Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Journal Club

A Prophylactic Role for Creatine in Hypoxia?

Elisabeth Engl and Mona M. Garvert
Journal of Neuroscience 24 June 2015, 35 (25) 9249-9251; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1195-15.2015
Elisabeth Engl
1Department of Neuroscience, Physiology and Pharmacology, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, United Kingdom, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Elisabeth Engl
Mona M. Garvert
2Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, Institute of Neurology, University College London, London WC1N 3BG, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Mona M. Garvert
  • Article
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

The brain requires ∼20% of the body's total energy. When the body is deprived of energy, such as during hypoxia and in pathologies involving an altered energy metabolism, the brain's function can be severely and irreversibly disrupted. However, humans routinely cope with transient hypoxia, for example when diving under water. How do our brains resist this temporary disruption in energy supply, and is it possible to harness these mechanisms for therapeutic use?

Much of the brain's energy is spent generating synaptic and action potentials and subsequently re-establishing the resting membrane potential by powering the Na+/K+-ATPase. To fuel these and other processes, the brain generates ATP. Under normoxic conditions, the predominant source of energy is the oxidative metabolism of glucose. During hypoxia, intracellular ATP levels fall and the Na+/K+-ATPase slows down. As a consequence, ionic gradients decrease. Neurons become depolarized and may either become more excitable or enter a depolarizing block of action potential production. To prevent neuronal damage by depolarization-evoked calcium entry, adenosine is released into the extracellular space, which in turn suppresses glutamate release from presynaptic terminals, reducing synaptic activity (Fowler, 1993). Brief hypoxia or even anoxia and the resulting adenosine release are reversible and do not lead to neuronal cell death (Miyamoto and Auer, 2000). However, the external K+ concentration jumps to ∼50 mm after a few minutes of anoxia, leading to a rapid depolarization of cellular membranes, the so-called “anoxic depolarization” (Hansen, 1985). A prolonged rise of extracellular glutamate concentration then triggers widespread neuronal cell death, after which anoxic damage becomes irreversible, leading to severe neuropsychological impairments.

Importantly, several reserve energy substrates can be used to replenish ATP without a need for oxygen, thus delaying the anoxia-induced alteration of ion gradients. In addition to glycogen, creatine is used as such an energy buffer in the brain. Creatine kinase catalyzes the dephosphorylation of phosphocreatine and the simultaneous phosphorylation of adenosine diphosphate (ADP) to ATP in the following reaction: PCr + ADP + H+ ↔ Cr + ATP.

Physiological creatine and creatine kinase concentrations are not homogeneous throughout the brain, but instead reflect regional differences in energy demands (Pouwels and Frahm, 1998); indeed, creatine kinase level correlates with brain activity as measured with EEG (Sauter and Rudin, 1993). Furthermore, both in vitro and behavioral evidence suggest that creatine is used as an emergency energy substrate, at least for a short time. Rodent hippocampal slices with fourfold enriched phosphocreatine levels retain synaptic transmission in hypoxia three times as long as without extra phosphocreatine, and ATP levels take 90 instead of 30 s to decline (Lipton and Whittingham, 1982). While remaining constant at rest, the ratio of hippocampal phosphocreatine to ATP concentration declines significantly when glucose uptake is impaired under cognitive load, suggesting that phosphocreatine is converted to creatine to replenish ATP (Emmanuel et al., 2013).

Can artificially increasing phosphocreatine levels help conserve neuronal function in disorders of energy supply? Studies of the clinical benefits of creatine administration have been contradictory. In a mouse model of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS), a disease characterized in part by mitochondrial dysfunction and therefore impaired energy supply, oral creatine supplementation protected against neuronal loss and improved motor performance. Crucially, it also led to longer survival rates, from 144 d with an unsupplemented diet to 157 d with 1% creatine and 169 d with 2% creatine (Klivenyi et al., 1999). Klivenyi et al. (1999) hypothesized that creatine both acted as an energy buffer and prevented the mitochondrial transition pore from opening (O'Gorman et al., 1997), thereby preventing the mitochondrial swelling and cell death characteristic of ALS. In contrast, a comparable dose of creatine in ALS patients failed to improve motor behavior (Shefner et al., 2004). Similarly, in patients with Huntington's disease, which involves a defective energy metabolism and excitotoxicity, 2-year oral supplementation of creatine did not improve motor or cognitive function (Tabrizi et al., 2005).

