Abstract
Behavioral analyses of the ontogeny of memory have shown that hippocampus-dependent learning emerges relatively late in postnatal development compared with simple associative learning. Maturation of hippocampal mnemonic mechanisms has been hypothesized to underlie the development of the later emerging learning processes. However, the role of hippocampal maturation in learning has not been examined directly. The goal of the present study was to examine developmental changes in hippocampal neuronal coding during acquisition of a hippocampus-dependent learning task. We recorded activity from CA1 pyramidal cells in rat pups while they were trained on trace eyeblink conditioning. Trace eyeblink conditioning is a Pavlovian conditioning task that involves the association of a conditioned stimulus (CS) with an unconditioned stimulus over a stimulus-free trace interval. The inclusion of the trace interval is what makes the task hippocampus dependent. In the present study, rats were trained at 21–23, 24–26, and 31–33 d of age. Previous research from our laboratory and others shows that trace conditioning begins to emerge during the third postnatal week. The results indicate that hippocampal neurons show a substantial increase in responsiveness to task-relevant events during development. Moreover, there is an age-related increase in the proportion of neurons that respond to a combination of trial events (e.g., CS and trace). Our findings indicate that the developmental emergence of hippocampally mediated learning is related to increases in the strength and complexity of CA1 associative coding.
Introduction
Developmental studies of learning and memory indicate that hippocampus-dependent memory develops after hippocampus-independent memory in altricial mammalian species such as humans, monkeys, and rats (Stanton, 2000; Stanton et al., 2009). Learning that is independent of hippocampal function tends to emerge much earlier in development, in some cases even prenatally (Smotherman, 1982). However, due to the relatively late maturation of forebrain structures such as the hippocampus, other types of learning do not begin to emerge until much later in development (Ivkovich and Stanton, 2001). Several approaches have been used to examine the development of learning, including behavioral analyses of hippocampus-dependent tasks such as spatial learning. Spatial learning emerges relatively late in altricial species and parallels some aspects of hippocampal anatomical development (Rudy and Paylor, 1988; Green and Stanton, 1989; Freeman and Stanton, 1991). Rudy and Paylor (1988) found that although postnatal day 22 (P22) rat pups successfully learn the water maze place-learning task, P19 pups are unable to. One possible explanation for the delayed development of place learning could be that it is limited by the development of hippocampal function. Recent work characterizing the development of place fields in the hippocampus has indirectly supported this hypothesis by demonstrating that the development of place fields occurs between the ages of P16 and P28 (Langston et al., 2010; Wills et al., 2010). However, these studies did not directly examine the relationship between hippocampal physiological development and the ontogeny of memory. Moreover, the behavioral development of complex memory processes such as spatial navigation can be difficult to compare across ages due to the concurrent development of sensory processing and motor control.
One possible solution to these limitations is to employ a learning task that is less affected by motor and sensory development such as eyeblink conditioning (Stanton et al., 1992). The primary goal of the present study was to examine the relationship between hippocampal physiological development and the ontogeny of a hippocampus-dependent task, trace eyeblink conditioning. In trace eyeblink conditioning, a conditioned stimulus (CS, e.g., a tone) is followed by an unconditioned stimulus (US) that elicits a reflexive blink. The CS and US are temporally separated by a brief stimulus-free “trace” interval. The inclusion of this trace interval makes the task hippocampus dependent (Solomon et al., 1986; Moyer, Deyo, and Disterhoft, 1990; Clark et al., 2002). Adult hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells respond to the events within the trace conditioning trials including the CS, trace interval, and the US (McEchron and Disterhoft, 1997; Weible et al., 2006; Green and Arenos, 2007). The late emergence of trace conditioning parallels the developmental trajectory of spatial learning and is likewise believed to be due to delayed development of the hippocampus and other forebrain structures (Ivkovich et al., 2000). If the developmental trajectory of this learning task is indeed due to the late development of the hippocampus we would anticipate age- and learning-related changes in the responsiveness of hippocampal neurons.
Materials and Methods
Subjects.
Subjects were 15 Long–Evans rat pups (7 females and 8 males), all from different litters. For paired training (6 sessions), there were 3 pups used in each age group. For unpaired training (also 6 sessions per animal), there were 3 pups in the oldest age group, 2 pups in the middle age group, and 1 pup in the youngest age group. For neuron counts per group, please see Table 1. All pups were born and reared in the Spence Laboratories of Psychology animal colony at the University of Iowa. The colony was maintained on a 12/12 h light/dark cycle, with light onset at 7:00 A.M. All procedures were approved by the University of Iowa Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
Surgery.
