Abstract
Distinct brain regions are highly interconnected via long-range projections. How this inter-regional communication occurs depends not only upon which subsets of postsynaptic neurons receive input, but also, and equally importantly, upon what cellular subcompartments the projections target. Neocortical pyramidal neurons receive input onto their apical dendrites. However, physiological characterization of these inputs thus far has been exclusively somatocentric, leaving how the dendrites respond to spatial and temporal patterns of input unexplored. Here we used a combination of optogenetics with multisite electrode recordings to simultaneously measure dendritic and somatic responses to afferent fiber activation in two different populations of layer 5 (L5) pyramidal neurons in the rat medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC). We found that commissural inputs evoked monosynaptic responses in both intratelencephalic (IT) and pyramidal tract (PT) dendrites, whereas monosynaptic hippocampal input primarily targeted IT, but not PT, dendrites. To understand the role of dendritic integration in the processing of long-range inputs, we used dynamic clamp to simulate synaptic currents in the dendrites. IT dendrites functioned as temporal integrators that were particularly responsive to dendritic inputs within the gamma frequency range (40–140 Hz). In contrast, PT dendrites acted as coincidence detectors by responding to spatially distributed signals within a narrow time window. Thus, the PFC extracts information from different brain regions through the combination of selective dendritic targeting and the distinct dendritic physiological properties of L5 pyramidal dendrites.
Introduction
To guide complex goal-directed behavior, the prefrontal cortex (PFC) integrates information from numerous cortical and subcortical regions. Two important afferent inputs that the PFC integrates derive from the hippocampus and the contralateral PFC. Damage to either is implicated in mental disorders (Godsil et al., 2013; Hoppenbrouwers et al., 2014; Wheeler et al., 2014), and their disruption is associated with deficits in performance on working memory-like tasks (Miu et al., 2006; Tseng et al., 2008; Dickerson et al., 2010; Sigurdsson et al., 2010). The timing of hippocampal inputs to the PFC is particularly important, as hippocampal–PFC synchrony elevates at the “decision point” in several learned tasks (Benchenane et al., 2010; Sigurdsson et al., 2010; Adhikari et al., 2011; Fujisawa and Buzsáki, 2011). How long-range afferent inputs are able to influence PFC activity depends greatly on what neuron subpopulations they target.
Commissural and hippocampal afferent fibers are distributed across cellular layers, and thus may target several pyramidal and interneuron populations (Jay et al., 1989; Hoover and Vertes, 2007). Supragranular (layers 1–3) inputs target the apical dendrites and perisomatic regions of layer 2 pyramidal neurons (Little and Carter, 2012). Whether these inputs also target layer 5 (L5) pyramidal neurons' apical dendrites is not known. A further complication is that L5 pyramidal neurons can be separated into two categories: (1) pyramidal tract (PT) neurons, which project subcortically via the pyramidal tracts, and (2) intratelencaphalic (IT) neurons, which project across the cerebral commissure. PT and IT neurons make and receive different local connections within the PFC and are distinct electrophysiologically and anatomically (Morishima and Kawaguchi, 2006; Otsuka and Kawaguchi, 2008; Dembrow et al., 2010). While PT and IT neurons have both been shown to respond to activation of commissural fibers en masse, whether these inputs are monosynaptic and whether they target the dendrites remains uncertain (Lee et al., 2014). In both the PFC and other cortical regions, supragranular input to L5 pyramidal neurons has only been tested using somatic recordings, where it elicits little or no response (Petreanu et al., 2007; Anderson et al., 2010; Oviedo et al., 2010; Mao et al., 2011; Sheets et al., 2011; Lee et al., 2014). It remains unclear whether this is because long-range supragranular terminals do not innervate L5 pyramidal neurons or because filtering obscures dendritic responses.
Using simultaneous dendritic and somatic recordings, we have found that L5 neurons in medial PFC (mPFC) received substantial dendritic input from long-range afferent fibers. Whether these inputs directly targeted the dendritic compartment depended upon the afferent fiber type and the postsynaptic neuron identity. Furthermore, how PT and IT neurons processed simulated synaptic input made them sensitive to different spatial and temporal patterns of input. IT neurons summated synaptic input across a broader temporal window but with significant temporal distortion. Meanwhile, PT dendrites were highly responsive to spatially distributed signals within a narrow temporal window, and thus could act as coincidence detectors. Our data suggest that how inputs communicated to the mPFC microcircuit would depend upon the filtering properties of the dendritic subcompartments they targeted.
Materials and Methods
Surgeries.
All procedures involving animals were performed with the approval of the University of Texas Animal Care and Use Committee. Male Sprague Dawley rats 9–15 weeks old were anesthetized with isoflurane (1–4% mixed in oxygen), placed in a stereotaxic apparatus, and prepped for injections with craniotomies over the target injection regions. Injections were performed using a pulled glass pipette (10–15 μm diameter tip) mounted on a Nanoject II small-volume injector (Drummond Scientific). Each individual injection was performed at a speed of 23 nL/s, separated by a 2–3 min interval. Recombinant adeno-associated virus (rAAV)-expressing channel-rhodopsin2-sfGFP (ChR2) from a synapsin promoter was injected into either the contralateral PFC (2.5–3.5 mm anterior, 0.7–0.8 mm lateral, and 3.5–4.4 mm ventral to bregma; six injections; 13.8 nL each) or the ipsilateral ventral hippocampus (6.3 mm anterior, 5.7 mm lateral, 5.8–6.8 mm ventral to bregma; eight 13.8 nL injections separated ventrally by 150 μm). Red fluorescent-labeled microspheres (Lumafluor) were injected into either the pons (right; 7.4 mm posterior, 1.1 mm lateral, 9.8 mm ventral to bregma; 2–3 injections in same location; 50.6 nL each) or mPFC (left; 2.5–3.5 mm anterior, 0.8 mm lateral, and 4–4.4 mm ventral to bregma; two injections; 50.6 nL each) to retrogradely label corticopontine PT and commissural IT neurons, respectively. For all injections, the pipette was left in place for 3–5 min before removing it from the brain. Rats were given analgesics (carprofen; 5 mg/kg) postsurgery, and monitored daily to ensure complete recovery. For virally injected animals, viral expression was allowed for 7–12 weeks after surgery before collecting tissue.
