Abstract
During movement, there is a transition of activity across the population, such that place-field centers ahead of the rat are sequentially activated in the order that they will be encountered. Although the mechanisms responsible for this sequence updating are unknown, two classes of models can be considered. The first class involves head-direction information for activating neurons in the order that their place fields will be traversed. An alternative model contends that motion and turn-related information from the posterior parietal cortex shift the subset of active hippocampal cells across the population. To explicitly test these two models, rodents were trained to run backward on a linear track, placing movement in opposition with head orientation. Although head-direction did not change between running conditions, place-field activity remapped and there was an increase in place-field size during backward running compared with forward. The population activity, however, could still be used to reconstruct the location of the rat accurately. Moreover, theta phase precession was maintained in both running conditions, indicating preservation of place-field sequences on short-time scales. The observation that sequence encoding persists even when the animal is orientated away from the direction of movement favors the concept that posterior parietal cortical mechanisms may be partially responsible for updating hippocampal activity patterns.
Introduction
During movement through an environment, principal cells of the hippocampus express a firing pattern tuned to spatial location, referred to as the neuron's place field (O'Keefe and Dostrovsky, 1971). This is accompanied by a prominent 4–12 Hz oscillation in the local field potential, termed the theta rhythm (Vanderwolf, 1969; Buzsàki and Eidelberg, 1983). Importantly, within each theta cycle, the active population of neurons with common place fields dynamically changes (O'Keefe and Recce, 1993; Harris, 2005). This propagation of activity is sequentially organized, such that neurons with maximum firing behind the rat fire before neurons with place fields ahead of the rat, providing a “look-ahead” of upcoming spatial locations (Burgess et al., 1994; Skaggs et al., 1996; Tsodyks et al., 1996; Maurer et al., 2012). Furthermore, between adjacent theta cycles, as neurons with receptive field locations behind the rat become inactive, cells with fields representing locations ahead of the animal begin to fire. The consequence of the sequential updating of active cell assemblies are that neurons will exhibit a progressive shifting of spikes relative to the hippocampal theta rhythm (Dragoi and Buzsàki, 2006; Foster and Wilson, 2007; Geisler et al., 2010; Maurer et al., 2012). This phenomenon, termed theta phase precession (O'Keefe and Recce, 1993), is thought to be a fundamental mechanism of spatial ensemble sequence encoding (Lisman and Idiart, 1995; Jensen and Lisman, 1996; Skaggs et al., 1996; Lisman and Buzsàki, 2008), as well as nonspatial information (Harris et al., 2002; Harris, 2005), such as episodic recall and action planning (Pastalkova et al., 2008). The afferent drive responsible for updating hippocampal representations, and thus phase precession, however, are unknown.
During a random foraging task, it has been shown that the look-ahead is selectively oriented toward the rodent's upcoming movement direction, suggesting that head-direction is a primary mechanism for updating network activity (Huxter et al., 2008). Moreover, attractor models of hippocampal activity dynamics traditionally use a combination of head-direction (Taube et al., 1990) and self-motion information to move the distribution of activity in the network (Redish and Touretzky, 1997; Samsonovich and McNaughton, 1997; McNaughton et al., 2006). These models place a significant dependence on the egocentric head-direction signal, necessitating that the movement trajectory is aligned with head orientation to appropriately move the “bump” of activity. Alternatively, velocity and motion information from the parietal cortex could be responsible for appropriately updating the hippocampal network (Conklin and Eliasmith, 2005). As the activity of neurons in the posterior parietal cortex are sensitive to simple motion states (McNaughton et al., 1994; Burke et al., 2005) that precede the actual onset of movement up to 500 ms (Whitlock et al., 2012). Thus, anticipatory motor planning could be responsible for the hippocampal “look ahead” phenomenon (Maurer and McNaughton, 2007). By training rats to run forward, as well as backward, on a linear track, the present study sought to test the dissociating predictions of the two models when a rodent's movement trajectory is decoupled from the direction that it was currently facing (Conklin and Eliasmith, 2005).