Detailed surgical methods have been described previously (Ng and Freeman, 2012). We implanted rat pups with microdrives for neuronal recording with multiple tetrodes on P19, P22, and P29, 2 d before training. Pups were anesthetized using 1.5–3% isoflurane gas. During surgery, rats were fitted with a microdrive for neuronal recording, differential EMG electrodes to record blink activity, and a bipolar stimulating electrode for US delivery.
For microdrive implantation, a small hole was drilled in the skull directly above the right dorsal hippocampus (AP, −4.0 mm; ML, −2.5 mm). The microdrive was lowered until the tetrodes touched the brain surface. Any additional space between the drive and the skull was filled with a low-viscosity silicone gel (Kwik-Sil; World Precision Instruments). The microdrive was then grounded with a stainless steel screw fixed to the skull. Immediately after implantation, the tetrodes were lowered ∼0.6 mm into the brain.
To record differential EMG activity from the eyelid, two stainless steel electrodes were threaded through the left upper orbicularis oculi muscle and a ground wire was attached to the skull with a screw. The bipolar stimulating electrode for US delivery was placed subdermally immediately caudal to the left eye.
On the day after surgery, spike activity was monitored as the recording tetrodes were lowered into the CA1 layer (∼DV, −2.1 mm) and the reference tetrode was lowered into the cortex dorsal to the hippocampus (∼DV, −0.9).
Spike data acquisition.
Spike data acquisition has been described previously in detail (Ng and Freeman, 2012). Pups were surgically implanted with either an 8- or 16-channel microdrive array for either 2 or 4 recording tetrodes, respectively. Each drive had a separate, independently moving reference channel tetrode. Before implantation, each tetrode was gold-plated until the tetrode impedance was ∼350 kΩ.
During data collection and tetrode lowering, the microdrive was connected to a spike acquisition system (Neuralynx). The spike signal was then amplified at a gain of 10,000–25,000 and band-pass filtered between 0.6 and 6.0 kHz. Signals were digitized and stored at 32 kHz (Cheetah; Neuralynx).
Conditioning apparatus.
The conditioning apparatus has been described previously in detail (Ng and Freeman, 2012). Rat pups were trained within a conditioning chamber that was contained within a sound-attenuation chamber. Lightweight cables with connectors for both the recording EMG and the bipolar US electrode were attached to a commutator above the conditioning chamber and threaded through a hole in the ceiling of the chamber. Computer software controlled the delivery of both CS and US while simultaneously recording differential eyelid EMG activity (sampling rate = 250 Hz). EMG activity was amplified (×2000), filtered (500–5000 Hz), and integrated (20 ms time constant).
Conditioning procedures.
To parse out nonassociative responding to the CS from learning, pups were given either paired or unpaired presentations of the CS and US. Pups in the paired group received 100 trials per session of trace EBC with a 250 ms (2 kHz) tone CS, a 500 ms trace interval, and a 25 ms periorbital stimulation US (2–3 mA; see Fig. 1B). In adult animals, this training paradigm results in a learned association between the CS and US. Each paired session was divided into 10 blocks of 10 trials. The first nine trials of the block were paired CS–US presentations and the tenth trial of each block was a probe trial containing only the tone CS. The probe trials were used to evaluate conditioned responding (CR) in the absence of the unconditioned response (Gormezano et al., 1983). Paired trials were separated by a variable intertrial interval that averaged 30 s. Learning was demonstrated by the presence of a CR after CS onset but before US onset on a given trial. The CR threshold was 0.4 V above the amplified and integrated baseline EMG activity. Pups in the unpaired group received 200 trials per session of either the CS or the US. In adult animals, unpaired training does not result in a learned association because the CS and US are presented separately. Unpaired trials were separated by a variable intertrial interval that averaged 15 s. Any response that occurred during the first 80 ms of the CS was considered a startle response and was omitted from future analyses.
Neuronal recording analyses.
Offline neuron separation was initially performed automatically with KlustaKwik (Kadir et al., 2014). Separated neurons were then manually inspected and refined using MClust-3.5 (Redish et al., 2010). Neurons were classified as pyramidal cells if they: (1) showed a bursting pattern of activity as demonstrated by a peak in the autocorrelogram at 3–8 ms, (2) had a baseline (500 ms sample duration before CS onset) firing rate of <10.5 spikes/s, and (3) had at least 300 spikes during the training session.
