Abstract
Following unilateral stroke, the contralateral (paretic) body side is often severely impaired, and individuals naturally learn to rely more on the nonparetic body side, which involves learning new skills with it. Such compensatory hyper-reliance on the “good” body side, however, can limit functional improvements of the paretic side. In rats, motor skill training with the nonparetic forelimb (NPT) following a unilateral infarct lessens the efficacy of rehabilitative training, and reduces neuronal activation in perilesion motor cortex. However, the underlying mechanisms remain unclear. In the present study, we investigated how forelimb movement representations and synaptic restructuring in perilesion motor cortex respond to NPT and their relationship with behavioral outcomes. Forelimb representations were diminished as a result of NPT, as revealed with intracortical microstimulation mapping. Using transmission electron microscopy and stereological analyses, we found that densities of axodendritic synapses, especially axo-spinous synapses, as well as multiple synaptic boutons were increased in the perilesion cortex by NPT. The synaptic density was negatively correlated with the functional outcome of the paretic limb, as revealed in reaching performance. Furthermore, in animals with NPT, there was dissociation between astrocytic morphological features and axo-spinous synaptic density in perilesion motor cortex, compared with controls. These findings demonstrate that skill learning with the nonparetic limb following unilateral brain damage results in aberrant synaptogenesis, potentially of transcallosal projections, and this seems to hamper the functionality of the perilesion motor cortex and the paretic forelimb.
Introduction
Regenerative plasticity in the injured brain is greatly shaped by behavioral experiences and linked with functional outcomes (Allred et al., 2014). Unilateral brain damage frequently causes humans and other animals to rely heavily on the nonparetic body side and to neglect to use the paretic side, limiting functionality of the impaired side due to learned nonuse (Bury and Jones, 2002; Taub et al., 2002; Adkins et al., 2004). The intact hemisphere also can have disruptive influences on the injured one, which is linked with worsened behavioral outcome in stroke patients (Ward and Cohen, 2004; Grefkes and Fink, 2011). Skilled reach training with the nonparetic forelimb (NPT) following unilateral motor cortical lesions interferes with the behavioral benefits of subsequent rehabilitative training (RT) of the paretic forelimb in rodents (Allred et al., 2005, 2010; Allred and Jones, 2008). The detrimental effect of NPT on paretic limb function is dependent upon transcallosal connections and contralesional cortex (Allred et al., 2010). Furthermore, training with the nonparetic limb decreases FosB/ΔFosB expression in the perilesion cortex (Allred et al., 2008), an early gene accumulating in response to repeated neuronal activation (McClung et al., 2004). This is potentially problematic given that plasticity in perilesion cortex is strongly implicated in functional outcome in both animals and humans (Nudo, 2013). However, a great deal is still unknown about the neural mechanisms underlying the maladaptive effects of compensatory learning with the nonparetic limb. The present study begins to reveal these mechanisms.
We found that NPT reduced forelimb representations and increased synapses in perilesion motor cortex compared with controls, which were negatively correlated with paretic limb functional improvements. Unlike the synaptogenesis observed following RT (Hsu et al., 2007), which includes increases in functionally mature perforated synapses (Ganeshina et al., 2004a), NPT increased only multiple synaptic boutons (MSBs), potentially indicating heightened synaptic competition. Furthermore, while axo-spinous synaptic densities were strongly correlated with quantities of perisynaptic astrocytes in controls, this was not found in animals receiving NPT. Our data indicate that aberrant synaptic plasticity is likely to be a mechanism underlying the detrimental effects of compensatory learning with the nonparetic body side after unilateral stroke.
Materials and Methods
Animals, surgeries, and experimental design.