Materials and Methods
Animals and surgical procedures.
Four Fischer-344 male rats (8- to 12-months-old, RRID: F344/DuCrl Rat) were used in this study. Rats were housed individually and maintained on a 12 h light/dark cycle. Recordings took place during the dark phase. Surgery was conducted according to NIH guidelines for rodents and approved Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee protocols. The rats were implanted, under isoflurane anesthesia, with a “hyperdrive” array of 12 separately moveable tetrodes. Two additional tetrodes, with wires shorted together, served as references. For all rats, hippocampal recordings were made from CA1 (3.0 mm posterior to bregma, 1.4 mm lateral to the midline).
Electrophysiological recording.
After surgery, tetrodes were lowered and allowed to stabilize for several days above CA1, and then gradually advanced into the stratum pyramidale. A reference electrode was placed in or near the corpus callosum. Each tetrode was attached to four separate channels of a 50-channel unity-gain head stage (Neuralynx). A multiwire cable connected the head stage to digitally programmable amplifiers (Neuralynx). The spike signals were amplified (gain of 1000–5000), bandpass filtered between 600 Hz and 6 kHz, and transmitted to the Cheetah Data Acquisition system (Neuralynx). Signals were digitized at 32 kHz, and events that reached a predetermined threshold were recorded for 1 ms. Spikes were sorted offline by means of the semiautomatic clustering algorithm KlustaKwik (K. D. Harris, University College, London, RRID: nif-0000-10182). The resulting classification was refined manually with custom-written software (MClust; A. D. Redish, University of Minnesota), resulting in spike-train time series for each of the well isolated cells.
Pyramidal neurons were identified using the standard parameters of firing rate, burstiness, spike waveform characteristics (Ranck, 1973), and the first moment of the autocorrelation (Csicsvari et al., 1998). EEG signals were bandpass filtered between 1 and 300 Hz, sampled at 2.4 kHz, and amplified on the head stage with unity gain, and then again with variable gain amplifiers (up to 5000). Several light-emitting diodes were mounted on the head stage to allow position tracking. The position of the diode array was detected by a television camera placed above the experimental apparatus, recorded with a sampling frequency of 60 Hz, and a spatial resolution was 0.2 cm/pixel.
Behavior.
The animals were food deprived to 85% of ad libitum weight. During this time, rats were shaped to walk forward and backward on a narrow linear track. The rat was placed on the track and allowed to run forward to obtain food reward. After eating, without letting the rats turn around, the trainer would gently place a plastic spoon against their nose. If the rat stepped back, a small food-reward was immediately presented. This shaping continued until the rats were fully trained to cover the entire track for reward. Recording began after surgery recovery. Each running session was bookended by a rest epoch of at least 20 min to assess baseline firing and stability.
Analyses.
Instantaneous velocity was determined by convolving the position data over multiple frames and then calculating the absolute value of the difference in position. Track locations where occupancy exceeded 10% of the total time were removed, which was always associated with reward. The position data were then “unwrapped” to examine forward and reverse directions separately. Lap duration was calculated as the time from leaving one reward location and entering the other rewarded area.
Place-field diagrams and phase-precession diagrams (Fig. 1) were constructed as described by Maurer et al. (2006a,b). Briefly, each running trajectory, forwards versus backward ambulation were plotted separately to control for the unidirectional nature of place fields during track running (McNaughton et al., 1983). Spikes were binned by location on the track into 2.5 cm bin widths, and rates were normalized by bin occupancy time (Maurer et al., 2006a,b). To construct the theta phase of firing versus position plots, each spike was assigned a phase relative to the hippocampal theta rhythm using methods similar to those of Belluscio et al. (2012; described below). The spikes were binned into a two dimensional matrix, convolved, and normalized by occupancy to provide an occupancy normalized phase-position density plot. To display the position of each spike, lap-by-lap spike rasters were created by linearly interpolating the spike times of a neuron onto the rodents position-and-time matrix.