Slice preparation.
Male Sprague Dawley rats 10–20 weeks old were anesthetized using a ketamine (100 mg/kg)/xylazine (10 mg/kg) mixture delivered intraperitoneally. Rats were perfused through the heart with ice-cold saline containing the following (in mm): 2.5 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 25 NaHCO3, 0.5 CaCl2, 7 MgCl2, 7 dextrose, 205 sucrose, 1.3 ascorbate, and 3 sodium pyruvate (bubbled with 95% O2/5% CO2 to maintain pH at 7.4). Brains were removed, and a blocking cut was made 11° off coronal to maintain the plane of the dendrites within the slice. Using a vibrating tissue slicer (Vibratome 3000, Vibratome), 300-μm-thick coronal sections were collected from 2.5 mm and up to the fusion of the genii of the corpus callosum. Slices were cut in ice-cold saline that was identical to the saline used during the perfusion, subsequently stored in holding solution containing the following (in mm): 125 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 25 NaHCO3, 2 CaCl2, 2 MgCl2, 12.5 dextrose, and 3 sodium pyruvate (bubbled with 95% O2/5% CO2) at 35°C for 30 min, and then at room temperature. In cases where tissue contained ChR2-EYFP-expressing neurons, great care was taken to minimize light exposure during slicing, tissue storage, and recording.
Recording and light stimulation.
For recording, slices were continuously perfused (1–2 mL/min) with ACSF containing the following (in mm): 125 NaCl, 3.0 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 25 NaHCO3, 2 CaCl2, 1 MgCl2, and 12.5 dextrose bubbled continuously with 95% O2/5% CO2 at 32–35°C. Neurons were visualized using either a Zeiss Axioskop with infrared video microscopy and differential interference contrast optics or a resonant scanning Leica two-photon system (with Dodt contrast). Somatic patch pipettes (4–6 MΩ) were filled with an internal solution containing the following (in mm): 120 K-gluconate, 16 KCl, 10 HEPES, 8 NaCl, 4 MgATP, 0.3 NaGTP, and 7 K2-phosphocreatine, pH 7.3 with KOH, with neurobiotin (0.1–0.2%) and Alexa-594 (20–40 μm). Bead-labeled neurons or Alexa-594-filled neurons were visualized using either two-photon excitation at 840 nm or light from a mercury lamp (X-Cite, Lumen Dynamics) passed through a 540/605 nm excitation/emission filter set. Simultaneous soma and dendrite recordings were made by first patching the soma of an L5 neuron with a pipette containing Alexa-594 (40 μm). This permitted the dendrite to be visualized and subsequently patched with a dendritic patch pipette (6–10 MΩ) using simultaneous Dodt contrast and fluorescence microscopy. Dendritic pipettes were wrapped with Parafilm to minimize capacitance. For a subset of the dual recordings, we categorized IT versus PT neurons from one another using the neurons' somatic and dendritic electrophysiological properties. As established by our previous reports (Dembrow et al., 2010; Kalmbach et al., 2013), PT neurons display somatic resonance of >2.2 Hz and dendritic resonance of >5 Hz.
Electrophysiological data were acquired using either a Dagan BVC-700 (Dagan) amplifier with custom data-acquisition software written using Igor Pro (Wavemetrics) or a Multiclamp 700B (Molecular Devices) amplifier with AxoGraph X (AxoGraph Scientific) data-acquisition software. Data were acquired at 10–50 kHz, filtered at 5–10 kHz, and digitized by an ITC-18 (InstruTech) interface. Pipette capacitance was compensated, and the bridge was balanced with each recording. Series resistance was monitored throughout each experiment and was 10–25 MΩ for somatic recordings and 15–45 MΩ for dendritic recordings. Voltages are not corrected for the liquid-junction potential (estimated as ∼12 mV based on relative ionic mobilities and charge).
ChR2 inputs were triggered by field illumination (Thorlabs LED; 1 ms, 0.2–3.5 mW at focal plane) focused through a 20× (740 μm diameter) or a 60× (225 μm diameter) objective. For a subset of experiments where smaller field of illumination was needed, an internal iris prior to the 20× objective reduced the spot size to a circle 210 μm in diameter or a rectangle measuring 260 × 390 μm. Each trial of light illumination was separated by 1 min, to minimize any potential plasticity or release run-down. For all tests of monosynaptic responses, the minimal light intensity stimulation was used that was sufficient to drive three concurrent responses. Using this same level of illumination, 1.25 μm d-2-amino-5-phonovalerate (d-APV) and 1 μm DNQX were bath applied for 15 min while responses every minute were monitored. To examine terminal release, 1 μm TTX and 100 μm 4-AP were bath applied. In some cases, increasing the light stimulation (2–5 ms light exposure) was necessary to observe responses. Within each neuron, light intensity was kept consistent for all subcompartments illuminated.
Dynamic clamp.
Synaptic currents were simulated using dynamic clamp as described by Vaidya and Johnston (2013). Briefly, the dynamic-clamp system was implemented by StdpC software on a standard PC (Intel Core 2 Duo E8400; 3 GHz with 2 GB RAM) and Windows 7 (64 bit) interfaced using a National Instruments acquisition card (PCIe-6251) via a connector block (BNC-2210) and a unity gain amplifier. Experiments were performed at an update speed of 10 kHz. Synaptic events were signaled by a 0.1 ms transistor–transistor logic pulse from Axograph during current or voltage-clamp acquisition. This pulse drove the “presynaptic” simulated neuron in StdpC (with a voltage threshold of 50 mV and voltage slope of 25 mV) to produce a synaptic conductance with a near-instantaneous rise to its maximum value (set at 1–10 nS), followed by a single exponential decay (set to 2 ms). The reversal potential of the synaptic conductance was set at 0 mV. Conductance and decay times were chosen as they best fit light-evoked responses observed in the dendrites.