Turner and colleagues (2015) tried to address these discrepancies by investigating whether prophylactic oral creatine intake prevented the detrimental effects of hypoxia on cognitive performance in humans. The authors tested corticomotor excitability by measuring motor-evoked potentials with TMS and assessed cognitive performance on a variety of tasks contained in a standard neuropsychological test battery under normoxic conditions and after 90 min of hypoxia. Participants (n = 15) underwent the test procedure twice. Seven days of placebo or oral creatine administration (20 g/d), allocated according to a double-blind randomized procedure, were followed by a wash-out period of 5 weeks, after which the procedure was repeated with the other treatment (creatine or placebo, respectively). Using magnetic resonance spectroscopy, the authors demonstrated that creatine treatment increased creatine and phosphocreatine levels by 9.2% on average in a region of interest over the hand motor area, the same area used to determine corticomotor excitability.

Hypoxia was induced by having participants breathe air containing an inspired oxygen fraction of 10%, or half the level in air at sea level. The arterial oxygen saturation fell by 19% as expected from the oxygen-hemoglobin dissociation curve (Turner et al., 2015, their Fig. 3), which is well below the clinical threshold for hypoxemia (Majumdar et al., 2011). To put that level of hypoxia into perspective, an inspired oxygen fraction of 10% approximately corresponds to air at the summit of Mount Whitney, at an altitude of around 4.4 km.

Hypoxia impaired a wide range of neuropsychological measures, including alertness, executive function, and verbal memory. Creatine supplementation significantly reduced the detrimental effects of hypoxia on a factor the authors call “complex attention” (Turner et al., 2015, their Table 1 and Fig. 4). Turner and colleagues (2015) assessed complex attention by pooling performance errors across three cognitive tasks: a Stroop test, in which subjects concurrently processed the semantic and the visual features of a color word; a Shifting Attention test, in which subjects switched between task instructions; and a Continuous Performance test, in which subjects responded to target, but not distractor letters. The effect of creatine on composite memory, psychomotor speed, and the overall neurocognitive index were statistically inconclusive, even though all domains were adversely affected by hypoxia. Given the heterogeneous distribution of creatine in the brain, it is conceivable that these differences in behavior reflect differences in creatine sensitivity across networks engaged in these different tasks. Similarly, the differences in cognitive effects for hypoxia under placebo and under creatine might indicate that hypoxia and creatine differentially affect different cognitive domains.

It is worth noting that the authors tested participants on a battery comprising a large number of neurocognitive tests. Testing for independent effects would require corrections for multiple comparisons, which would have eliminated the statistically significant effect of creatine supplementation. However, in this case it is unlikely that all of the neurocognitive tests are truly independent. Several identical subscores were included in different composite cognitive domains scores, and different domains, such as psychomotor speed and reaction time, probably recruit similar cognitive processes. In consequence, corrections for multiple comparisons might be overly conservative. To tease apart the effects of creatine on specific cognitive processes, future studies using a similar cognitive test battery could first perform a dimensionality reduction to clearly identify independent cognitive domains.

Turner and colleagues (2015) hypothesize that the effect of creatine on cognitive performance might, at least in part, be due to creatine's energy-buffering capabilities. However, it is unlikely that creatine can supply energy for the entire 90 min of hypoxia. In the absence of ATP production, all the ATP in the rat brain will be consumed in ∼7 s (Allen et al., 2005). The concentration of free ATP in the rodent and human brain is ∼2.5 mm, while the phosphocreatine concentration is ∼4.9 mm (Roth and Weiner, 1991). Thus, ATP produced from the physiological level of phosphocreatine should be able to lengthen the time until ATP is consumed by ∼12 s in rat (Allen et al., 2005), or 24 s in humans and nonhuman primates [because primate gray matter consumes only around half as much energy as rodent gray matter, or 0.2 mm ATP per second (Kennedy et al., 1978)]. Since the increase of creatine and phosphocreatine produced was <1 mm (as shown in Turner et al. (2015), their Fig. 2A), any energy-buffering effect of the extra creatine must occur within a few seconds and should therefore be restricted to the first neuro-psychological test that participants performed. As Turner and colleagues (2015) do not indicate whether the order of the tests was randomized, it is not clear whether the selective effects of creatine on complex attention might be explained by it being the first test in the battery.

Any longer-lasting effect of creatine on cognitive performance would instead support the alternative hypothesis of Turner and colleagues (2015) of a substantial neuromodulatory role of creatine, possibly by directly enhancing the function of the Na+/K+-ATPase (Rambo et al., 2012). Creatine also directly stimulates glutamate uptake into synaptic vesicles (Xu et al., 1996).