Neuronal activity was then analyzed in relation to trial event responsivity using NeuroExplorer. First, neurons were classified according to the trial event(s) they responded to (CS, trace interval, US). To accomplish this, the trial was divided into nine 125 ms intervals. The nine time intervals included baseline (125 ms), CS (250 ms), trace (500 ms), and US periods (250 ms). Firing rate for each neuron was normalized to the pre-CS baseline using z-score values in NeuroExplorer. Intervals that had values exceeding the preestablished 99% confidence limits based on the Poisson distribution (two-tailed, α < 0.05) were considered to be statistically significant from the baseline firing rate, thus showing either excitatory or inhibitory responses to trial events. Based on which intervals were different from baseline, neurons were classified as unresponsive, CS-responsive, trace-responsive, US-responsive, or a combination thereof. Therefore, categories were overlapping and combination units could be considered as belonging to more than one category. For example, a unit that showed increased activity during both the CS and the US periods would be categorized as a CS-responsive unit and a US-responsive unit (see Fig. 2). The proportion of neurons that fell into each response category was compared across age groups and sessions with χ2 analyses.
The magnitude of the neuronal response was also examined. Peristimulus-time histograms of neuronal activity were generated with 12.5 ms bins and normalized as follows: normalized bin = (bin mean − 125 ms baseline mean)/SD of 125 ms baseline). The normalized bin values of responsive neurons were then compared across age and CR/no-CR trials with a repeated-measures ANOVA and the Tukey HSD post hoc test to examine age-related differences in the magnitude of neuronal responding during the trial.
Histology.
Histological methods have been described previously in detail (Ng and Freeman, 2012). Tetrode placement was determined by creating small electrolytic lesions after the last session of training. Brains were placed in a 30% sucrose-formalin solution upon removal, sectioned at 50 μm, mounted on slides, and stained with thionin. Histology was then examined with a light microscope to determine tetrode placement. Only placements confirmed to be in the CA1 layer of the hippocampus were included in the analysis.
Results
Behavioral data
Behavioral analyses of learning, as demonstrated by conditioned responding across age and training type, revealed that there were significant differences between groups (Fig. 1A). When pups received paired training, there was an age-related increase in learning, with older pups learning the association better than younger pups. However, there were no significant differences between age groups when pups received unpaired training. These observations were confirmed with a repeated-measures ANOVA examining differences in the percentage of CRs across sessions (sessions 1–6), training condition (paired vs unpaired), and age (P21–P23, P24–P26, or P31–P33), which found a session × training condition × age interaction (F(10,45) = 2.654, p = 0.012; Fig. 1A). This interaction was further examined with Tukey HSD post hoc tests. During paired training, the P31–P33 age group had a significantly greater CR percentage on sessions 1–6 than did the P21–P23 group (p < 0.01). The P31–P33 age group also had a significantly higher CR percentage on sessions 1–4 compared with the P24–P26 age group (p < 0.01). Finally, the two youngest age groups differed from each other on sessions 5–6 (p < 0.01). For unpaired training, there were no significant differences in the percentage of CRs across age.
Neuronal responsiveness
Recorded pyramidal neurons from the CA1 field of the hippocampus were categorized (Fig. 2) according to their firing rates as either responsive or unresponsive to trial events (n = 1812). Responsiveness was further categorized as either excitatory (showing increases in activity) or inhibitory (showing decreases in activity). The proportion of neurons that showed inhibitory responses was extremely low and was therefore excluded from further statistical analyses. The proportion of neurons that showed either excitatory or no response were then compared across categories using χ2 analyses (Table 1). The proportion of excitatory responsive neurons was greater when pups were given paired training than unpaired training [X2(1, N = 1812) = 18.49, p = 0.0001; Fig. 3]. These differences in neuronal responsiveness across paired and unpaired training indicate that the associative nature of the CS and US during paired training leads to an increase in neuronal recruitment.
Within the paired training group, there were no differences across age for the proportion of responsive versus unresponsive neurons [X2(2, N = 1310) = 1.30, p > 0.05]. However, this analysis only examined overall responsiveness and did not take into account responsiveness to individual trial events (e.g., the US). When trial components were broken down into CS, trace, and US components, several age-related changes were found.