Analysis and statistics.
To analyze the relative power of frequency components within current and voltage waveforms, we used the continuous wavelet transform (CWT) as described by Vaidya and Johnston (2013). CWT was performed using a complex Morlet wavelet implemented in Igor Pro (Wavemetrics) following guidelines from Torrence and Compo (1998). The transform coefficients for power were corrected for their energy bias to allow comparison across frequencies. We report the average of the frequency spectrum across the time series as a “global average” to permit comparisons across cell types. For both neuron types, the global average satisfactorily represented the spectral components of simulated synaptic events across the time series. Paired t tests, ANOVA with repeated measures, or mixed-factor ANOVA, ANCOVA, and post hoc t tests with Bonferonni's correction were used to test for statistical differences between conditions. Error bars represent SEM. Analyses and graphing were performed with a combination of Axograph, Excel, and Igor. Statistical tests were performed in R and/or Prism (GraphPad).
Drugs.
Drugs were prepared from concentrated stock solutions in water [gabazine, DNQX, d-APV, 4-AP, TTX, 4-ethylphenyl-amino-1,2-dimethyl-6-methylaminopyrimidinium chloride (ZD7288)] or DMSO (CGP55845; final concentration of DMSO, <0.1%) as appropriate stored at −20°C. All drugs were obtained from Abcam.
Results
Commissural and hippocampal inputs to prefrontal neurons
To examine how hippocampal and commissural afferents target the mPFC in the rat, we delivered rAAV expressing ChR2-sfGFP to either the contralateral PFC (Fig. 1a) or the ipsilateral ventral hippocampus (Fig. 1b). By 3 weeks, fluorescent commissural axons and terminals were visible throughout the cortical layers, but were most dense within L1B and L5B (Fig. 1c,e). Hippocampal innervation was sparser than commissural input (Fig. 1d,f). Hippocampal fibers were densest in the more ventral regions of mPFC (prelimbic and infralimbic cortex) and did not extend to anterior cingulate or agranular motor cortex. In prelimbic cortex, hippocampal fibers formed two broader bands of labeling, one supragranular and one infragranular (Fig. 1d). The dendritic arbors of L5 pyramidal neurons (filled with 20–40 μm Alexa-594) extended throughout the same regions that both commissural (Fig. 1e) and hippocampal (Fig. 1f) fibers innervated. Thus, in addition to making contacts proximal to the soma, hippocampal and commissural axons may potentially synapse on L5 pyramidal neurons throughout the cortical layers. Previous reports both in anesthetized animals in vivo and in slices ex vivo demonstrated that stimulating hippocampal and commissural inputs can elicit synaptic responses in L5 neurons (Cowan and Wilson, 1994; Dégenètais et al., 2003; Parent et al., 2010; Lee et al., 2014). To confirm that L5 mPFC neurons elicited responses at the soma, we briefly illuminated the whole field (1 ms, 0.2–2.5 mW). Commissural inputs elicited responses in all PT neurons (13 of 13) and most IT neurons (13 of 19). Hippocampal inputs selectively targeted IT neurons: fiber activation elicited responses in fewer PT neurons (12 of 21) than IT neurons (23 of 31).
While previous reports have shown that fiber activation can elicit responses in L5 neurons, it remained unclear whether these inputs were monosynaptic. Hippocampal and commissural inputs both directly synapse onto L2 neurons, which are capable of providing feedforward excitation to L5 neurons (Little and Carter, 2012; Otsuka and Kawaguchi, 2008). Thus, these long-range afferent inputs might be indirect: processed first by the supragranular pyramidal neurons before reaching the primary output layer (L5) of the mPFC. To test whether observed responses were monosynaptic, we minimized the recruitment of local neurons with low concentrations of AMPA and NMDA receptor blockers (2.5 μm d-AP5, 2 μm DNQX). Responses that were eliminated in an all-or-none fashion were presumed to be polysynaptic, while responses that were reduced in amplitude but that nevertheless persisted were presumed to be monosynaptic (hippocampal to IT: 66.4 ± 14.9%, n = 13; hippocampal to PT: 14.7 ± 6.9%, n = 3; commissural to IT: 29.9 ± 4.8%, n = 7; commissural to PT: 52.9 ± 3.5%, n = 6).
Commissural input was monosynaptic in most IT (seven of nine) and PT (six of seven) neurons. Similarly, hippocampal input was monosynaptic in most IT neurons (13 of 15). In contrast, hippocampal input was monosynaptic in less than half of the PT neurons tested (five of 12). Thus, while hippocampal input can monosynaptically connect to PT neurons, this connection was less prevalent. Furthermore, commissural inputs elicited responses that were not significantly different between PT and IT neurons in amplitude, initial slope, and rise times (Table 1; Fig. 2c, right); however, hippocampal input to PT neurons was smaller in amplitude, initial slope, and rise time (PT, n = 5; IT, n = 14; t test p < 0.05; Table 1; Fig. 2d, right). Simultaneous recordings of PT and IT neurons within 70 μm of each other confirmed that these differences were consistent across different slices (Fig. 2e). In six of six paired recordings, PT responses from hippocampal inputs were either absent, polysynaptic, or smaller than the observed response in the adjacent IT neuron. These data suggest that, measured at the soma, hippocampal inputs directly target IT neurons. Combined, these data suggested that hippocampal and commissural inputs entered the mPFC microcircuit very differently. Commissural inputs elicited robust direct responses in both IT and PT neurons, whereas hippocampal inputs elicited much stronger direct responses in IT than in PT neurons.