Spatial population vectors were constructed in the same manner as by Maurer et al. (2012) with the exception that spatial bins were reduced to 0.33 cm (compared with 0.7 cm) and were generated on a lap-by-lap basis. The rationale for constructing lap-by-lap population vectors, as opposed to collapsing across all laps, was to attempt to control for the higher variability in the mean position of spikes across laps in backwards running. Briefly, to generate the population vector matrix, the spatial firing rate distributions of all recorded pyramidal neurons in a given condition were combined into a single, two-dimensional array: cell number in rows and linearized location in the columns. Each column therefore represented an estimate of the composite population vector for the corresponding location. The vectors for the forward and reverse running directions were computed separately. To reconstruct the position of the rat, one spatial population vector matrix was selected as the reference and was then correlated against every other spatial population vector for the other laps. This process was repeated until each lap was correlated against all other laps, excluding the autocorrelation of the reference lap with itself, providing a reconstructed position for each lap-combination.
Inspection of our data using the lap-by-lap rastergrams and phase by position density plots (Fig. 1), suggests that during backward running, place fields may undergo a mild drift or dispersion. This observation is similar to findings from experiments in which rodents forage in dark arenas (Markus et al., 1994; Hafting et al., 2005) or when landmarks are perceived to be unstable (Knierim et al., 1995). To measure the consistency of place-field location on a lap-to-lap basis, we applied firing rate boundaries to the spatial firing rate distributions (Muller et al., 1987; Thompson and Best, 1990; cf. Maurer et al., 2006b). Spikes across all laps were binned (2.5 cm bin size) and occupancy normalized. To be included in the analysis, the firing rate of the neuron needed to exceed 10% of the mean firing rate for 11 or more contiguous bins (for a minimum place-field size of 27.5 cm, which was below the size of place fields observed in forward running conditions estimated from the population vector correlation). Using the boundaries of the regions that passed the firing rate criteria, the lap-by-lap center of mass of firing in the field was calculated (Mehta et al., 1997; Shen et al., 1997; Ekstrom et al., 2001; Lee et al., 2004; Burke et al., 2008). To measure whether place-field centers varied from lap-to-lap, we calculated the SD of the center-of-mass across all laps. In the backward condition, the SD of the center-of-mass was significantly larger (3.34 cm, SEM ± 0.16) relative to the forward running condition (2.69 cm, SEM ± 0.17; t(3) = 3.05, p < 0.05). This quantitatively supports our qualitative impression that the place field-location undergoes a modest jitter across laps in the backward running condition relative to forward running. In light of this, we considered methods of determining theta phase precession.
Although there were specific examples where spike phase versus position shows clear precession during backward running (Fig. 1, left column row 4) in most cases the plots of spike phase versus position during backward running did not show clear precession, due to the higher variability in place-field position during backward running. However, we demonstrate consistent theta phase precession versus time (see Figs. 3, 6).
Although not an explicit measure of theta phase precession, prior studies have used phase-position correlations to infer theta phase precession (Maurer et al., 2006b; Burke et al., 2008, 2011). This analysis, however, does not demonstrate that the spike times shift their phase relative to the hippocampal theta rhythm, but only provides a means to determine the slope of precession in space and the coefficient of determination (how well the variation in one variable explains the variance in another). Moreover, as the center-of-mass is higher during backward running compared with forward traversals, meaning that the position of spikes cannot be treated as a fixed value (violating the weak exogeneity assumption). There are a number of other factors that make regression analysis an inappropriate model for determining spike phase-location relationships. These include the fact that linear–linear and circular–linear regression analyses are vulnerable to outliers in the data (Diba and Buzsàki, 2008), have difficulty in quantifying bimodal or nonlinear data (Kempter et al., 2012), and violate the basic statistical assumptions of homoscedasticity and normality. That is, phase variance is not equal across all positions (i.e., are not homoscedastic; Yamaguchi et al., 2002), and place fields can have a strong kurtosis (i.e., are not normally distributed; Mehta et al., 1997, 2000). Because of these factors, other techniques were required to quantify theta phase precession that are independent of position information (Harris et al., 2002; Pastalkova et al., 2008).