Proportion of responsive neurons during paired training
When CS responsivity was examined, an age-related increase in the proportion of neurons that responded to the CS was observed [X2(2, N = 1310) = 14.71, p = 0.0006; Fig. 3A]. However, the proportion of neurons that were responsive to the CS alone, as opposed to those responsive to the CS in combination with other trial events, was too low to compare statistically between age groups. During paired training, there were no significant differences across age in the proportion of neurons that showed responses to the US [X2(2, N = 1310) = 0.71, p = 0.70; this category included combination neurons that were also responsive to the CS and trace interval; Fig. 3B]. However, when examining the proportion of neurons that showed increases in activity uniquely during the US (therefore not US combination neurons), a significant effect of age was found, with the proportion of US-only responsive neurons decreasing with age [X2(2, N = 1310) = 9.61, p = 0.008; Fig. 3C]. Therefore, although all age groups showed similar levels of US responsivity, older pups tended to have neurons that responded to the US in combination with other trial events, whereas younger pups were more likely to have neurons that responded only to the US. In younger animals, the increased proportion of neurons that were responsive to the US alone could be interpreted as a neuronal representation of surprise to the US presentation due to weaker associative prediction (Rescorla and Wagner, 1972). A second interpretation of these results is that the increased activity during the US is learning related and serves as a precursor to the overt learned response (Berger et al., 1976).
There was also an age-related difference in the proportion of neurons that showed responding to a combination of trial events [X2(2, N = 1310) = 6.64, p = 0.036; Fig. 3D]. Younger animals had a lower proportion of cells that responded to a combination of trial events when compared with older animals. Therefore, the older a pup is, the more likely its hippocampal neurons are to respond to a combination of trial events (Fig. 3).
Learning-related changes in the proportion of responsive neurons during paired training
To investigate how learning affected neuronal activity, the proportion of neurons that showed changes in responsiveness between CR and no-CR trials were compared across age and sessions. Only neurons that showed responsiveness to trial events were included in the analysis. Depending on whether the responsiveness category was same or different between CR and no-CR trials, 2 different categorical values (e.g., 1 for same and 0 for different) were assigned to each neuron. These values were compared across sessions as well as across the age group with a Pearson's χ2 test. Note that we excluded sessions in which the CR percentage was <5% or >95% to have minimum numbers of samples in both the CR and no-CR conditions.
The proportion of neurons that responded differentially during CR and no-CR trials did not differ significantly when compared across age groups [X2(2, N = 648) = 1.210, p = 0.546]. Therefore, although all three age groups had a large number of neurons that differed in responsiveness between CR and no-CR trials, there were no significant age-related differences (Fig. 4A). When broken down by session as well as age group, between-group differences in responding were found during session 2 [X2(2, N = 122) = 7.098, p = 0.029]. However, during the other sessions, the proportions were not statistically different across age groups [X2(2, N ≥ 84) < 2.297, p > 0.317; Fig. 4B]. Last, the proportion of units that showed differential responses between CR and no-CR trials was examined across sessions separately for each age group. In the youngest two age groups, the proportion of neurons that showed differential responsiveness was not statistically different across sessions [X2(2, N ≥ 140) < 7.268, p > 0.201]. In the P31–P33 age group, however, the proportion of neurons that responded differentially to CR and no-CR trials significantly decreased across sessions [X2(5, N = 294) = 16.755, p = 0.005].
Proportion of responsive neurons during unpaired training
In contrast to paired training, during unpaired training, the proportion of responsive neurons differed significantly across age, with an age-related decrease in responsivity [X2(2, N = 502) = 22.84, p = 0.00001]. When examining trial events during unpaired training, there was a difference in US responsiveness across age (Fig. 3B). The youngest age group had a greater proportion of responsive neurons compared with the oldest two age groups [X2(2, N = 502) = 21.73, p = 0.000003]. This increased proportion of US-responsive neurons was primarily due to the extremely high proportion of US-responsive neurons during the first three sessions of P21–P23 unpaired training (session 1: 88%, session 2: 58%, session 3: 64%). Indeed, there was a significant decrease in the proportion of US-responsive neurons as unpaired training continued [X2(5, N = 169) = 39.21, p = 0.0000002]. By the end of training, the P21–P23 group neurons showed a similar level of US responsiveness to that seen in the older groups.