In addition to amplitude, we characterized the time course of elicited EPSPs by measuring their half-widths. For both afferent input types, half-widths were greater in IT neurons (Table 1; commissural PT, n = 7; IT, n = 7, t test p < 0.05; hippocampal PT, n = 5; IT, n = 14, t test p < 0.05). Disparate somatic intrinsic properties of PT and IT neurons, due in part to hyperpolarization-activated cation current, Ih (Dembrow et al., 2010), might partially account for the difference in EPSP half-width. To test this, we used dynamic clamp to inject an excitatory conductance at the soma in PT and IT neurons. Indeed, half-width evoked from simulated EPSCs (simEPSCs) were broader in IT neurons, but this did not fully account for the difference (PT: 13.60 ± 0.75 ms, n = 15; IT: 17.11 ± 1.46 ms, n = 12; t test p < 0.05). Because half-width is also particularly sensitive to dendritic filtering (Williams and Stuart, 2000; Berger et al., 2001), we performed dual dendritic–somatic recordings to specifically examine dendritic input.
Dendritic integration of synaptic inputs to prefrontal neurons
Voltage measurements of long-range inputs to pyramidal neurons have thus far been exclusively somatic (Petreanu et al., 2007; Anderson et al., 2010; Oviedo et al., 2010; Mao et al., 2011; Sheets et al., 2011; Lee et al., 2014). These measurements only represent a portion of the synaptic input, however, as filtering distorts the amplitude and time course of dendritic signals once they arrive at the soma (Magee and Cook, 2000; Williams and Stuart, 2002; Golding et al., 2005). Even under voltage-clamp conditions designed to minimize the effects of dendritic filtering, distortion of distal inputs is severe in L5 pyramidal neurons (Williams and Mitchell, 2008). To examine the dendritic input during long-range afferent fiber stimulation, we performed whole-field illumination while simultaneously recording from two zones of integration in these neurons: (1) the nexus at which the tuft of the apical dendritic arbor converges and (2) the soma.
Regardless of presynaptic input (commissural vs hippocampal) or postsynaptic target (PT vs IT), dendritic recordings revealed substantial dendritic responses that were not observable at the somatic recording electrode (Figs. 3⇓–5). Dendritic responses <1 mV were barely detectable within noise at the somatic recording location (∼200 μV). Whether the somatic electrode was in voltage-clamp or current-clamp mode, the somatic measurement is greatly attenuated (Fig. 3a). Indeed, the voltage escape at the dendritic electrode was similar whichever mode the somatic electrode was in. To examine how well current was measured under whole-cell voltage-clamp conditions, we used dynamic clamp to inject simulated synaptic currents at the dendrite (Fig. 3b–d). Injected dendritic currents with a peak <100 pA were not detectable at the soma. The inability of the somatic electrode to sufficiently clamp dendritic events was emphasized by the significant voltage escape in the dendrites (Fig. 3d). Thus, even under whole-cell voltage-clamp conditions, the intrinsic properties of dendritic compartments substantially distorted recorded events at the soma.
Responses to commissural input were larger at the dendrite in all PT (15 of 15) and most IT (six of seven) neurons (Fig. 4a,c). The dendritic EPSP amplitude was 4.08 ± 0.60 times greater in PT neurons (paired t test, p < 0.05, n = 15) and 2.12 ± 0.35 times greater in IT neurons (paired t test, p < 0.05, n = 7). Similarly, responses to hippocampal input were larger in the dendrites in most PT (eight of 12) and IT (four of five) neurons (Fig. 4b,d). Dendritic inputs were 2.51 ± 0.61 times greater in PT neurons (paired t test, p < 0.05, n = 12) and 3.47 ± 0.54 times greater in IT neurons (paired t test, p < 0.05, n = 5). For both input types and postsynaptic target, the dendritic EPSP half-widths were smaller than somatic EPSPs (Table 1; Fig. 4f,g).
Clearly, dendritic responses were larger (and briefer) than somatic ones in both neuron types. How did PT and IT dendritic responses compare to each other? This depended on input type. Commissural inputs elicited significantly larger EPSPs in PT dendrites (Table 1; PT, n = 15; IT, n = 7; t test, p < 0.05). Hippocampal inputs elicited dendritic EPSPs that were not significantly different between the neuron types (Table 1; PT, n = 11; IT, n = 5; t test, p = 0.11). Regardless of the input, half-widths of dendritic EPSPs were significantly less in PT than in IT neurons (Fig. 4f,g; F(34,1) = 31.138, p < 0.01). These data were consistent with faster functional membrane time constants in the dendrites of both PT and IT neurons (Kalmbach et al., 2013).
To test whether responses we measured at the nexus of the dendritic tuft could trigger a dendritic action potential, we depolarized the dendritic recording location by 5–15 mV while stimulating commissural fibers. In PT neurons, this reliably triggered a dendritic action potential, followed by a somatic action potential (four of five neurons; Fig. 4e). For IT neurons, commissural input could trigger a dendritic action potential (one of four neurons), but this depolarization was either insufficient to trigger an action potential (two of four neurons) or a somatic action potential preceded the dendritic action potential (one of four neurons). These results suggest that for PT neurons, a prominent integration zone for action potential initiation exists in the distal dendrites, which may be capable of recruiting axonal output.
Wide-field activation of hippocampal and commissural fibers elicited substantial dendritic responses in both PT and IT neurons. These dendritic responses were complicated by two factors. First, polysynaptic activity was intact and, as such, feedforward excitation could target the dendrites of PT and IT neurons. Second, stimulating a wide field might activate axons that synapse onto subcellular compartments outside the field of activation. To compare the monosynaptic somatic versus dendritic input required more selective stimulation. To do this, we blocked axonal propagation pharmacologically (1 μm TTX, 100 μm 4-AP) and reduced the field of illumination to allow selective activation of terminals over the apical dendritic tuft, the perisomatic region (including the basal dendrites), and the portion of apical dendrite between them (Fig. 5). Responses persisted with axonal propagation blocked (commissural: three of four IT and four of four PT neurons; hippocampal: five of seven IT and five of 10 PT neurons); further confirming that commissural and hippocampal fibers provide monosynaptic connections to both neuron types.