Our first nonspatial measure of theta phase precession was based on the analysis first developed by Harris et al. (2002), aimed at determining phase shifts that occur between the onset and offset of firing periods of neuronal spiking. For the present study, spikes in the backward and forward directions that belonged to an epoch when firing rate was increasing (the previous 8 theta cycles contained no more than 4 spikes, whereas the following 8 theta cycles contain at least 16 spikes) or decreasing (the previous 8 theta cycles contained 16 or more spikes, whereas the following 8 theta cycles contain 4 spikes or fewer; Harris et al., 2002) were determined. The rationale for these values is that, assuming theta is 8 Hz, the spike frequency increases either from 4 to 16 Hz (or vice versa for the decreasing epochs). By calculating the mean circular phase of the onset and offset of spikes, it was possible to identify whether the spikes advanced relative to the theta rhythm.
The second phase precession analysis involved comparing the autocorrelogram frequency of individual neurons to the theta frequency across different running velocities (O'Keefe and Recce, 1993). To calculate the autocorrelograms by velocity, the animal's velocity at the occurrence of each spike was determined in bins of 10 cm/s. A moving window method was used to optimize the calculation of the spike autocorrelograms and theta peak autocorrelation frequency. To calculate the frequency of autocorrelograms, spectra were calculated using a multitaper method (Geisler et al., 2007).
Finally, a novel method for quantifying theta phase precession across the entire population of recorded CA1 cells was developed, the theta-time population vector autocorrelogram. This temporal population vector was constructed in a manner similar to the position population vector, with n cells by t temporal bins. Time was binned into ∼14° of a theta cycle with 546 bins over 21 consecutive theta cycles centered on a single theta trough. The population vector within each theta bin was then correlated with all other bins across the 20 theta cycles. To account for the asymmetry of hippocampal theta oscillation (Buzsàki et al., 1983) in the determination of neuronal firing phase, we used methods similar to those of Belluscio et al. (2012). The tetrode that recorded the most pyramidal neurons was selected as a theta reference. To “seed” our initial peak/trough finding algorithm, we digitally filtered the EEG, bidirectionally in time to avoid phase shifts, with a 6–8 Hz Chebyshev bandpass filter. In these conditions, theta approximates a sinusoid facilitating peak determination by using the derivative of the oscillation. Next, we found the absolute minimum in the 1–80 Hz filtered EEG that fell between the time stamps of two peaks in the 6–8 Hz filtered data. These troughs were saved and used to find the absolute maximum in the 1–80 Hz trace that fell between the timestamps of two troughs (Belluscio et al., 2012).
Unless otherwise indicated, for all summary statistics means were calculated for each rat for comparisons between forward and backward running conditions and significance was tested using paired-samples t tests with α ≤ 0.05. The positions at which rats receive reward were excluded from the analysis.
The degree to which the place fields showed stable location-specific firing on repeated runs in the same direction, as well as runs in opposite movement directions (bidirectionality) were assessed by calculating a standardized measurement of place-field overlap (S; Battaglia et al., 2004) as follows: P̄lap is the normalized firing rate vector composed of the firing rate at each position bin on the linear track divided by the sum of firing rates across bins for a given lap. P̄lap is calculated for all laps. This overlap measure is invariant for rescaling of the place-field profiles, and it assumes the limit values of 0 for nonoverlapping place-field profiles and of 1 for profiles that are identical. The s value was computed for all pairwise lap combinations. These values were then averaged for each cell to get a single measure of field stability. To attain a measurement of chance field overlap, the s values were then calculated for each pairwise lap combination for each cell compared with all other cells. Finally, the field overlap of each cell was z-transformed using the chance overlap mean and SD to attain the degree of field overlap that was above chance (Z > 1.6).