Changes in neuronal responsiveness across sessions in all age groups
Differences in neuronal responsiveness across sessions were found. There was a significant decrease in the proportion of responsive neurons across sessions during both paired [X2(5, N = 1310) = 26.82, p = 0.00006] and unpaired [X2(5, N = 502) = 24.81, p = 0.0002 training; Fig. 5]. When neuronal responsiveness during unpaired sessions was further examined, χ2 analyses showed that the significant decrease in responding across sessions was most likely due to the US-responsive neurons. The proportion of US-responsive [X2(5, N = 502) = 21.57, p = 0.0006], but not CS-responsive [X2(5, N = 502) = 10.35, p = 0.066] neurons significantly decreased across training sessions. When neuronal responsiveness across sessions was examined during paired training, χ2 analyses revealed that the proportion of both CS-responsive [X2(5, N = 1310) = 24.25, p = 0.0002] and US-responsive [X2(5, N = 1310) = 24.95, p = 0.0001] neurons decreased significantly as training continued. The proportion of trace-responsive neurons, however, did not change across sessions [X2(5, N = 1310) = 10.20, p = 0.07].
Magnitude of neuronal responses to trial events
Magnitude of neuronal response during paired training
To determine how the magnitude of the neuronal response changed across age, learning, and stimulus type, activity during trials was normalized to the pre-CS baseline (see “Materials and Methods” section). Normalized neuronal activity of responsive neurons (Figs. 6, 7, 8) was then examined with repeated-measures ANOVA to determine which time bins were significantly affected by age and whether the animal showed a CR. Therefore, the magnitude of the response was compared across age groups and learning. Overall, there was a significant age × CR × time bin interaction (F(41.87, 19196.93) = 1.486, p = 0.022). Tukey post hoc tests controlling for the number of comparisons and the Tukey–Kramer approach to unequal “n” were run on individual bins. These post hoc tests indicated that the oldest two age groups showed greater neuronal activity during the CS and trace periods on CR trials relative to no-CR trials (Fig. 8). Specifically, in the P24–P26 and P31–P33 age groups, there were 30 and 45 (respectively) of 60 total bins that were significantly lower in the no-CR trials compared with the CR trials during the CS and trace periods. In contrast, when examining this averaged activity, all age groups had a decrease in the magnitude of responding to the US during CR trials when compared with no-CR trials, with this effect being most evident in the youngest age group with 13 of 20 bins showing significant decreases (Fig. 8). Learning was therefore associated with an age-related increase in the magnitude of the neuronal response during the CS and trace interval, but a decrease after the US. In addition, there were age-related differences in the magnitude of neuronal responding across groups during CR trials. Specifically, four of 20 bins were significantly lower in the youngest age group than the two older age groups during the CS (p < 0.05). During the trace interval, six of 40 bins were significantly lower in the youngest age group compared with the older two age groups and, during the US period, 11 of 20 bins were significantly lower in the youngest age group than the older two age groups (p < 0.05).
Magnitude of neuronal response during unpaired training
The magnitude of the neuronal response was also evaluated during unpaired training (see Fig. 6 for an example of single-unit neuronal activity). Normalized neuronal activity of responsive neurons during either CS- or US-alone unpaired trials was compared across age groups. When the magnitude of responding for unpaired CS-responsive neurons was examined, there was no effect of age on the magnitude of the neuronal response. This is in stark contrast to the robust age-related changes in response magnitude to the CS observed during paired training, a finding that suggests that the presence of the associative context is sufficient to produce increased activity during the CS presentation.
When the magnitude of responding for unpaired US responsive neurons was examined, there was a significant effect of age on the magnitude of the neuronal response (F(178, 18245) = 2.175, p = 0.0000001). Post hoc tests showed that differences in the magnitude of responding were observed only in the first 150 ms after the US onset (p < 0.05). Specifically, four of 20 bins were significantly higher in the P21–P23 than the P24–P26 group, three of 20 bins were significantly higher in the P21–P23 than the P31–P34 group, and two of 20 bins were higher in the P31–P34 group than the P24–P26 group. However, unlike the differences observed in the magnitude of responding during paired training, no clear overall developmental changes were evident in these differences.
Changes in the magnitude of neuronal response across session
When the magnitude of responding for different age groups was compared across sessions, a repeated-measures ANOVA on responsive cells found an age × session × time bin interaction (F(219.927, 14735.123) = 1.664, p = 0.0000001; Fig. 9). Tukey post hoc tests (P < 0.05) indicated that the majority of age-related changes in activity to the CS and trace interval occurred during the first two sessions of training (Fig. 9). Although there were significant group differences in the magnitude of the neuronal response for individual bins during the US period, a clear developmental trend was not apparent. Table 2 depicts the number of significant bins between age groups during the CS, trace, and US intervals. During both sessions 1 and 2, the oldest age group had a significantly increased magnitude of responding during the CS period compared with the younger two age groups. Although there were a few bins that showed increases in magnitude during the trace interval compared with the younger two age groups, this effect was far more noticeable during session 2.
