Abstract
Eyeblink conditioning in restrained rabbits has served as an excellent model of cerebellar-dependent motor learning for many decades. In mice, the role of the cerebellum in eyeblink conditioning is less clear and remains controversial, partly because learning appears to engage fear-related circuits and lesions of the cerebellum do not abolish the learned behavior completely. Furthermore, experiments in mice are performed using freely moving systems, which lack the stability necessary for mapping out the essential neural circuitry with electrophysiological approaches. We have developed a novel apparatus for eyeblink conditioning in head-fixed mice. Here, we show that the performance of mice in our apparatus is excellent and that the learned behavior displays two hallmark features of cerebellar-dependent eyeblink conditioning in rabbits: (1) gradual acquisition; and (2) adaptive timing of conditioned movements. Furthermore, we use a combination of pharmacological inactivation, electrical stimulation, single-unit recordings, and targeted microlesions to demonstrate that the learned behavior is completely dependent on the cerebellum and to pinpoint the exact location in the deep cerebellar nuclei that is necessary. Our results pave the way for using eyeblink conditioning in head-fixed mice as a platform for applying next-generation genetic tools to address molecular and circuit-level questions about cerebellar function in health and disease.
Introduction
Pavlovian eyeblink conditioning is a simple associative task that has proven particularly useful for investigating the neural mechanisms underlying motor learning (Medina et al., 2000). In the simplest version, animals learn to blink in response to an initially neutral conditioned stimulus (CS; e.g., a tone), if it is repeatedly paired with an eyeblink-eliciting unconditioned stimulus (US; e.g., a periocular air puff). The learned blink, which is known as the eyelid conditioned response (CR), displays two hallmark features of motor learning tasks. First, CRs are acquired slowly over the course of many CS–US pairings (Hilgard and Marquis, 1935, 1936; Hilgard and Campbell, 1936; Schneiderman et al., 1962; Skelton, 1988; Ivarsson and Svensson, 2000). Second, CRs are precisely timed such that the time of maximum eyelid closure matches the interval separating the CS and the US during training (Boneau, 1958; Mauk and Ruiz, 1992; Domingo et al., 1997; Freeman et al., 2003; Koekkoek et al., 2003). The success of eyeblink conditioning as a model to study the neural basis of motor learning can be traced back to a series of groundbreaking experiments in rabbits (McCormick et al., 1982; McCormick and Thompson, 1984a; Krupa et al., 1993). This work identified the cerebellum as the key component of the neural circuitry necessary for the initial learning and the subsequent expression of CRs in these animals (Christian and Thompson, 2003).
In mice, the role of the cerebellum has been more difficult to ascertain. First, mice do not tolerate restraint as well as rabbits, so mouse eyeblink conditioning experiments are typically done using freely moving systems (De Zeeuw et al., 2004), which present unique challenges for using neurophysiological approaches that could help identify the relevant circuitry. Second, the properties of eyelid CRs differ significantly between mice and rabbits, calling into question whether the same neural systems are involved. For example, mice can learn very quickly in just a few conditioning trials (Vogel et al., 2002; Woodruff-Pak, 2006; Boele et al., 2010), and their CRs usually start with a fixed short-latency eyelid movement that is not properly timed (Aiba et al., 1994; Kishimoto et al., 2001, 2002; Koekkoek et al., 2003; Wada et al., 2007). These behavioral properties do not match what would be expected for a motor learning task, hinting instead at a possible contribution from fear conditioning circuits (Lennartz and Weinberger, 1992; Vogel et al., 2002; Boele et al., 2010). Indeed, recent studies indicate that mouse CRs are not always entirely driven by the cerebellum (Koekkoek et al., 2003, 2005; Sakamoto and Endo, 2008, 2010) and may include an amygdala-dependent component (Boele et al., 2010; Sakamoto and Endo, 2010).
We have developed a new apparatus for eyeblink conditioning in head-fixed mice, with the goal of isolating the motor learning component of the behavior and assessing whether it is cerebellar dependent. Our experiments were designed to evaluate, for the first time in individual mice, whether eyeblink conditioning displays two hallmark features of motor learning tasks: (1) gradual acquisition and (2) adaptive timing. Because our mice were head fixed, we were able to perform functional mapping experiments in the cerebellum with unprecedented spatial resolution, taking advantage of neurophysiological tools to pinpoint the exact location in the deep cerebellar nuclei (DCN) that is critical for the expression of CRs.