Results
There were qualitative differences between how rats traversed the track forward and backward. For instance, rats tended to lower their nose toward the track and use whiskers to determine the edges (Movie 1). Moreover, the amount of time the rat spent in reverse ambulation (mean: 35.0 min, SEM ± 7.3) tended to be longer compared with forward ambulation (mean: 18.8 min, SEM ± 3.9), although this difference was not statistically significant (t(3) = 2.25, p = 0.15). After extensive training, the rats did not show a statistical difference in their average velocities during forward (mean: 13.4 cm/s, SEM ± 2.6) compared with backward (mean: 7.4 cm/s, SEM ± 0.4) runs (t(3) = 3.46, p = 0.08). Nonetheless, when comparing the highest 10% of velocities, there was a significant difference between running forward (mean: 41.6 cm/s, SEM ± 6.5) and backward (mean: 15.8 cm/s, SEM ± 1.1) conditions (t(3) = 7.11, p < 0.05). These data indicate that the rats spent more time running at faster speeds in the forward relative to the backward running conditions.
Only those cells with an information per spike score >0.2 (Skaggs et al., 1993), a spatial coherence score >0.7 (Muller et al., 1987), a minimum average of 10 spikes per lap, and a peak firing rate >110% of the average firing rate for both running directions were included. A total of 437 of 1033 cells met the inclusion criteria. Figure 1 shows examples of single-unit activity. In traditional track-shuttling tasks, place-field activity is uncorrelated between inbound and outbound journeys (McNaughton et al., 1983), whereas place fields are omnidirectional in random foraging conditions (Muller et al., 1994). The reason for this discrepancy is unknown. The current study hypothesized that a rodent, in the same physical location, with the same head orientation, should have the same place-field activity independent of direction of travel. Therefore, place fields should be bidirectional as a rat ambulates forward and reverse on a linear track. To test this idea, a spatial population matrix was computed for every 0.33 cm spatial bin within each lap and then correlated with the matrices from all other laps and spatial bins (Gothard et al., 1996) within each running condition and between the forward and backward runs. Average bidirectional correlation values never exceeded 0.04, and were significantly reduced relative to the correlation values across laps for the forward (t(3) = 4.5, p < 0.02) and backward (t(3) = 12.9, p < 0.001) running conditions alone. Contrary to the hypothesis, the data indicated place-field remapping between behavioral conditions, despite persistent head orientation (Fig. 2). In addition, hippocampal cells exhibited higher correlations over a larger area in reverse traversals relative to forward (Fig. 2A,B; 35.7 cm during backward runs, 17.9 cm for forward runs; t(3) = 5.8, p < 0.01). The overall mean correlation across laps between the forward (r = 0.22) and backward (r = 0.20) directions, however, was not significantly different from each other (t(3) = 1.6, p = 0.21), indicating similar stability for the population as a whole across laps in both conditions (Fig. 2C).
The population vectors of spatial activity for one lap can also be used to “reconstruct” the rat's position in other laps by finding the location of maximum correlation (Wilson and McNaughton, 1993). The position of maximum correlation tended to fall around unity in both forward and backward running conditions (Fig. 2D). Although place fields were larger during backward running, the mean reconstruction error between the forward and reverse directions was not significantly different (t(3) = 0.8, p = 0.47). This indicates that, despite reduced spatial specificity and decreased consistency in the locations of place-field centers across laps (see Materials and Methods), the population activity was able to update the rat's position similarly in both directions.
Although the population of hippocampal neurons on broad timescales is able to reconstruct the rodent's position in space, we were also interested to determine whether the short-time scale temporal dynamics were maintained during backward running. To explicitly measure the advancement of spikes relative to the hippocampal theta rhythm, we used three nonspatial analyses. By comparing the phase of spikes that lead an epoch of high firing rate versus spikes that fell on the tail end of a high firing rate epoch (Harris et al., 2002; see Materials and Methods) one can determine whether the phase of onset of spikes occurs later relative to the phase of offset bursts during forward and backward running (Fig. 3). In both the forward (F(1, 5125) = 66.76) and reverse (F(1,4192) = 57.04) running conditions, the phase of spikes at the end of a high firing rate epoch occurred earlier than the spikes at the beginning of the high firing rate epoch (p < 000.1; pairwise Mardia-Watson-Wheeler test).