Superficially, one might expect an increase of corticomotor excitability in hypoxia, altering the cortical excitation/inhibition balance with potentially detrimental consequences for cognitive performance. The neuromodulatory effects of creatine might then reverse these effects. Instead, Turner and colleagues (2015) saw no statistically significant effect of hypoxia on excitability in placebo, but an increased excitability with creatine (their Fig. 5A). However, the analyses in Figure 5 do not allow a direct comparison of changes in corticomotor excitability between placebo and creatine, as the amplitudes were separately normalized to the largest motor-evoked potential recorded in the placebo and creatine sessions, and so are not comparable across conditions.

In comparison to clinical studies using a similar dose of creatine that found no beneficial effects on cognitive performance, a crucial difference that could explain the efficacy of creatine supplementation in this study is that creatine was allowed to accumulate before energetic disruption. In the clinical studies by Shefner and colleagues (2004) and Tabrizi and colleagues (2005), creatine treatment was only started after ALS or Huntington's were already manifest. In contrast, creatine supplementation preceded disease onset in the ALS mouse model, where it was efficacious (Klivenyi et al., 1999). If a beneficial effect of creatine is successfully replicated, future clinical implementation could therefore focus on prophylactic, rather than post hoc, effects of creatine in the treatment of disorders involving changes in energy metabolism.

In conclusion, Turner and colleagues (2015) show that even relatively mild hypoxia impairs cognition, which might, at least in part, be recovered by oral creatine supplementation. This suggests that creatine helps to maintain normal cognitive function when energy metabolism is impaired, although the precise mechanism, as well as the reason that some cognitive processes are more affected than others, remain unexplained. As the energy-buffering effects of added creatine are of brief duration (a few seconds), future mechanistic studies should focus on the neuromodulatory actions of creatine under prolonged hypoxia.

Footnotes

  • Editor's Note: These short, critical reviews of recent papers in the Journal, written exclusively by graduate students or postdoctoral fellows, are intended to summarize the important findings of the paper and provide additional insight and commentary. For more information on the format and purpose of the Journal Club, please see http://www.jneurosci.org/misc/ifa_features.shtml.

  • This work was supported by the Wellcome Trust (4-year PhD studentship 097265/Z/11/Z to E.E. and 4-year PhD studentship 097267/Z/11/Z to M.M.G). We thank David Attwell, Renaud Jolivet, Erman Misirlisoy, and Peter Smittenaar for comments on the manuscript.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Elisabeth Engl at the above address. e.engl.11{at}ucl.ac.uk