Materials and Methods
Subjects.
All experiments were performed in adult male C57BL/6 mice (The Jackson Laboratory). Mice were singly housed in a room with an inverted light/dark cycle (7:00 A.M. lights off, 7:00 P.M. lights on), and all experiments took place during the dark phase. Procedures were performed in accordance with protocols approved by the University of Pennsylvania Animal Care and Use Committee based on guidelines of the National Institutes of Health.
Surgical procedures.
Before all experiments, stereotaxic surgery was performed to implant a “head plate.” Mice were anesthetized with isoflurane (1.5–2% by volume in O2; SurgiVet) and kept on a heating pad to maintain body temperature. In addition, Meloxicam was given perioperatively to reduce swelling and provide postoperative analgesia. A midline incision was made to expose the skull, and the underlying fascia was cleared with cotton swabs. Two small screws were placed on either side of the midline near bregma. A thin stainless steel or aluminum head plate was then placed over bregma such that the screws fit into the central hole in the head plate (Fig. 1B,C) and secured to the screws and skull using Metabond cement (Parkell). A mark was placed in the cement to indicate the location of bregma so that stereotaxic measurements could be made during experiments.
In some mice (n = 12), a cannula (Plastics One) targeting the DCN was implanted by drilling a small hole in the skull overlying the cerebellum and stereotaxically guiding the bottom of the cannula into a position 1.8 mm above the target. The cannula was then permanently affixed with Metabond, and an internal “dummy” cannula that extended to the end of the guide cannula was inserted to prevent clogging while the mouse was in its home cage. In other mice (n = 4), a 3 mm craniotomy centered ∼6.5 mm posterior and 2.0 mm lateral from bregma was performed to allow access to the cerebellar cortex and deep nuclei for microstimulation and neural recording experiments. A plastic ring (inner diameter, 3.5 mm) serving as a chamber was then implanted over the craniotomy and affixed to the skull with dental acrylic (Jet). A thin layer of antibiotic ointment was applied on top of the dura, and the chamber was filled with a fast-curing silicone elastomer (Kwik Sil; WPI), which could be removed before each daily experiment to give access to the brain. A stainless steel screw was implanted in the bone above the contralateral cerebellum to serve as a reference for microstimulation and neural recording.
Apparatus.
The basic components of the head-fixed eyeblink conditioning system are shown in Figure 1A. The apparatus consists of a foam cylinder mounted to an axle via low-friction bearings, posts, and clamps to hold the cylinder in the air and a breadboard for mounting the various components (full parts list in Table 1). During experiments, the mouse's head plate (see above, Surgical procedures) was attached to a pair of machined rods via 2–56 machine screws (Fig. 1B–D), and the mouse was free to walk on top of the cylinder at will. A high-speed camera and infrared illumination directed at the side of the mouse's face were mounted on a separate post via knuckle joints. Additional posts and clamps (not shown) were used to hold a plastic tube connected to a 23 gauge needle for the air-puff US and either an LED for a light CS or a plastic tube for a vibrissal CS, as described below.
For neurophysiology experiments, a microdrive was mounted on another post such that electrodes could be driven in and out of the brain on a daily basis through a craniotomy (see above, Surgical procedures). To facilitate stereotaxic placement of the electrode a microscopic camera (DynaLite) was placed above the craniotomy to allow live visualization of the electrode as it was lowered into place. To provide additional recording stability, the entire rig was mounted on a breadboard sitting atop a vibration isolation table (Newport) instead of the breadboard listed in Table 1.
Eyeblink conditioning.
Mice were habituated to head restraint for 2 d before beginning the conditioning sessions by placing them on top of the foam cylinder with their heads fixed for 1 h. No stimuli were delivered during this time. We then began several daily sessions (typically a single session comprising 100 trials each day) of CS–US pairing with interstimulus intervals (ISIs) ranging from 100 to 400 ms. In some experiments, there were occasional test trials in which the CS was presented without the US. The intertrial interval (ITI) was set to a minimum of 10 s, but the actual ITI varied slightly because trials were only initiated if the eyelids were open >70–80% and were stable for at least 200 ms (median ITI, 11.0 s; 85% of ITIs fell within 5 s of the minimum for all mice). These criteria were occasionally relaxed during periods of squinting, which was rare.