Pyramidal cells showed theta-modulated firing in both forward (mean: 0.6 depth of modulation, SEM ± 0.25) and backward (mean: 0.6 depth of modulation, SEM ± 0.25) running conditions with no significant difference in the depth of modulation (t(3) = 1.08, p = 0.48). As initially noted by O'Keefe and Recce (1993), the occurrence of theta phase precession requires that the burst frequency of neurons is slightly faster than the hippocampal theta rhythm (Fig. 4). Therefore, it is possible to demonstrate that a neuron's spike time shifts relative to the theta oscillation if the frequency of the spike autocorrelogram is faster than the frequency of the autocorrelogram of theta peak times (Geisler et al., 2007). Figure 5 shows the mean values of the frequency of the autocorrelograms for the cells versus theta during forward and backward running. These frequency values were subject to a two-way factorial ANOVA with the factors of running condition (forward vs backward) and autocorrelogram (spikes vs theta). The main effect of autocorrelogram was significant (F(1,70) = 541.56, p < 0.001), such that the frequency of the spike autocorrelogram was significantly faster compared with theta. Running condition, however, did not significantly affect the overall autocorrelogram frequency (F(1,70) = 0.57, p < 0.43). The interaction between running condition and autocorrelogram was also significant (F(1,70) = 12.47, p < 0.01). Post hoc analysis showed that during both forward and backward running there was a significant difference between the frequency of the spike and theta autocorrelograms (p < 0.001 for both comparisons; Tukey). This indicates that the cells precessed relative to theta in both running conditions. Importantly, the frequency of the theta autocorrelogram did not significantly differ between running conditions (p = 0.33; Tukey), but cell autocorrelogram frequency was significantly greater during forward running relative to backward (p < 0.05; Tukey). This is consistent with the observation that place-field size was significantly larger during backward running relative to forward as indicated by the spatial population vector correlation.
To evaluate the effect of backward ambulation on the theta phase precession across the recorded ensemble, population activity was temporally binned relative to the hippocampal theta oscillation and correlated across multiple theta cycles (Fig. 6A). The phase of maximum correlation for the trough of the two cycles preceding and following the reference cycle was then calculated. This method quantifies how the population activity changes over successive theta cycles. During forward running laps, the average phase shift over five cycles was 50°, which was significantly different from 0 (t(3) = 3.2, p < 0.05; one-sample t test). Moreover, during the backward running laps the total phase shift was 31° and this was also significantly different from 0 (t(3) = 5.8, p < 0.01; one-sample t test). The forward and backward phase shifts were not statistically different from each other, however (t(3) = 1.38, p < 0.3), suggesting that the population dynamics of CA1 neurons show theta phase precession in both running directions (Fig. 6B,C).
Discussion
By decoupling a rodent's movement direction from its head orientation, the current experiment tested two competing attractor network models. One class of models uses head-direction, and self-motion signals, to shift the ensemble of active neurons to fields located ahead of the rodent during movement (Redish and Touretzky, 1997; Samsonovich and McNaughton, 1997; McNaughton et al., 2006). The second model proposes that a velocity vector, potentially from the parietal cortex, provides directional input to the hippocampal network (Conklin and Eliasmith, 2005). If a rat were to run backward, in opposition to the direction it is facing, there are two potential outcomes, each lending support to one model over the other. The head-direction hypothesis would predict that hippocampal spatial activity should be distorted or destroyed during backward running. Alternatively, the posterior parietal model of hippocampal updating suggests that the place code should be maintained regardless of direction of ambulation (Conklin and Eliasmith, 2005). This is not to suggest that other brain regions involved in movement information, such as the prefrontal cortex, striatum, or cerebellum, are not also critical for updating hippocampal activity patterns.