References

  1. ↵
    1. Allen NJ,
    2. Káradóttir R,
    3. Attwell D
    (2005) A preferential role for glycolysis in preventing the anoxic depolarization of rat hippocampal area CA1 pyramidal cells. J Neurosci 25:848–859, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4157–04.2005, pmid:15673665.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    1. Emmanuel Y,
    2. Cochlin LE,
    3. Tyler DJ,
    4. de Jager CA,
    5. Smith AD,
    6. Clarke K
    (2013) Human hippocampal energy metabolism is impaired during cognitive activity in a lipid infusion model of insulin resistance. Brain Behav 3:134–144, doi:10.1002/brb3.124, pmid:23533158.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Fowler JC
    (1993) Changes in extracellular adenosine levels and population spike amplitude during graded hypoxia in the rat hippocampal slice. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch Pharmacol 347:73–78, pmid:8446184.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Hansen AJ
    (1985) Effect of anoxia on ion distribution in the brain. Physiol Rev 65:101–148, pmid:3880896.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    1. Kennedy C,
    2. Sakurada O,
    3. Shinohara M,
    4. Jehle J,
    5. Sokoloff L
    (1978) Local cerebral glucose utilization in the normal conscious macaque monkey. Ann Neurol 4:293–301, doi:10.1002/ana.410040402, pmid:103488.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Klivenyi P,
    2. Ferrante RJ,
    3. Matthews RT,
    4. Bogdanov MB,
    5. Klein AM,
    6. Andreassen OA,
    7. Mueller G,
    8. Wermer M,
    9. Kaddurah-Daouk R,
    10. Beal MF
    (1999) Neuroprotective effects of creatine in a transgenic animal model of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Nat Med 5:347–350, doi:10.1038/6568, pmid:10086395.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Lipton P,
    2. Whittingham TS
    (1982) Reduced ATP concentration as a basis for synaptic transmission failure during hypoxia in the in vitro guinea-pig hippocampus. J Physiol 325:51–65, doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1982.sp014135, pmid:6286944.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Majumdar SR,
    2. Eurich DT,
    3. Gamble JM,
    4. Senthilselvan A,
    5. Marrie TJ
    (2011) Oxygen saturations less than 92% are associated with major adverse events in outpatients with pneumonia: a population-based cohort study. Clin Infect Dis 52:325–331, doi:10.1093/cid/ciq076, pmid:21217179.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    1. Miyamoto O,
    2. Auer RN
    (2000) Hypoxia, hyperoxia, ischemia, and brain necrosis. Neurology 54:362–371, doi:10.1212/WNL.54.2.362, pmid:10668697.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. O'Gorman E,
    2. Beutner G,
    3. Dolder M,
    4. Koretsky AP,
    5. Brdiczka D,
    6. Wallimann T
    (1997) The role of creatine kinase in inhibition of mitochondrial permeability transition. FEBS Lett 414:253–257, doi:10.1016/S0014–5793(97)01045–4, pmid:9315696.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Pouwels PJ,
    2. Frahm J
    (1998) Regional metabolite concentrations in human brain as determined by quantitative localized proton MRS. Magn Reson Med 39:53–60, doi:10.1002/mrm.1910390110, pmid:9438437.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Rambo LM,
    2. Ribeiro LR,
    3. Schramm VG,
    4. Berch AM,
    5. Stamm DN,
    6. Della-Pace ID,
    7. Silva LF,
    8. Furian AF,
    9. Oliveira MS,
    10. Fighera MR,
    11. Royes LF
    (2012) Creatine increases hippocampal Na+, K+-ATPase activity via NMDA–calcineurin pathway. Brain Res Bull 88:553–559, doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2012.06.007, pmid:22742935.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Roth K,
    2. Weiner MW
    (1991) Determination of cytosolic ADP and AMP concentrations and the free energy of ATP hydrolysis in human muscle and brain tissues with 31P NMR spectroscopy. Magn Reson Med 22:505–511, doi:10.1002/mrm.1910220258, pmid:1812384.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Sauter A,
    2. Rudin M
    (1993) Determination of creatine kinase kinetic parameters in rat brain by NMR magnetization transfer. Correlation with brain function. J Biol Chem 268:13166–13171, pmid:8514755.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Shefner JM,
    2. Cudkowicz ME,
    3. Schoenfeld D,
    4. Conrad T,
    5. Taft J,
    6. Chilton M,
    7. Urbinelli L,
    8. Qureshi M,
    9. Zhang H,
    10. Pestronk A,
    11. Caress J,
    12. Donofrio P,
    13. Sorenson E,
    14. Bradley W,
    15. Lomen-Hoerth C,
    16. Pioro E,
    17. Rezania K,
    18. Ross M,
    19. Pascuzzi R,
    20. Heiman-Patterson T,
    21. et al.
    (2004) A clinical trial of creatine in ALS. Neurology 63:1656–1661, doi:10.1212/01.WNL.0000142992.81995.F0, pmid:15534251.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Tabrizi SJ,
    2. Blamire AM,
    3. Manners DN,
    4. Rajagopalan B,
    5. Styles P,
    6. Schapira AH,
    7. Warner TT
    (2005) High-dose creatine therapy for Huntington disease: A 2-year clinical and MRS study. Neurology 64:1655–1656, doi:10.1212/01.WNL.0000160388.96242.77, pmid:15883340.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Turner CE,
    2. Byblow WD,
    3. Gant N
    (2015) Creatine supplementation enhances corticomotor excitability and cognitive performance during oxygen deprivation. J Neurosci 35:1773–1780, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3113–14.2015, pmid:25632150.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    1. Xu CJ,
    2. Klunk WE,
    3. Kanfer JN,
    4. Xiong Q,
    5. Miller G,
    6. Pettegrew JW
    (1996) Phosphocreatine-dependent glutamate uptake by synaptic vesicles: a comparison with ATP-dependent glutamate uptake. J Biol Chem 271:13435–13440, doi:10.1074/jbc.271.23.13435, pmid:8662761.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 35 (25)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 35, Issue 25
24 Jun 2015
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
A Prophylactic Role for Creatine in Hypoxia?
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
A Prophylactic Role for Creatine in Hypoxia?
Elisabeth Engl, Mona M. Garvert
Journal of Neuroscience 24 June 2015, 35 (25) 9249-9251; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1195-15.2015

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
A Prophylactic Role for Creatine in Hypoxia?
Elisabeth Engl, Mona M. Garvert
Journal of Neuroscience 24 June 2015, 35 (25) 9249-9251; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1195-15.2015
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Cortical Inhibition, Plasticity, and Sleep
  • Dissociating Hippocampal and Cortical Contributions to Predictive Processing
  • Uncovering the Hippocampal Mechanisms Underpinning Spatial Learning and Flexible Navigation
Show more Journal Club
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.