Activating commissural terminals over the tuft elicited responses in both PT and IT neurons at the dendritic recording location (Table 1; Fig. 5a,b). These responses were attenuated in amplitude at the soma (PT: 14.32 ± 0.39%; IT: 15.32 ± 0.31% of the original signal). Conversely, commissural terminals over the soma elicited somatic responses (Table 1), which were attenuated in amplitude at the dendritic electrode (Fig. 5a,b; PT: 13.64 ± 0.82%; IT: 59.80 ± 15.24% of the original signal). Local dendritic responses were larger than local somatic responses in both neuron types, but this was only significant in IT neurons (IT: ANOVA with repeated measures, F(2,4) = 25.49, p < 0.01, post hoc comparisons, p < 0.05; PT: F(2,6) = 12.63, p < 0.01, post hoc comparison, p > 0.1). This was in contrast to our dual recordings without axonal propagation intact (Fig. 4), suggesting that significant feedforward excitation targets the dendrites of PT neurons. For both neuron types, illuminating between the two electrodes elicited no response at either the somatic or dendritic recording location (Fig. 5a,b). As this response was smaller than the propagated responses from more distal locations, our data suggested that commissural inputs to the middle region were relatively weak. Thus commissural inputs converge onto both dendritic and perisomatic compartments, and in the case of IT neurons the dendritic input was significantly larger.
Hippocampal inputs targeted subcompartments differently than commissural inputs, depending upon the neuron type. Activating hippocampal terminals over the tuft elicited a dendritic EPSP in IT neurons (Table 1) that was attenuated at the somatic electrode (Fig. 5c; 36 ± 9% of the original signal). Only one of five PT neurons responded to activation of hippocampal terminals at the dendrite while the remaining four of five did not (Table 1). Activating perisomatic hippocampal terminals elicited a somatic EPSP in both PT and IT neurons (Table 1) that was attenuated at the dendritic electrode (PT: 64.8 ± 8%; IT: 36 ± 9% of the original signal). As was the case for commissural fibers, local responses to hippocampal input were significantly larger in IT dendrites than the IT soma (ANOVA with repeated measures, F(2,6) = 5.122, p < 0.05, post hoc comparisons, p < 0.05). Similar to commissural inputs, no responses were detected from illuminating between the recording locations for either neuron type. Combined, these data suggest that PT and IT neurons integrate long-range afferent input at two electrically distant locations: the nexus of the apical dendrite and the soma.
Frequency dependence of dendritic filtering in PT and IT neurons
For both PT and IT neurons, long-range afferent fibers elicited distinct synaptic input at the dendrite and soma. These locations were electrotonically distant and therefore dendritic filtering could play an important role in how they interact. To test how time-varying signals were filtered in amplitude depending upon their frequency, we calculated impedance amplitude profiles (ZAPs) by injecting sinusoidal current over a range of frequencies. PT and IT neurons exhibited different filter characteristics, depending on the signal's temporal properties. IT neurons function as low-pass filters, attenuating fast signals such that slow signals (<2 Hz) were preferentially amplified. PT neurons function as bandpass filters, attenuating both slow (<2 Hz) and fast (>7 Hz) signals, such that a frequency band (3–6 Hz) was amplified. As we have shown previously, differences in the dendritic ZAP were substantial between PT versus IT dendrites (Fig. 6b,c; Kalmbach et al., 2013). To test how time-varying signals were transferred from dendrite to the soma, we calculated the transferred ZAP from the somatic voltage response to dendritic current injection (Fig. 6b–d). The peak transfer frequency from dendrite to soma was 5.82 ± 0.17 Hz for PT neurons (n = 51) and 1.09 ± 0.42 Hz for IT neurons (n = 19). Notably, signals with higher-frequency components (>7 Hz) were filtered similarly as they passed from dendrite to soma in the two neuron types. Thus, differences in the amplitude attenuation of voltage signals depended greatly on which frequency components it contained. Differences in the ZAPs of PT and IT neurons were likely due to differences in the h current (Dembrow et al., 2010). The distinct filtering properties of PT and IT neurons were further accentuated in the apical dendrite where hyperpolarization-activated cyclic-nucleotide gated ion channels (h channels) are enriched (Williams and Stuart, 2000; Berger et al., 2001; Kalmbach et al., 2013). To test whether h channels were necessary for the bandpass characteristics observed in PT neurons, we bath applied the h-channel blocker 4 ZD7288 (20 μm, n = 5). In ZD7288, dendritic resonance frequency was reduced from 6.35 ± 1.37 to 1.29 ± 0.24 Hz (paired t test, p < 0.05, n = 5) and transfer frequency was reduced from 4.69 ± 0.83 to 1.06 ± 0.02 Hz (Fig. 6j, paired t test, p < 0.05, n = 5).
In addition to being attenuated in amplitude in a frequency-dependent manner, signals were also altered in phase. To quantify this, we calculated the local and transferred impedance phase profiles (ZPPs). In hippocampal pyramidal dendrites, slow-frequency signals are phase advanced relative to the current driving them, while faster frequency signals lag in phase. This is the result of the phenomenological inductance caused by the high density of h channels in the dendrites (Narayanan and Johnston, 2008; Vaidya and Johnston, 2013). We observed this phase advancement in the dendrites of PT but not IT neurons (Fig. 6f,g). The local somatic ZPP was not phase advanced at any frequency in either neuron type. As has been reported in hippocampal pyramidal neurons, the transferred ZPP (dendrite to soma) was phase advanced relative to the somatic ZPP at lower frequencies, then phase delayed at higher frequencies (Fig. 6h). At the particular frequency at which these two plots intersect, the local somatic signal and the transferred dendritic signal were synchronized. This synchronization frequency was 6.63 ± 0.21 Hz (n = 51) for PT neurons. Most IT neurons did not exhibit synchronization frequency within the detection range of our analysis (four of 19 neurons, 1.55 ± 0.16 Hz; 15 of 19 neurons, no synchronization frequency >1 Hz). These data suggest that unlike PT neurons, input to the IT dendrites will always be phase delayed relative to somatic input. Synchronization frequency in PT neurons was dependent upon h channels. Blocking h channels with ZD7288 (20 μm) made dendritic signals always lag somatic ones, effectively eliminating any synchronization frequency (Fig. 6k).