Although there was a higher variance of center-of-mass across laps during backward running, the rat's position reconstruction could be obtained from the population of active cells. Moreover, this was comparable to the reconstruction accuracy obtained during forward running. Furthermore, intact theta phase precession observed during both forward and backward running, as indicated by two independent single-cell analyses and a population-based temporal vector correlation measure, is evidence that the network activity can be appropriately updated on short-time scales as well. Therefore, the present data favor the parietal mechanism of updating.
As place fields were significantly larger in backward compared with forward running, which could be interpreted as a distortion of the map, this finding could potentially support models of head-direction mechanisms for hippocampal updating. An alternate explanation, however, is that the increase in place-field size is a result of contradictory velocity processing between the parietal cortex (Whitlock et al., 2012) and the head-direction cells, which are known to increase their firing rates with linear velocity (Taube et al., 1990). In the parietal model of hippocampal updating, head-direction neurons govern the velocity input into the network (Conklin and Eliasmith, 2005). As sensitivity to incoming self-motion information, inferred by velocity-firing rate relationships, has been suggested to govern hippocampal place-field size (Maurer et al., 2005; Terrazas et al., 2005), it is feasible that the head-direction system manages the flow of parietal velocity input to the hippocampal network. During reverse ambulation, this signal is mitigated. Moreover, place cells have been described as speed-controlled oscillators, in which their autocorrelation burst frequency increases with velocity (O'Keefe and Recce, 1993; Geisler et al., 2007). In line with this idea, pyramidal neurons were found to oscillate at a slower frequency during backward running relative to forward running, supporting the idea that there was attenuated velocity information during backward movement.
In a recent experiment by Cei et al. (2014), rodents were trained to run forward on a treadmill. The treadmill could be moved forward in space, congruent with the rat's head-direction and running-direction, or it could move backward, incongruent with the head direction and running direction of the rat. In this experiment, the posterior parietal velocity information is not in conflict with head-direction, as the rat is always ambulating forward even when the train is moving backward. One interesting difference between the current data and those in Cei et al. (2014) is that in the present study CA1 place cell activity was unidirectional as opposed to bidirectional. As there is a population of neurons in the posterior parietal cortex that show activity correlated with “simple motion states” (McNaughton et al., 1994; Whitlock et al., 2012), one explanation for the disparate findings regarding direction-selectivity could be differences in posterior parietal neuron activity between the treadmill and reverse ambulation conditions. As posterior parietal cortical neurons have altered activity in north- and south-bound routes in a hairpin maze (Whitlock et al., 2012), their directional selectivity may be responsible for unidirectional place fields on linear tracks. When the movement vector is maintained, such as during treadmill running, parietal information is hypothetically maintained, resulting in bidirectional place fields (Cei et al., 2014). Given that the posterior parietal activity precedes that of the onset of movement by up to 500 ms (Gold and Shadlen, 2000; Whitlock et al., 2012), it is also possible that a signal from this region is responsible for driving the observed predictive hippocampal sweeps (Johnson and Redish, 2007), as well as forward and reverse reactivation observed at the ends of a track (Foster and Wilson, 2006; Diba and Buzsàki, 2007). Finally, as the route-specific correlations of posterior parietal neurons are scalable in size (Nitz, 2006) and capable of holding multiple reference frames at a single moment (Nitz, 2012), it is possible that this cortical input contributes to the scaling of hippocampal place fields and grid cells in conditions when a familiar environment is compressed or stretched (O'Keefe and Burgess, 1996; Gothard et al., 2001; Huxter et al., 2003; Barry et al., 2007).