This study used animals from a previous study (Allred et al., 2009). The prior study examined the effects of learning with the nonparetic forelimb a skilled reaching task, which before the stroke had been performed proficiently with the paretic limb (Fig. 1A). This reduced improvements in the paretic limb during subsequent RT on the same task. Briefly, 35 adult (5-month-old), male Long–Evans Hooded rats (Harlan Laboratories) were used. Lesions were induced by pial application of endothelin-1, a vasoconstrictive peptide, to partially damage the caudal forelimb area (CFA). Groups were matched for lesion-induced reaching deficits as measured 5 d postlesion. Animals thereafter underwent NPT (60 trials/d for 5 d, and 120 trials/d for 10 d) or control procedures, followed by RT with the paretic limb (30 trials/d for 5 d, and 60 trials/d for an additional 5 d). The 10 d RT period was chosen to prevent intense paretic limb training from masking NPT effects. NPT began either 6 or 20 d postlesion, such that there were the following four groups: Control_Early (n = 7), NPT_Early (n = 7), Control_Late (n = 8), and NPT_Late (n = 9). Training was on the single-pellet retrieval task, as described previously (Allred et al., 2010). In the control procedure, animals were placed in the reaching chamber for the same time period with food pellets on the floor. Reaching performance of the paretic limb was quantified as the percentage successful retrievals/reach attempt. For intracortical microstimulation (ICMS), an additional 18 maps from naive rats were used as reference maps, and a small subset received no training control procedures throughout the study period (Untrained, n = 4) to aid interpretations of the RT effects. All animal use was in accordance with a protocol approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Texas at Austin. Experimenters were blind to conditions during ICMS and electron microscopy data collection.
Intracortical microstimulation mapping.
At the end of RT, rats underwent standard ICMS procedures to reveal the organization of the remaining movement representations in injured motor cortex, as previously described (Boychuk et al., 2011; Tennant et al., 2011). Briefly, under superficial ketamine/xylazine anesthesia, 350 Hz trains of cathodal pulses (13 × 200 μs) were delivered via microelectrodes in layer V. Movements elicited at the lowest thresholds (60 μA maximum) defined movement representations. Remaining forelimb, as well as jaw and neck, representations of caudal motor cortex (CMC; primary motor) and rostral motor cortex (RMC; premotor/supplementary motor) were mapped at 470 × 470 μm increments in their entirety. There were no group differences in proportions of proximal versus distal forelimb, or jaw versus neck, representations, and these were combined for analyses of forelimb and head representations, respectively. Maps were analyzed with respect to motor map subdivisions defined in naive animals (Fig. 1B). Alignment with these subdivisions was verified by histological measures of cytoarchitectural landmark positions in injured cortex and by comparison with less detailed maps of contralesional cortex that were generated after the perilesion maps. Representations were analyzed as relative areas (forelimb minus head) or proportions of total responsive area.
Histology.
Immediately after ICMS mapping, microinjections of DiI into anterior, posterior, and lateral borders of the CFA were made to guide sample dissections. Rats were then transcardially perfused with 0.1 m phosphate buffer (PB) with heparin sodium (0.05 g/L) followed by 2% paraformaldehyde and 2.5% glutaraldehyde in PB. Samples between 1.2 and 1.6 mm anterior to bregma, lateral to the lesions, and within lateral agranular cortex were chosen because this was the most common intact CMC region where forelimb movements were evoked (Fig. 1B). In a subset (n = 3 controls, n = 5 NPT rats), this sample location did not include forelimb representations as revealed in ICMS, but there was no difference in anatomical variables compared with those that did. Samples were osmicated, dehydrated, resin embedded, and sectioned as described previously (Kim and Jones, 2010). Serial coronal semithin sections (800 nm) stained with Toluidine Blue were used to guide sample block trimming to layer V. Serial ultrathin (70 nm) sections were mounted on Formvar-coated slotted grids and stained with lead citrate. Samples from one animal (NPT_Late group) were omitted due to histological errors.
Transmission electron microscopy and stereological quantification.