CSs were either an LED pulse of blue light or a weak puff of air (2–3 psi) directed at the vibrissal pad contralateral to the conditioned eye (Carrel et al., 2012). The pressure of the vibrissal air puff was set so that it did not produce any reflexive eyelid movements. The US was a puff of air (20–30 psi) of 20–30 ms duration directed at the cornea via a 23 gauge needle placed 3 mm from the mouse's eye. The pressure and duration of the periocular air puff were set for each mouse to produce a full reflexive blink (UR) when delivered alone. The CS and US coterminated or, for timing experiments, the CS remained on for 500 ms regardless of ISI.
High-speed videography.
Eyelid movements were monitored under infrared illumination using a high-speed (200 or 350 frames/s) monochrome camera (Allied Vision) interfaced with MATLAB using custom-written software and the Video Acquisition Toolbox.
We developed two different algorithms for measuring eyelid movements from the high-speed videos (Fig. 2), both of which were fast enough to be used in real time during the experiment. The first used an area measurement to calculate the fraction of eyelid closure (FEC) frame by frame (Fig. 2, top). A region of interest (ROI) was selected, corresponding to the pupil, iris, and immediately surrounding fur when the eye was fully open. For each frame, the grayscale values of the pixels in the ROI were converted to binary by setting a threshold such that the pupil and iris had a value of 0 and the fur a value of 1. To eliminate “salt and pepper” noise in the binary image, a 5 × 5 pixel median filter was applied to the grayscale image before converting it to binary. Then, the values of all the pixels in the ROI were summed to calculate the area consisting of fur in each frame. These raw pixel counts were normalized into units of FEC, which ranges from 0 (fully open) to 1 (fully closed). Because the ROI has an elliptical shape, the FEC is proportional to the distance between the two eyelids (i.e., minor axis of the ellipse).
The second method gives a direct readout of the upper and lower eyelid positions (Fig. 2, bottom). A narrow rectangular window ∼25 × 150 pixels was selected such that its long axis extended from the fur of the upper eyelid, through the center of the pupil, to the fur of the lower eyelid. If the reflection of the infrared light fell on the center of the pupil, the window was moved slightly off center. For each frame in the movie, the median pixel intensity was taken along the short axis to give an ∼1 × 150 pixel vector of grayscale values. This grayscale vector was then converted to a binary image as in the FEC calculation. Going from top to bottom in the resulting binary vector, the transitions from white (1) to black (0) and black to white give, respectively, the position of the upper and lower eyelids in pixel units.
The two methods gave consistent results for all properties assessed in this study, but we have exclusively used the “area” method for all analyses and figures.
Pharmacological inactivation.
Reversible inactivation of DCN was accomplished with muscimol (1 mm) or lidocaine (4%) infusions through an implanted cannula (see above, Surgical procedures). In some sessions, fluorescent muscimol (1 mm; BODIPY TMR-X muscimol conjugate; Invitrogen) was used instead. Before an infusion, an internal cannula connected to a 1 μl Hamilton syringe was inserted into the implanted cannula such that the tip protruded 1.8 mm from the bottom. The mouse was then given at least 30 baseline trials and the session was stopped to infuse the drug (0.1–0.15 μl) over the course of 1 min. Conditioning trials began again within 3 min of the infusion. At the end of the session, the internal cannula was carefully removed and replaced with the dummy cannula. Once outside the brain, the internal cannula was tested for clogging by depressing the syringe an additional 0.1 μl, and the session was excluded from analysis if this did not produce a visible drop at the tip (<10% of all sessions).
Microstimulation.
Functional mapping of DCN was performed using 80-μm-diameter platinum iridium monopolar electrodes (100 KΩ; Alpha Omega). Electrodes were positioned using stereotaxic coordinates relative to a mark on the cement (see above, Surgical procedures). Mapping was mostly confined to the anterior interpositus (AIP), dorsolateral hump (DLH), and lateral nucleus (LN). Electrodes were advanced in steps of 100 μm, and currents in the range of 1–15 μA (200 ms pulse trains; 250 μs biphasic pulses; 500 Hz) were systematically tested to identify the threshold for evoking movement.