Discussion
As seen in previous studies (Ivkovich et al., 2000), rat pups trained in trace eyeblink conditioning did not show robust levels of conditioned responding until nearly the fourth postnatal week. This developmental trajectory is very similar to that seen in studies of spatial learning (Rudy and Paylor, 1988; Green and Stanton, 1989; Freeman and Stanton, 1991). However, just as observed in place cell development (Langston et al., 2010; Wills et al., 2010), the development of neuronal activity preceded the development of the behavior. Even at the youngest age tested here (P21–P23), hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells showed changes related to associative learning. There was significantly greater responsivity to trial events when presented in the paired context than when presented in the unpaired context. Therefore, even though the youngest rats were not showing overt behavioral signs of learning, they did, at the neuronal level, recognize the difference between associative and nonassociative contexts. However, the pattern of neuronal responsivity in younger rats differed substantially from more mature rats. This difference in CA1 responsiveness may be one of the underlying factors contributing to the developmental change in trace eyeblink conditioning. During paired sessions, neurons of younger rats were more likely to respond to a single trial event, such as the US, whereas neurons of older rats were more likely to respond to a combination of trial events. The CA1 pyramidal cells of younger pups also showed a lower magnitude of responding during conditioning trials. This pattern of responsiveness suggests that the hippocampus of older rats is sufficiently developed to bind the US with other trial events, thus providing the basis for richer associative coding.
CA1 pyramidal cells also showed changes in responsivity across training sessions. Just as seen in adult animals, there was a decrease in responding to both the CS and the US as sessions progressed (McEchron and Disterhoft, 1997). Moreover, these changes were most evident in the oldest age group. These results support the current hypothesis that hippocampal involvement may be most critical early in training (Takehara et al., 2003).
All age groups showed differences in the magnitude of CA1 pyramidal cell responding to the US between learning and nonlearning trials. However, in older animals, learning was associated with an increase in neuronal responsiveness to the CS and trace intervals. This increased responding could be a neuronal representation of the pups attending to CS and trace intervals as salient predictors of the US. Conversely, the increased magnitude of neuronal responding to the US during no-CR trials could indicate that the pup is “surprised” by the arrival of the US (i.e., a prediction error; Rescorla and Wagner, 1972).
A number of developmental changes in both neurogenesis rates and cellular function occur during these ages that might explain the developmental changes in CA1 activity. There is an age-related increase in the strength of CA3–CA1 synapses due to an increase in the probability of transmitter release (Dumas and Foster, 1995), which in turn could be a result of age-related changes in adenylyl cyclase expression (Matsuoka et al., 1997). Moreover, there is an overall increase in synaptic connectivity across development as indicated by an age-related increase in synaptic number and density (Harris et al., 1992; Hsia et al., 1998). There is also some evidence that there is a postsynaptic decrease in LTP induction threshold, thus contributing to greater levels of depolarization (Dumas, 2012). These and other changes in synaptic connectivity result in overall changes in synaptic plasticity. In addition to synaptic plasticity mechanisms, the rate of neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus could play a role in the development of hippocampal forms of learning (Akers et al., 2014). High rates of neurogenesis, as seen early in the postnatal development of altricial species such as rats, are related to weaker memory and thus blocking dentate gyrus neurogenesis in young animals results in better retention (Akers et al., 2014). Developmental changes in dentate gyrus function related to changes in neurogenesis might have downstream effects on CA1 function that influence CA1 responses during associative learning. Therefore, developmental changes in synaptic function and neurogenesis rates in the dentate gyrus may be reflected in the development of learning-related neuronal activity in CA1 pyramidal cells described here.
Developmental changes in hippocampal associative coding identified in the present study provide a more mechanistic understanding of the development of learning and memory relative to previous studies that relied on inferred mechanisms from developmental changes in learning or deficits produced by lesions. Previous studies of hippocampal function found that some of the quantitative features of place fields developed as a nearly linear function of postnatal age (Langston et al., 2010; Wills et al., 2010). Our findings suggest, however, that developmental changes in hippocampal associative coding are more abrupt developmentally, with a substantial transition in between P21 and P24. The developmental increases in the strength and complexity of hippocampal coding may underlie the development of other hippocampus-dependent processes such as episodic memory, which requires integration of object, spatial, and temporal information.
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health (Grant NS038890 to J.H.F.). We thank Magdalyn Elkin and Ka Ng for technical assistance.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to John H. Freeman, Department of Psychology, E11 Seashore Hall, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242. john-freeman{at}uiowa.edu