Axodendritic (spine and shaft) synaptic density was estimated using the physical disector method (Sterio, 1984). Four sets of four serially positioned micrographs per brain were obtained using an FEI Tecnai transmission electron microscope with an Advanced Microscopy Techniques Advantage HR 1 megapixel digital camera (16,500× image capture magnification, digitally viewed at 50,000×). All images were coded to obscure experimental conditions. The criteria to identify synapses and astrocytic processes were the same as described previously (Kim and Jones, 2010). Synaptic density was calculated as the sum of synapses counted per brain divided by sample volume (138.3 μm3), the product of the sample frame area (82.3 μm2), section thickness (70 nm), and number of disector pairs (24 pairs). Synapse subtypes were classified, including the following: (1) perforated synapses possessing discontinuous postsynaptic densities; (2) MSBs forming synapses with more than one distinct dendritic element (spine and/or shaft); (3) multiple synaptic spines (MSSs) forming synapses with more than one axonal bouton; and (4) axo-spinous, (5) axo-shaft, and (6) symmetric synapses. The software program RECONSTRUCT (Fiala, 2005) was used to trace astrocytic processes and estimate their volume (Vastro) and surface area (Saastro). Volume fraction (Vv = Vastro/total sample volume) and surface area/volume ratio (Saastro/Vastro) were then calculated.
Statistical analyses.
Data were analyzed using Prism 6 (GraphPad). Because there were no effects of the onset of training on anatomical measures, early and late groups were combined to simplify the presentation. Data were analyzed with unbiased Student's t tests and Pearson correlations with two-tailed significance tests. Effects were considered significant at p < 0.05. Descriptive results are presented as the mean ± SE.
Results
Learning with the nonparetic limb diminishes forelimb, at the expense of head, representations in perilesion motor cortex
Previously, NPT beginning either 6 or 20 d postlesion was found to interfere with spontaneous recovery of the paretic limb and its functional improvements over 10 d of subsequent RT (Allred et al., 2009). In the present study, we found that NPT diminished the representation of the paretic forelimb in perilesion cortex as revealed with ICMS (Fig. 1C,D). Overall, NPT groups had more head representation area in CMC (0.814 ± 0.120 mm2) than forelimb representation area in CMC (0.455 ± 0.110), whereas controls had more forelimb representation area in CMC (0.945 ± 0.271) than head representation area in CMC (0.358 ± 0.052). The relative area of forelimb representations was significantly reduced in NPT rats versus controls in CMC (t(29) = 3.20, p = 0.003), but not in RMC (t(28) = 0.60). There were no significant group differences in lesion volume (NPT rats, 10.02 ± 1.36 mm3; controls, 10.67 ± 0.88 mm3), total CMC map area (NPT rats, 1.90 ± 0.24 mm2; controls, 1.80 ± 0.40 mm2), or movement thresholds (NPT rats, 41.7 ± 1.4 μA; controls, 44.1 ± 1.6 μA), which were similar for head (44.3 ± 1.8 μA) and forelimb (42.2 ± 1.3 μA) representations.
Because the CFA core region was mapped exhaustively, we also analyzed other movement representations here, which were largely vibrissae representations. Considering animals with any remaining representations in CFA core (n = 14 NPT rats; n = 13 controls), forelimb representations made up the majority of the remaining map of controls (75.0 ± 8.12% vs 32.1 ± 9.3% in NPT rats; t(25) = 3.46, p = 0.002). In contrast, jaw/neck and vibrissae representations together made up the majority in NPT rats (64.9 ± 9.8% vs 23.8 ± 7.8% in controls; t(25) = 3.25, p = 0.003). In rats without training during either NPT or RT periods (Untrained, n = 4), head representations also dominated the CFA core map (83.3 ± 14.4% jaw/neck/vibrissae). This suggests that RT maintained and/or resurrected forelimb movement representations in the remaining CFA of controls and that NPT countered this effect.
Paretic forelimb performance as averaged over test days was positively correlated with the proportion of forelimb representations in the CFA core (r = 0.49, p = 0.011) and their relative area in CMC overall (r = 0.39, p = 0.032). Together, these results suggest that the capacity of RT to promote functionally relevant motor map organization in perilesion cortex is disrupted by NPT.