Depending on the frequency components of dendritic input, PT and IT neurons integrated signals at the soma quite differently. To test how more realistic time-varying signals were integrated, we used dynamic clamp to inject a point conductance (2–10 nS) that mimicked the synaptic response we observed using ChR2 stimulation. We injected these simEPSCs at the dendrite while simultaneously recording the voltage response from the soma (Fig. 7). In the absence of precise information regarding the kinetics and amplitude of a synaptic conductance in PT and IT neurons elicited from hippocampal and/or commissural input, we used a range of conductance amplitudes and chose a current decay that approximated our dendritic ChR2-evoked responses in PT and IT neurons (decay time constant, 2 ms).
Across a range of conductances, a single EPSP attenuated in amplitude from the dendrite to soma similarly in both neuron types (Fig. 7b). The magnitude of the amplitude attenuation depended upon the distance between the dendritic and somatic recording location (Fig. 7c). The slope of the distance-dependence increase in attenuation was not significantly different between PT and IT neurons (ANCOVA, F(1,30) = 0.017, p = 0.897). Thus, our data indicate that for single EPSPs, amplitude attenuation was not different between IT and PT neurons. To assess why this was the case, we analyzed the frequency components of the injected simEPSC signals using a CWT (for 10 nS; Fig. 8a). As expected, simEPSCs in PT and IT neurons were composed of identical frequency components. The strongest frequency component of a simEPSC peaked at ∼88 Hz, with weaker power at the lower frequencies. Thus a single simEPSC comprised primarily frequency components above the range where PT and IT dendrites had different filtering properties (>15 Hz; Fig. 6d). These data were consistent with small differences between the peak amplitudes of transferred EPSPs of PT and IT neurons.
While the amplitude of transferred EPSPs attenuated similarly, the time courses of the transferred EPSPs for PT and IT neurons were very different. The half-width of transferred EPSPs was significantly greater in IT neurons (PT: 18.39 ± 0.83 ms, n = 13; IT: 38.51 ± 2.36 ms, n = 12; Student's t test, p < 0.05). The delay to the peak of EPSPs traveling to the soma was also longer in IT neurons (Fig. 7e). These data were consistent with our sinusoid current injections, which indicated that unlike transferred ZAPs, transfer ZPPs of PT and IT neurons do not overlap at higher frequencies. To examine how PT and IT neurons integrate inputs at different distances, we compared the delay to peak of a transferred EPSP to the delay to peak of a local (somatic) EPSP. In PT neurons, the delay of the transferred EPSPs did not change significantly with increasing distances (ANCOVA, F(1,17) = 2.913, p = 0.106). In contrast, the delay in peak of the transferred EPSP increased with distance in IT neurons (ANCOVA, F(1,12) = 9.959, p < 0.01). We conclude that while the amplitude attenuation of simEPSCs was equivalent for IT and PT neurons, the temporal distortion was much more severe in IT neurons. To test whether the temporal normalization of dendritic inputs in PT neurons were caused by h channels, we tested the delays in transferred EPSPs in the presence of ZD7288 (20 μm; n = 5). In ZD7288, there was a striking distant-dependent increase in the delay of dendritic inputs (Fig. 7e). Thus, the temporal normalization along the apical dendrite in PT neurons depends upon h currents.
Recordings of both simEPSCs and ChR2-evoked EPSPs indicated that, for a single EPSP, the primary effect of the unique filtering properties of PT and IT dendrites was upon the time course and not the peak of synaptic events. As such, trains of synaptic input might be filtered differently between PT and IT neurons. To test this, we triggered five simEPSCs in the dendrites across a range of frequencies (20–200 Hz) and examined how they transferred to the soma (Fig. 7e). Within a select burst range (40–140 Hz), transferred EPSPs summated (fifth peak/first peak) more in IT neurons compared with PT neurons. To test what burst frequencies were dependent upon h channels in PT neurons, we bath applied ZD7288 (20 μm). We found that at all burst frequencies tested, ZD7288 enhanced the summation of EPSPs at the soma (Fig. 7i; ANOVA with repeated measures, F(9,36) = 411.2, p < 0.01, post hoc comparisons, p < 0.01). Thus, as expected, h-channel blockade greatly enhanced temporal summation in PT neurons. Analyzing the frequency components of simEPSCs using CWT revealed that simEPSC bursts also possess slower frequency components (Fig. 8d–j). The peak frequency of this slow component spans frequency ranges over which PT and IT ZAPs differed significantly. To examine whether this might account for differences in the summation of transferred bursts, we compared the transfer impedance of PT versus IT neurons at the peak of the slow component for each burst (normalizing for its relative power). Matching our summation data, we found that in the gamma frequency range (40–120 Hz), the efficiency of signal transfer to the soma was much greater in IT neurons (Fig. 8f,i). The frequency components of the somatic voltage waveform revealed the temporal distortion of bursts in PT versus IT neurons. In PT neurons, the voltage signals comprised a slow peak frequency component (2–10 Hz) that increased depending upon the input burst frequency (Fig. 8j). In contrast, the voltage waveform in IT neurons across a wide range of burst input frequencies (100–200 Hz) exhibited the same peak frequency (4.16 ± 0.12 Hz, n = 54; Fig. 8j) with equivalent power (Fig. 8k). These data suggest that while PT somatic voltage waveforms reflect the burst frequency of dendritic input, IT somatic voltage waveforms do not.