Numerous empirical data and theoretical models indicate that the sequential activation of place cells within a theta cycle is responsible for the phenomenon of theta phase precession (Dragoi and Buzsàki, 2006; Foster and Wilson, 2007; Maurer and McNaughton, 2007; Maurer et al., 2012) and perhaps theta itself (Geisler et al., 2010). Thus, considering the current data in terms of mechanisms for governing temporal sequence dynamics may be more pertinent than determining which theta phase precession model best accounts for the these results. Fortunately, models of hippocampal theta phase precession complement each other when it comes to understanding the phenomenon of ensemble sequence encoding. The intrinsic oscillation of neurons (O'Keefe and Recce, 1993; Kamondi et al., 1998; Bose et al., 2000; Harris et al., 2002; Lengyel et al., 2003) can be synchronized via strong synaptic network connections (Jensen and Lisman, 1996; Tsodyks et al., 1996) and precisely controlled by reciprocal inhibitory-excitatory connections (Wallenstein and Hasselmo, 1997; Mehta et al., 2002). Thus, each component of the prior models can be integrated to support the self-organized dynamic patterns (Kelso, 1995) critical for ensemble sequence encoding.
There is recent experimental evidence to validate the idea that there is dramatic interconnectivity between the models. For example, driving parvalbumin interneurons in a network with an optical chirp stimulus can rhythmically entrain pyramidal neurons; an effect dependent on hyperpolarization-activated cyclic nucleotide-gated (HCN) channel function (Stark et al., 2013). Moreover, the recent observation that neurons maintain their firing rate magnitude across environments and behavioral states supports the idea of a preconfigured network capable of asymmetric neuronal activation (i.e., “trajectories”; Pastalkova et al., 2008; Mizuseki and Buzsàki, 2014) In light of these findings, inhibition and excitation models of phase precession, along with intrinsic oscillation dynamics and asymmetric network connectivity models, can be integrated to propagate a sequence of activity across the network Under this framework, at the beginning of each theta cycle, input into the hippocampus activates a specific cell assembly with the magnitude of activation proportional to the velocity input (Conklin and Eliasmith, 2005; Maurer et al., 2012). This “node of attraction” (Izhikevich, 2007) is transient, limited by mechanisms, such as the distribution of pyramidal cell HCN channels (Giocomo and Hasselmo, 2008; Dougherty et al., 2013), and feedback projections from inhibitory interneurons (Marshall et al., 2002; Maurer et al., 2006a; Geisler et al., 2007). Provided a vector of displacement (i.e., heading and velocity as described by Conklin and Eliasmith, 2005), and the asymmetric firing rate biases of the network (Mizuseki and Buzsàki, 2014), the network activity moves toward he next most stable state. In this view, a chain reaction occurs in which the total number of distinct assemblies activated within a single theta cycle is proportional to the magnitude of the velocity input (Conklin and Eliasmith, 2005; Maurer et al., 2012). Notably, as the signal propagates within the theta cycle, each sequential state is less stable than its predecessor due to a loss of excitation (partially due to feedback inhibition). Moreover, the amount of time that each assembly is active becomes progressively shorter, shifting later in the theta cycle. The propagation of activity therefore effectively accelerates across assemblies while simultaneously losing strength until it collapses. This description effectively accounts for the heteroskedasticity in phase-position portraits (cells are active for significantly longer periods of time early in the theta cycle), and bimodality (the activity of the network accelerates away from the initial state). This also accounts for the current finding that the short-time scale dynamics of the hippocampus are maintained during backward running. Together, the present data show that CA1 ensemble sequence encoding persists even when the sequence is in the direction opposite of the animal's orientation. This may be due to self-organized hippocampal dynamics in conjunction with parietal cortical input.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the McKnight Brain Research Foundation, and NIH Grants AG012609 and NS070464. We thank Kim Bohne, Lan Hoang, Jie Wang, Michael Montgomery, Michelle Carroll, and Luann Snyder for help with completing this paper.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Carol A. Barnes, University of Arizona, Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Institute, Life Sciences North Building, Room 355, Tucson, AZ 85724-5115. carol{at}nsma.arizona.edu