Learning with the nonparetic limb increases synaptic density in perilesion motor cortex, and this is negatively correlated with paretic limb function
In the same region showing diminished forelimb representations, axodendritic synaptic density was found to be significantly higher in NPT groups than in controls (t(28) = 2.45, p = 0.02; Fig. 2C). The density of axo-spinous synapses was increased in NPT groups compared with controls (t(28) = 2.97, p = 0.006; Fig. 2D), while axo-shaft synaptic densities did not change (t(28) = 0.32, p = 0.75). Thus, the newly formed synapses were mostly axo-spinous. There were no group differences in neuronal densities (data not shown).
To determine whether synapse density in perilesion cortex was associated with functional outcome, correlations between synaptic density and reaching performance were examined. There was a negative correlation between densities of axodendritic synapses and paretic limb reaching performance on the first day of its testing after NPT (r = −0.57, p = 0.001). There was a stronger inverse relationship between paretic limb reaching performance and the density of axo-spinous synapses (Fig. 2E). The inverse relationship between reaching performance and synaptic densities remained on the last day of RT (Day10; with axodendritic synapses, r = −0.46, p = 0.01; with axo-spinous synapses; Fig. 2F). Correlations within individual groups were also examined. Axo-spinous synaptic densities of both groups had an inverse relationship with reaching performance on the first day of paretic limb testing (r = −0.65, p < 0.01 for controls; r = −0.56, p < 0.05 for NPT rats). While such an inverse relationship between synapses and reaching performance still existed in NPT on the last day of RT (Day10; r = −0.56, p < 0.05), there was no significance correlation in controls (r = −0.23, p > 0.05). Similarly, there was an inverse correlation between axo-spinous synapses and the relative area of forelimb representations in the sampled region (Fig. 2G). Thus, the induction by NPT of new axo-spinous synapses in the perilesion cortex is associated with neither the functional integrity of surviving forelimb cortex nor improvements in the paretic limb.
NPT induces the formation of multiple synaptic boutons in perilesion motor cortex
To characterize which synapse subtypes were formed in the perilesion cortex following NPT, densities of four subtypes were examined (Fig. 3). While there were no group differences in other types of synapses (p values >0.05), NPT increased the density of MSBs in the perilesion cortex (t(28) = 2.33, p = 0.027; Fig. 3F). The density of single synaptic boutons (non-MSBs) was similar across the group (controls, 0.820 ± 0.031; NPT rats, 0.900 ± 0.034; p = 0.091). Thus, increases in synaptic density in perilesion cortex following NPT primarily resulted from the addition of MSBs.
Coordinated plasticity between astrocytes and synapses was not apparent following NPT
Perisynaptic astrocytic processes rapidly alter their morphology and change proximity with synapses in coordination with structural changes in spines (Haber et al., 2006). We examined whether there was a tight association between astrocytic morphological features and synaptic densities in perilesion cortex. While axo-spinous synaptic densities in controls were significantly correlated with both astrocytic volume fraction and surface area/volume ratio (p values <0.05), there was no such significant association following NPT (Fig. 4). NPT did not alter astrocytic volume fraction or surface area/volume ratio (p values >0.05).
Discussion
Many previous studies (Johansson, 2000; Murphy and Corbett, 2009; Nudo, 2013) have investigated how behavioral experiences can promote functionally beneficial brain plasticity after injury. However, unilateral brain damage often leaves the use of paretic limbs neglected, a condition called “learned nonuse” (Taub et al., 2002), and increases reliance on the nonparetic side. We found that NPT had a major influence on the organization of perilesion motor cortex. After 10 d of RT, the remaining motor maps of controls had expanded representations of the paretic forelimb, linked with better behavioral function, while the maps of those that underwent NPT before RT were dominated by head representations. Movement representations are maintained by activity-dependent competition between bordering representations (Nudo, 2013). Thus, NPT appears to diminish effective competition between surviving forelimb and surrounding head representations. Surprisingly, this effect was linked with increased axo-spinous synapses in perilesion cortex. These synapses had an inverse relationship with functional improvements of the paretic limb and its representation in perilesion cortex. A specific synapse subtype, synapses formed by MSBs, was the most prominent contributor to NPT-induced increases in synapse number. While there was a tight association between astrocytic morphological features and synapse numbers in controls, this was not found after NPT. Therefore, these findings indicate that NPT induces aberrant synaptic plasticity that is neither associated with paretic limb improvements nor is accompanied by coordinated astrocytic changes.