Neural recording.
Single-unit recordings were performed using 80-μm-diameter 2–5 MΩ tungsten microelectrodes (FHC), which were reinforced by gluing them inside hypodermic tubing, attached to a micromanipulator (David Kopf Instruments). Spikes were sorted using principal component analysis after the extracellular signals had been digitized at 25 kHz and bandpass filtered between 0.1 and 8 kHz (System 3; Tucker Davis Technologies). Because of the stability of the head-fixed apparatus, single-unit recordings were remarkably stable during trials, and most neurons remained well isolated while the mice intermittently walked on the treadmill and groomed between trials (see Fig. 6D; SNR, 2.9 ± 0.5, mean ± SD; n = 16). The duration of recordings was generally limited more by the number of trials that the mice could perform than by the ability to maintain single-unit isolation, because 56% of neurons remained isolated for >45 min.
Lesions.
In mice used for pharmacological experiments (see above, Pharmacological inactivation), electrolytic lesions were performed as terminal experiments to mark the location targeted by the implanted cannulae. A 10 s pulse of 100 μA cathodal current was delivered through the cut end of an insulated stainless steel wire placed at the same depth as the internal cannula tip. In some of the mice used for neurophysiology experiments (see above, Neural recording), an electrolytic microlesion was made for mapping regions of the AIP nucleus necessary for CR expression. Pulses of cathodal current (10 s) were delivered through the tip of a 2–5 MΩ recording electrode. The intensity of the current was gradually increased in steps of 10 μA until CRs were completely abolished, with the minimum required being 10 μA and the maximum being 50 μA.
Histology and imaging.
Mice were deeply anesthetized with isoflurane and transcardially perfused with 0.1 m PBS, followed by 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 m PBS. Brains were postfixed overnight and cryoprotected in 30% sucrose until they equilibrated (1–2 d). Coronal sections were cut at 50 μm thickness on a cryostat (Leica) and immediately mounted on a glass slide. For mice with lesions (see Lesions), mounted sections were stained with cresyl violet using standard histological procedures, dehydrated, defatted, and coverslipped. For mice injected with fluorescent muscimol (see above, Pharmacological inactivation), mounted sections were stained with NeuroTrace Green Fluorescent Nissl (Invitrogen) using the vendor-recommended protocol with a 100-fold dilution of the stain and coverslipped with Prolong Gold (Invitrogen). Sections were imaged on a wide-field microscope (Zeiss Axioplan 2 or Olympus BX50) under bright-field (cresyl violet) or epifluorescent illumination.
Data analysis.
Data were analyzed in MATLAB using custom-written software. Eyelid traces for each trial were extracted from the video frames using the area (FEC) algorithm, as described above (see High-speed videography). CR amplitudes on paired trials were calculated by subtracting the mean FEC in a 20 ms window preceding the US from the mean FEC in the first 20 ms after the CS. Trials were considered to contain a CR if this amplitude was >10% of full closure. The time of maximum eyelid closure (i.e., the peak time) was calculated for every CS-alone trial with a CR, by taking the time of maximum FEC during the entire duration of the CS. Mean and SDs of peak times for individual mice were estimated from the parameters of Gaussian fits to their peak time distributions.
Results
We have developed a new apparatus for performing eyeblink conditioning in head-fixed mice. The experiments described below were designed with three goals in mind: (1) to assess whether the eyelid CRs of individual mice display two behavioral features that are hallmarks of cerebellar-dependent motor learning tasks; (2) to test the hypothesis that the CRs are entirely driven by cerebellar circuits; and (3) to identify which areas of the DCN are critical for CR expression.