The temporal distortion of dendritic inputs traveling to the soma suggested that IT neurons would integrate spatially distributed synaptic input over a wider temporal window. To test how L5 somata integrated input from two different locations [(1) the nexus of the apical dendrite; (2) the soma], we triggered dendritic and somatic simEPSCs at intervals spanning from −50 to + 90 ms (Δ10 ms steps; Fig. 7f). Dendritic and somatic simEPSCs triggered simultaneously made the somatic simulated EPSP significantly larger (Fig. 7g; PT: t = 4.9008, df = 11, p < 0.01; IT: t = 4.5063, df = 5, p < 0.05). Spatial summation was greater in IT neurons than in PT neurons and occurred over a wider temporal window (Fig. 7f). The magnitude of summation in IT neurons was distance dependent: more distal dendritic EPSPs correlated with weaker summation (r2 = 0.8628, p < 0.01). In PT neurons, there was no significant correlation between summation and the distance of the dendritic current injection (r2 = 0.0178, p = 0.6792). While PT and IT neurons both summated inputs along their dendrite at the soma, the magnitude of the summation depended upon different factors. IT neurons appeared more sensitive to the spatial distribution of synaptic inputs, whereas PT neurons were more sensitive to the temporal distribution of synaptic inputs.
Discussion
In this study we characterized how long-range synaptic inputs were integrated by the dendrites of PFC L5 projection neurons. PT and IT neurons received monosynaptic input from the hippocampus and contralateral PFC. Depending upon the input pathway and postsynaptic target, different subcompartments were targeted. In addition, PT and IT neurons filtered dendritic input differently: IT neurons integrated spatially distributed inputs with temporal distortion, while PT neurons were most responsive to spatially distributed synaptic inputs within a narrow time window. Thus, afferent inputs from contralateral cortex and the hippocampus communicate to mPFC differently by selectively targeting L5 mPFC neurons with distinct dendritic filtering properties.
Afferent input to the dendrites of L5 mPFC neurons
Optogenetic activation has previously been used to map synaptic inputs of afferent projections to sensory, motor, and prefrontal cortex (Petreanu et al., 2007; Anderson et al., 2010; Oviedo et al., 2010; Mao et al., 2011). For most neuron types examined, activating terminals over the apical dendrite while recording at the soma provides the impression that the synaptic input to the dendrite of L5 pyramidal neurons is relatively weak, while the strongest physiological response is produced from activating perisomatic terminals. However, these maps are derived from somatic measurements and cannot account for the substantial dendritic filtering of distal inputs. Our study is the first that we are aware of to directly examine the electrophysiological response in the dendrites to select long-range afferent inputs. In contrast to previous indirect measurements, we have discovered that the dendrites of L5 mPFC neurons receive significant synaptic input from long-range afferent fibers. Direct recordings from the nexus at which the apical tuft converges reveal significant dendritic depolarization (0.5–11 mV). Both light-evoked EPSPs and simulated synaptic EPSPs attenuate in amplitude greatly upon reaching the soma (ChR2-evoked: ≤9-fold reduction; simulated synaptic currents: ≤16-fold reduction). Measured at the somatic electrode, responses from activation of terminals over the apical tuft were smaller than those from perisomatic terminals. This is due to dendritic filtering: upon reaching the soma, dendritic responses attenuated to ∼15% of their original size. While not as strong as the attenuation observed in spontaneous EPSPs in L5B neurons of somatosensory cortex (∼40-fold reduction), the attenuation in PFC neurons was sufficient to effectively segregate small dendritic voltage fluctuations from the soma (Berger et al., 2001; Williams and Stuart, 2002).
Responses reported here are likely to underestimate the synaptic inputs. Voltage signals attenuate strongly from the tuft dendrites to the nexus in somatosensory L5 neurons (Berger et al., 2001; Williams and Stuart, 2002; Harnett et al., 2013), so it is likely that our recordings at the nexus are attenuated signals of synaptic input. Similarly, synaptic input onto the basal dendrites can be attenuated substantially (>30-fold) in somatosensory neurons (Nevian et al., 2007), and thus is likely to be underestimated by our somatic recordings.
What are the functional consequences of local depolarization in the apical dendrites? While single, local depolarizing events themselves may not reach the soma; they nonetheless contribute to the computing power of a neuron. Indeed, we found that with sufficient depolarization, dendritic input can drive a dendritic action potential in PT neurons that is distinct from (and can propagate to) the soma (Fig. 4e). As such, PT neurons have two distinct zones of integration: the nexus and the soma.
Even without directly triggering a dendritic action potential, dendritic depolarization can alter action potential output and local synaptic plasticity (Larkum et al., 2004; Golding et al., 2002). Dendritic EPSPs can alter the height of backpropagating action potentials by inactivating K+ channels (Watanabe et al., 2002). Dendritic depolarization coupled with backpropagating action potentials has been shown to trigger tuft-wide regenerative events that produce bursts of action potentials (Larkum et al., 1999a,b; Barth et al., 2008; Harnett et al., 2013). Such coordination between different neuronal compartments has been proposed as an associative mechanism at the single-neuron level (Larkum, 2013). Our data suggest that associative computational mechanisms also could occur in PFC neurons, but that while commissural input may more readily recruit such associative mechanisms on its own, hippocampal input would require coincident input from other synaptic sources.
Projection-specific targeting of afferent inputs
Direct hippocampal input was more robust in IT neurons than PT neurons, both in terms of the monosynaptic connection rates and the amplitude of responses (Fig. 2). This finding extended to the dendrites, where PT neurons infrequently received direct hippocampal input (Fig. 5d). In contrast, commissural input directly targeted the dendrites and the soma of both PT and IT neurons, and elicited similar amplitude EPSPs both neuron types. Thus, commissural input appears to directly communicate with PT neurons in a manner that hippocampal input does not. As a result, interhemispheric communication appears to be particularly privileged in directly targeting mPFC output neurons.