The detrimental effect of skill learning with the nonparetic limb is dependent upon both transcallosal connections and contralesional cortex (Allred et al., 2010). A higher density of synapses potentially reflects a greater quantity of transcallosal connections from the contralesional cortex. Most transcallosal cortical projections form direct excitatory synapses onto pyramidal neurons and GABAergic interneurons (Kawaguchi, 1992; Karayannis et al., 2007). Furthermore, either bilateral training (Allred and Jones, 2008) or home-cage bimanual experience (Kerr et al., 2013) can attenuate deleterious NPT effects, suggesting substantial involvement of interhemispheric competition in these effects.
Whatever the afferent source, the promotion of synaptogenesis by NPT can be assumed to alter connectivity in perilesion cortex, and this may compete with the capacity of RT to do so. The finding that NPT increases MSBs is consistent with greater synaptic competition. In rat cerebellum, excitatory synapses adjacent to MSBs are smaller, suggesting a compensatory weakening of synapses adjacent to MSBs (Lee et al., 2013). Thus, the proliferation of MSBs after NPT may reflect the strengthening of synapses activated during NPT, which then dampened the efficacy of the paretic limb in driving activity-dependent plasticity of neighboring synapses, interfering with establishment of more functionally relevant circuits.
The finding that animals with NPT had a greater overall density of axodendritic synapses, but not perforated synapses, a subtype having abundant AMPA receptors and greater synaptic efficacy (Ganeshina et al., 2004a, b), also implies altered synaptic maturation. In contrast, RT increases the density of perforated synapses, which is tightly associated with functional improvements (Hsu et al., 2007). Further study is needed to investigate whether receptor distributions and synaptic activity are affected by NPT.
Experience-dependent plasticity involves rapid structural changes in synapses, particularly dendritic spines (Xu et al., 2009). Astrocytes are highly motile in accordance with dendritic spine structural change (Haber et al., 2006). The coordinated structural plasticity between astrocytes and spines occurs prominently when synapses are functionally active (Perez-Alvarez et al., 2014). Astrocytes actively eliminate weak synapses when overall neuronal activity is heightened (Chung et al., 2013) and refine synaptic connectivity via synaptic competition (Risher et al., 2014) in cortex. The tight association between astrocytic morphological features and axo-spinous synapses in controls may reflect the coordination of astrocytic changes with that of synapses activated by paretic limb training during RT. Furthermore, synapses with more morphologically mature features have more stable contacts with astrocytes (Haber et al., 2006), yet have a longer astrocyte-free perimeter around synaptic cleft, presumably increasing synaptic cross talk (Witcher et al., 2007). The latter may partly explain the inverse correlation between astrocytic volume fraction and synapse number in controls. In contrast, NPT-induced synaptogenesis may mostly involve synapses that are weakly activated by RT and hence are more dissociated with astrocytic structure. In any event, the finding that the controls had better functional improvements and fewer synapses, but with stronger relationships with perisynaptic astrocytic structure and forelimb motor maps, is consistent with more effective activity-dependent remodeling of perilesion cortex compared with NPT. Further study of how NPT affects glia–neuron interactions is warranted by these results.
The present study demonstrates that maladaptive training with the nonparetic limb disrupts functionally relevant reorganization of perilesion motor cortex. The unexpected finding of higher densities of axo-spinous synapses and MSBs within the same region strongly implicates aberrant synaptic plasticity as a mechanism underlying the disruption of cortical reorganization and paretic limb functional improvements. We suggest that the increase in synapses, which may arise from transcallosal projections, confiscates circuitry in peri-infarct cortex, limiting its potential for later rewiring to mediate better paretic limb function.
Footnotes
This work is supported by National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grant NS-056839.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Soo Young Kim, Department of Integrative Biology, University of California, Berkeley, 430E Li Ka Shing Center, #3370, Berkeley, CA 94720-3370. sooykim{at}berkeley.edu