CRs are gradually learned
Figure 3A shows the average eyelid CR amplitude of six mice trained for 14 consecutive sessions with a 250 ms CS–US interval (∼100 trials per session; US onset indicated by arrow). As is evident in these traces, the average CR amplitude gradually built up session after session. Over the same training period, the mice gradually increased the percentage of trials in which they generated a CR (%CR; based on a threshold criterion of 10% eyelid closure), eventually producing CRs in >90% of trials (Fig. 3B). The gradual increases in CR amplitude and %CR were apparent in the trial-by-trial data of individual mice as well (Fig. 3C,D; median number of trials to go from 20% to 80% full eyelid closure, 244; range, 60–414), which indicates that these gradual effects are not simply attributable to averaging (Gallistel et al., 2004). Mice showed a range of learning rates but all reached asymptotic performance by trial 900 (beginning of session 9), reliably closing their eyelids almost all the way (75–80% closure) just before the delivery of the puff US. Importantly, none of the mice showed any evidence of having learned the CR in the first session (Fig. 3D), which is consistent with the slow acquisition rates reported in many motor learning tasks (Schmidt and Lee, 2013) and with previous studies of eyeblink conditioning in other animal species, including rabbits (Schneiderman et al., 1962).
CRs are adaptively timed
To examine the timing of CRs, we started by analyzing conditioned eyelid movements in one of our previous datasets (Chettih et al., 2011). In this experiment, we first trained mice with one of five different CS–US intervals (ISI) and subsequently measured eyelid movements in sessions that had a small number of randomly interleaved CS-alone trials in which we omitted the US (Fig. 4A). For mice trained with the 100 ms ISI, which is close to the minimum eyelid movement latency for a visual CS, the eyelids failed to accelerate fast enough and were <5% closed at the expected time of the US (Fig. 4A, blue arrow). Mice trained with longer ISIs performed better, reaching peak eyelid closure around the expected time of the US (Fig. 4A). Although the CRs were well timed on average, the exact time of peak closure became increasingly variable at longer ISIs (Fig. 4B). This resulted in a linear relationship between the mean and the SD of peak times over a wide range of ISIs (Fig. 4B, bottom), which is a characteristic feature of CRs that is seen in other species as well (Gallistel and Gibbon, 2000; White et al., 2000; Vogel et al., 2003). Interestingly, the amount of variability in the timing of the CR was more strongly correlated with the mean time of maximum eyelid closure than the particular ISI used for training (Fig. 4B, bottom, arrows). However, in contrast to rabbits (Mauk and Ruiz, 1992), the mice did not appear to regulate CR timing by changing the latency of the response (see analysis within Chettih et al., 2011).
The data in Figure 4, A and B, provide a between-animal comparison but do not answer whether individual mice can adjust the timing of CRs adaptively. To assess this possibility, we designed two additional experiments. First, we trained mice with one ISI for many consecutive sessions and then switched the ISI to a different value. We found that the timing of CRs shifted gradually over the course of several sessions and eventually reached an asymptote that was appropriate for the new ISI, when the ISI was shifted both from longer to shorter or shorter to longer values (Fig. 4C). Second, we trained individual mice on a differential conditioning task in which one CS (light) was paired with the US at one interval in 50% of the trials in a session, and a different CS (vibrissa) was paired with the US at a second interval in the remaining 50% of the trials. As shown in the average eyelid position traces of CS-alone trials, for each of the last five sessions of conditioning (Fig. 4D), mice learned to generate CRs that were appropriately timed for the particular CS presented on any given trial. Overall, our results clearly demonstrate that, as is the case in other species (Kehoe and Napier, 1991; Mauk and Ruiz, 1992), head-fixed mice in our apparatus learn to adaptively regulate the timing of the CR and achieve maximum eyelid closure at the time of the US under a wide range of stimulus contingencies. Importantly, we did not observe the fixed short-latency eyelid movements that have been reported in freely moving mice trained with an auditory CS (Vogel et al., 2002; Boele et al., 2010).
