. Pavlides C
    (2004) Distinct modulatory effects of sleep on the maintenance of hippocampal and medial prefrontal cortex LTP. Eur J Neurosci 20:3453–3462, doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03808.x, pmid:15610178.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Smith CT,
    2. Conway JM,
    3. Rose GM
    (1998) Brief paradoxical sleep deprivation impairs reference, but not working, memory in the radial arm maze task. Neurobiol Learn Mem 69:211–217, doi:10.1006/nlme.1997.3809, pmid:9619997.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Stickgold R,
    2. Whidbee D,
    3. Schirmer B,
    4. Patel V,
    5. Hobson JA
    (2000) Visual discrimination task improvement: a multi-step process occurring during sleep. J Cogn Neurosci 12:246–254, doi:10.1162/089892900562075, pmid:10771409.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Ward CP,
    2. McCoy JG,
    3. McKenna JT,
    4. Connolly NP,
    5. McCarley RW,
    6. Strecker RE
    (2009) Spatial learning and memory deficits following exposure to 24 h of sleep fragmentation or intermittent hypoxia in a rat model of obstructive sleep apnea. Brain Res 1294:128–137, doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2009.07.064, pmid:19643093.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Wilson MA,
    2. McNaughton BL
    (1994) Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. Science 265:676–679, doi:10.1126/science.8036517, pmid:8036517.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    1. Yoo SS,
    2. Hu PT,
    3. Gujar N,
    4. Jolesz FA,
    5. Walker MP
    (2007) A deficit in the ability to form new human memories without sleep. Nat Neurosci 10:385–392, doi:10.1038/nn1851, pmid:17293859.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Youngblood BD,
    2. Zhou J,
    3. Smagin GN,
    4. Ryan DH,
    5. Harris RB
    (1997) Sleep deprivation by the “flower pot” technique and spatial reference memory. Physiol Behav 61:249–256, doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(96)00363-0, pmid:9035255.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 34 (44)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 34, Issue 44
29 Oct 2014
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Apnea-Induced Rapid Eye Movement Sleep Disruption Impairs Human Spatial Navigational Memory
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Apnea-Induced Rapid Eye Movement Sleep Disruption Impairs Human Spatial Navigational Memory
Andrew W. Varga, Akifumi Kishi, Janna Mantua, Jason Lim, Viachaslau Koushyk, David P. Leibert, Ricardo S. Osorio, David M. Rapoport, Indu Ayappa
Journal of Neuroscience 29 October 2014, 34 (44) 14571-14577; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3220-14.2014

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Apnea-Induced Rapid Eye Movement Sleep Disruption Impairs Human Spatial Navigational Memory
Andrew W. Varga, Akifumi Kishi, Janna Mantua, Jason Lim, Viachaslau Koushyk, David P. Leibert, Ricardo S. Osorio, David M. Rapoport, Indu Ayappa
Journal of Neuroscience 29 October 2014, 34 (44) 14571-14577; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3220-14.2014
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP)
  • obstructive sleep apnea
  • randomized controlled trial
  • REM sleep
  • sleep fragmentation
  • spatial navigational memory

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Articles

  • Choice Behavior Guided by Learned, But Not Innate, Taste Aversion Recruits the Orbitofrontal Cortex
  • Maturation of Spontaneous Firing Properties after Hearing Onset in Rat Auditory Nerve Fibers: Spontaneous Rates, Refractoriness, and Interfiber Correlations
  • Insulin Treatment Prevents Neuroinflammation and Neuronal Injury with Restored Neurobehavioral Function in Models of HIV/AIDS Neurodegeneration
Show more Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Non-neural factors influencing BOLD response magnitudes within individual subjects
  • Stimulus-induced changes in 1/f-like background activity in EEG
  • Complementary roles of primate dorsal premotor and pre-supplementary motor areas to the control of motor sequences
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2022 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.