In addition to direct input, many hippocampally driven EPSPs in PT neurons were polysynaptic. Both long-range afferent inputs triggered strong dendritic responses in PT neurons that were reduced considerably in conditions that blocked polysynaptic activity (Fig. 5). These data are consistent with how PT neurons are connected within the PFC microcircuit. Both L2/3 and L5 IT neurons excite PT neurons (Morishima and Kawaguchi, 2006; Morishima et al., 2011; Otsuka and Kawaguchi, 2011; Hirai et al., 2012). Additionally, PT neurons form a high rate of reciprocal connections, meaning that PT neurons receive synaptic input from other PT neurons (Morishima et al., 2011). Although the majority of local connections have been proposed to synapse perisomatically or over the basal dendrites (Morishima and Kawaguchi, 2006; Morishima et al., 2011; Otsuka and Kawaguchi, 2011; Hirai et al., 2012), our data suggest that local connections may also target dendritic compartments. In addition to the direct connections we characterize here, robustly activating commissural input recruits feedforward excitation and inhibition (Lee et al., 2014). Interestingly, feedforward inhibition appears only in PT neurons. As a result, IT neurons may be more likely to form reverberant loops of activity, linking PFC subregions in the contralateral hemisphere. The physiological significance of synchronization across hemispheres for PFC function remains unclear. However, it is notable that disruption of these fibers results in deficits in working memory function (Miu et al., 2006).
Dendritic filtering in PT versus IT neurons
Neuronal networks that comprise neurons that favor coincidence detection versus temporal integration are hypothesized to selectively perform synchrony and rate coding, respectively (Ratté et al., 2013). Whether neurons function as coincidence detectors or temporal integrators is determined by the set of ion channels dominating their membrane dynamics. When suprathreshold, the operational mode of neurons can also be strongly influenced by voltage-gated and calcium-gated potassium channels, as well as voltage-gated sodium channels (Higgs et al., 2006; Higgs and Spain, 2011). In addition, subthreshold properties influence the suprathreshold firing frequency within particular firing rate and synaptic input regimes (Richardson et al., 2003; Broicher et al., 2012; Das and Narayanan, 2014).
We have found that for subthreshold dendritic inputs, PT neurons act more as coincidence detectors while IT neurons act as temporal integrators. Both neuron categories temporally distorted dendritic signals, but this distortion was greater in IT neurons. PT neurons exhibited distance-dependent temporal normalization, suggesting that synchronous inputs distributed across a wide spatial range could summate at the soma (Fig. 7d). As a result, PT neurons were tuned to respond selectively to coincident inputs distributed along their dendritic arbor. We found that h-channel blockade eliminates the coincident detection features that PT neurons display (Figs. 6j,k, 7e,i). Thus, h channels appear to contribute strongly to the mechanism by which PT neurons selectively respond to synchronous input. Similar temporal normalization has been observed in L5B somatosensory neurons and hippocampal CA1 neurons, where it is caused by the increasing density of h current along the apical dendrite (Williams and Stuart, 2002; Vaidya and Johnston, 2013). In contrast, IT neurons did not exhibit temporal normalization: dendritic signals lagged somatic ones across all frequencies. These delays made IT neurons integrate signals that were injected both at the dendrite and soma over a wider temporal window (Fig. 7f). While our data are limited to only two locations (i.e., at the nexus of the apical dendrite and at the soma), they suggest that IT neurons are more sensitive to spatially distributed signals along their dendritic arbor.
A few important caveats must be considered in our characterization of the integrative properties of IT versus PT neurons. To examine synaptic integration, we chose to inject identical simulated synaptic conductances into PT and IT neurons. Even when PT and IT neurons receive identical synaptic input, the dendritic voltage response they exhibited was distinct (Fig. 7b). Of course, we cannot exclude the possibility the synaptic contacts from hippocampal or commissural inputs onto PT versus IT dendrites might additionally possess different physiological properties. For example, the amplitude, kinetics, and/or ratio of AMPA to NMDA receptors may differ depending upon the postsynaptic neuron and presynaptic input. Furthermore, it is possible that with the parallel recruitment of local interneurons included (Lee et al., 2014), the integrative properties of PT versus IT neurons might also change. Inhibitory interneurons alter how neurons integrate inputs arriving at different dendritic compartments (Palmer et al., 2012; Chiu et al., 2013). Nonetheless, we find that without any of these distinctions included, the dendritic intrinsic properties alone can alter the integration of synaptic signals.
The distinct filtering properties of PT and IT dendrites suggest that how long-range afferent inputs are processed depends upon which neuron type is targeted. Commissural inputs to the mPFC would be processed in parallel: sensitive to both highly synchronous (via PT dendrites) and temporally distributed (via IT dendrites) signals (Fig. 9). In contrast, direct hippocampal input, which primarily targeted IT dendrites, would be most responsive to temporally distributed inputs, particularly within the gamma frequency range, one of the noted correlates of working memory in mPFC (Howard et al., 2003; Roux et al., 2012). The IT somatic response to a wide range of inputs (100–200 Hz) displayed similar temporal components, always resulting in somatic waveform that was primarily 4 Hz. In light of this, it is intriguing that a similar frequency synchronizes activity between the hippocampus and the PFC immediately before behavioral choice (Fujisawa and Buzsáki, 2011). Our findings suggest that one mechanism by which a cortical region can extract particular information from an afferent input is by selectively targeting the dendrites of neurons with disparate filtering properties.
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grants MH048432 and MH094839 and a Memory and Cognitive Disorders Award from the McKnight Foundation to D.J. and Michael Mauk, the Human Frontiers Science Program to B.V.Z., and a Brain & Behavior Foundation Young Investigator award from the Walter K. Sartory Estate to N.C.D. Special thanks to Eedann McCord and Moriah Gottman for performing surgeries; Drs. Sachin Vaidya and Richard Gray for help and input on implementing dynamic-clamp, transfer, and wavelet analysis code; and Drs. Raymond Chitwood, Brian Kalmbach, and Darrin Brager for discussions and input pertaining to this project.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Nikolai Dembrow, Center for Learning and Memory, the University of Texas at Austin, 1 University Station Stop C7000, Austin, TX 78712-0805. nikolai{at}ca1.clm.utexas.edu