CRs are cerebellar dependent
In 12 mice, we used pharmacological inactivation to assess the contribution of the cerebellum to CR expression. This pharmacological approach has been used to demonstrate that CRs are entirely driven by cerebellar circuits in rabbits (Clark et al., 1992; Krupa et al., 1993; Christian and Thompson, 2003) and rats (Freeman et al., 2005). After training the mice for several sessions (light CS; 250 ms ISI), we infused the GABA agonist muscimol (1 mm, 0.10–0.15 μl) through an implanted cannula targeting anterior regions of the DCN. Before the infusion, the mice performed at least 30 trials to help establish a baseline performance (Fig. 5A,D; blue). For mice with cannulae implanted 5.8–6.0 mm posterior to bregma (Fig. 5C, circles; n = 7), infusion in the vicinity of AIP, LN, or DLH caused a complete wipeout of the CR beginning on the first or second trial after the infusion and lasting for the remainder of the session (Fig. 5A,D; red arrow in A indicates time of infusion). This effect was not simply attributable to a nonspecific response to the infusion procedure because vehicle infusions had no effect (Fig. 5D), and infusion of the short-lasting sodium channel blocker lidocaine through the same cannula (4%, 0.10–0.15 μl) resulted in similar immediate effects on CR amplitude (Fig. 5B,D, red), but the CRs recovered within 15–20 min (Fig. 5B,D, green). In addition, the mice continued to blink normally in response to the periocular air-puff stimulus during all our infusions (Fig. 5A,B), which excludes the possibility that the wipeout of the CR was caused by a generalized impairment in motor performance.
Two additional observations provide some hints about the regions of DCN that are critical for CR expression. First, infusion of fluorescent muscimol (1 mm, 0.15 μl) in three of the mice revealed that the drug diffused only a few hundred micrometers from the infusion site in the AIP (diameter of spread, 416, 384, and 210 μm, measured as 2 SDs along the dimension of maximum spread), as shown for one of these mice in Figure 5E. Second, infusions of muscimol near the middle cerebellar peduncle 5.5–5.7 mm from bregma or near the posterior interpositus 6.4–6.7 mm from bregma (Fig. 5C, × symbols; n = 5) partially suppressed but did not wipe out the CR immediately or completely. These results suggest that, in our experiments, the areas of DCN necessary for generating CRs are located toward the most anterior regions of the AIP, similar to what has been observed in rabbits (Lavond et al., 1984b; Yeo et al., 1985).
The anterolateral AIP is critical for CR expression
To pinpoint the precise area of DCN responsible for the expression of CRs, we performed a series of electrophysiological mapping experiments. The strategy we used highlights the advantages of our head-fixed system for combining behavioral measurements with a variety of acute electrophysiological techniques. First, we made multiple stereotaxically guided electrode penetrations to map a small region of the AIP from which discrete, graded eyelid closure could be reliably evoked using electrical microstimulation with very low currents (see Materials and Methods). As shown in the eyelid traces and heat maps for two representative mice (Fig. 6A,B), the hotspot for evoking blinks was centered ∼1.5–1.8 mm lateral of the midline and was limited to the most anterior portion of the DCN (note that the anterior limit of DCN is located 5.6 mm posterior from bregma; Fig. 5C). We then used a recording electrode to search for neurons in this area whose activity was strongly modulated during the generation of CRs.
Figure 6C shows the response of an example neuron that was recorded in the hotspot during paired trials. This neuron produced a large excitatory response that preceded the CR, a pattern of modulation that has been observed for AIP neurons in rabbits (McCormick and Thompson, 1984b; Berthier and Moore, 1990) and rats (Freeman and Nicholson, 2000). We found a similar pattern across the population of neurons recorded near this location, as seen in the population average (Fig. 6E; mean ± SD lead, 19.1 ± 10.4 ms; n = 16). In three mice, we made a small electrolytic lesion (10–50 μA direct current, 10 s) through the recording electrode immediately after finding a single unit with CR-related activation. In all cases, the lesions completely abolished CRs (Fig. 6F, right), and the CRs remained absent the following day. Histological examination revealed that these lesions were all confined to the same small region of the anterolateral AIP abutting the LN and DLH (Fig. 6F, left; maximum mediolateral extent of lesions, 207, 294, and 301 μm). This is the same small area of DCN that has been shown to be essential for the initial learning and subsequent expression of CRs in rabbits (Christian and Thompson, 2003).
Discussion
We have introduced a novel experimental apparatus to examine the behavioral properties of eyeblink conditioning in head-fixed mice and to identify which areas of the DCN are critical for CR expression. Our results demonstrate that the behavior of individual mice displays two hallmark features of motor learning tasks (Schmidt and Lee, 2013): (1) eyelid CRs were acquired slowly over the course of many training trials; and (2) their timing was adaptively controlled. Furthermore, we found a small region in the most anterior portion of the DCN that was necessary for generating eyelid CRs in well trained mice. Below, we discuss our findings in the context of the existing mouse and rabbit eyeblink literature and highlight some of the key advantages of our head-fixed system for future experiments.
Similarities between mouse eyeblink conditioning and motor learning
A hallmark of motor learning is that improvements in performance occur gradually and often require multiple training sessions (Squire, 1986; Schmidt and Lee, 2013). During eyeblink conditioning, for example, CRs are acquired slowly over the course of many conditioning trials in a variety of animal species, including humans (Hilgard and Campbell, 1936), monkeys (Hilgard and Marquis, 1936), dogs (Hilgard and Marquis, 1935), ferrets (Ivarsson and Svensson, 2000), rabbits (Schneiderman et al., 1962), and rats (Skelton, 1988). In contrast, previous studies have reported that CR acquisition is much faster in mice and that performance can sometimes reach asymptote in the first session of conditioning (Vogel et al., 2002; Woodruff-Pak, 2006). It has been suggested that this rapid learning indicates the recruitment of additional neural circuits involved in fear conditioning (Boele et al., 2010). Such recruitment would open up an interesting avenue of research for looking at interactions between multiple systems (Lavond et al., 1984a; Neufeld and Mintz, 2001; Lee and Kim, 2004), but it would also diminish the utility of eyeblink conditioning as a behavioral model for studying neural mechanisms of motor learning in isolation. In our head-fixed apparatus, mice acquired the eyelid CR gradually over many trials and did not show any signs of learning in the first session of conditioning. The learning rate in our experiments was very similar to what has been reported in previous studies of eyeblink conditioning in rabbits (Schneiderman et al., 1962; Medina et al., 2001), which suggests that we were able to isolate the motor learning component of the task.
Mice trained in our head-fixed apparatus also displayed an additional behavioral signature of many motor learning tasks: adaptive timing (Schmidt and Lee, 2013). We found that individual mice learned to adjust the kinematic properties of the CR with millisecond precision, achieving maximum eyelid closure at the expected time of the US across a wide range of CS–US intervals and stimulus contingencies. To control CR timing, mice regulated the speed of the eyelid movement but not the latency to onset (Chettih et al., 2011), which differs from what is normally seen in other species, such as rabbits (Mauk and Ruiz, 1992), and may indicate distinct underlying mechanisms. Importantly, we did not observe the short-latency eyelid movements often reported in previous studies of eyeblink conditioning with freely moving mice (Aiba et al., 1994; Kishimoto et al., 2001, 2002; Koekkoek et al., 2003; Wada et al., 2007; Sakamoto and Endo, 2010). These short-latency responses (SLRs) are thought to originate in the amygdala (Boele et al., 2010; Sakamoto and Endo, 2010) and have a fixed onset regardless of CS–US interval that can complicate attempts to analyze CR timing with precision (Koekkoek et al., 2003; Boele et al., 2010). The lack of SLRs in our head-fixed apparatus provides an opportunity to follow up on groundbreaking studies about the molecular basis of motor timing in mice (Koekkoek et al., 2003), without interference from fear-related processes.
Eyelid CRs are entirely driven by the cerebellum
Much of the success that eyeblink conditioning has enjoyed as a behavioral model for studying the neural mechanisms of motor learning can be traced back to pioneering work demonstrating that the cerebellum is necessary for CR expression in rabbits (McCormick et al., 1982; Lavond et al., 1984b; McCormick and Thompson, 1984a; Krupa et al., 1993). Whether the cerebellum is also necessary for CR expression in mice has been less clear. A number of studies have shown that lesions or pharmacological inactivation of the DCN in freely moving mice impair CRs but do not wipe them out completely (Koekkoek et al., 2003, 2005; Sakamoto and Endo, 2008, 2010), suggesting that other noncerebellar sites, including the amygdala, play a prominent role (Boele et al., 2010; Sakamoto and Endo, 2010). In contrast, we found that, for mice trained in our head-fixed apparatus, eyelid CRs were entirely driven by cells located in the most anterior part of lateral AIP. This location coincides with the areas of DCN that are necessary for CR expression in rabbits (McCormick and Thompson, 1984a; Yeo et al., 1985) and is also in close proximity to areas of DLH and AIP that undergo structural plasticity during eyeblink conditioning in freely moving mice (Boele et al., 2013). Furthermore, this same region of DCN receives input from Purkinje cells capable of driving eyeblinks (Heiney et al., 2014). Our results are the first to pinpoint a critical component of the neural circuitry mediating the expression of CRs in mice, thus providing a target for genetic manipulation in future experiments looking to dissect the contribution of cerebellar mechanisms to motor learning.
Advantages of our head-fixed apparatus
Head-fixed preparations provide unparalleled reliability for controlling stimulus delivery and for simultaneously monitoring behavioral and neural activity in awake animals (Schwarz et al., 2010), which is why they are commonly used for neurophysiological studies in species that tolerate restraint well (Evarts, 1968; McCormick and Thompson, 1984a; Populin and Yin, 1998). In contrast, systems for head fixation in awake mice have lagged behind and have only recently begun to surface (Guo et al., 2014), partly because immobilizing these animals is more difficult and can sometimes cause stress-related impairments in behavior (Paré and Glavin, 1986; Li et al., 2012). One of the objectives in our study was to establish a paradigm for training head-fixed mice in a classical eyeblink conditioning task and to showcase some of its advantages over current freely moving systems (De Zeeuw et al., 2004).
Because our head-fixed system is suitable for high-speed videography, we were able to monitor the eyelid movements of our mice with unprecedented spatial and temporal resolution. Unlike EMG methods in freely moving systems (Aiba et al., 1994; Chen et al., 1996; Kishimoto et al., 2001, 2002; Vogel et al., 2002; Woodruff-Pak, 2006; Wada et al., 2007; Sakamoto and Endo, 2008, 2010), which do not measure movement directly and in small animals like mice can be contaminated by the contraction of nearby facial muscles (Koekkoek et al., 2002), high-speed video provides a veridical readout of actual eyelid position. This makes it possible, for instance, to quantify the kinematics of the movement and to correlate them with neural activity (Heiney et al., 2014). Other techniques, such as those that rely on gluing a small magnet on the eyelid (Koekkoek et al., 2002; Chettih et al., 2011), provide excellent temporal resolution as well, but they add inertia to the motor plant and suffer from nonlinearities that complicate measurements (Koekkoek et al., 2002; De Zeeuw et al., 2004). In addition, our video system is the only method that can be used to resolve the positions of the top and bottom eyelid separately, thus enabling future investigations about the neural control and coordination of agonist/antagonist muscles (Evinger, 1995; Delgado-García et al., 2003).
One of the key advantages of head fixation is that it affords the structural stability necessary for performing neurophysiological and imaging experiments that would be much more challenging in freely moving mice (Schwarz et al., 2010; Chen et al., 2013). In the current study, we used a combination of pharmacological inactivation, electrical microstimulation, and single-unit recordings to functionally map the location of eyeblink-related regions in the mouse DCN. Previously, we had shown that the same head-fixed apparatus is also suitable for other neurophysiological approaches during nonconditioning experiments in awake mice, including optogenetic manipulation of cell-specific neural populations (Heiney et al., 2014) and two-photon calcium imaging of Purkinje cells (Najafi et al., 2014). This versatility, combined with the elegant simplicity of eyeblink conditioning and the powerful genetic tools available for mice (Heintz, 2001; Huang and Zeng, 2013), makes our head-fixed system an ideal tool for dissecting the neural mechanisms underlying motor learning in the vertebrate brain.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the Searle Scholars Program and National Institutes of Health Grant R01 MH093727 (J.F.M.). We thank K. Ohmae and D. Subramanian for assistance with behavioral data collection and histology. S. Wang, I. Ozden, A. Giovannucci, and F. Najafi helped develop the original “floating ball” apparatus for eyeblink conditioning on which our “rotating treadmill” is based. S. Koekkoek, H-J. Boele, and C. DeZeeuw provided technical advice and helped with early versions of the eyeblink conditioning set up.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Javier Medina, Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, 3720 Walnut Street, Philadelphia, PA 19104. jmed{at}psych.upenn.edu