Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Dopaminergic Modulation of Decision Making and Subjective Well-Being

Robb B. Rutledge, Nikolina Skandali, Peter Dayan and Raymond J. Dolan
Journal of Neuroscience 8 July 2015, 35 (27) 9811-9822; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0702-15.2015
Robb B. Rutledge
1Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, London WC1N 3BG, United Kingdom,
2Max Planck University College London Centre for Computational Psychiatry and Ageing Research, London WC1B 5EH, United Kingdom, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Robb B. Rutledge
Nikolina Skandali
1Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, London WC1N 3BG, United Kingdom,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Nikolina Skandali
Peter Dayan
3Gatsby Computational Neuroscience Unit, University College London, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Peter Dayan
Raymond J. Dolan
1Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, London WC1N 3BG, United Kingdom,
2Max Planck University College London Centre for Computational Psychiatry and Ageing Research, London WC1B 5EH, United Kingdom, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Figures

  • Tables
  • Figure 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 1.

    Task design and performance on placebo and l-DOPA. A, In each of 300 trials, subjects made choices between safe and risky options. In 100 gain trials, the worst gamble outcome was zero. In 100 loss trials, the best gamble outcome was zero. In 100 mixed trials, both gamble outcome gains and losses were possible. Chosen gambles were resolved after a brief delay period. Subjects were also asked after every 3–4 choice trials “how happy are you at this moment?” and indicated their responses by moving a slider. B, On average, subjects (n = 30) gambled most in gain trials, less in mixed trials, and least in loss trials on both placebo and l-DOPA. Error bars indicate SEM. C, Subjects did not gamble more in mixed and loss trials on l-DOPA compared with placebo, but did gamble more in gain trials. *p < 0.05. D, Subjects who received higher effective drug doses chose more gain gambles on l-DOPA than placebo (p < 0.01).

  • Figure 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 2.

    Choice behavior and economic decision model fits. A, Parametric model based on prospect theory explained choice behavior. Error bars indicate SEM. B, Parameter estimates were similar on placebo and l-DOPA. C, Gain gamble value was determined relative to the value of the certain option. Deciles 1–2 corresponded to lower expected reward for the gamble than the certain option. Decile 3 corresponded to equal expected reward for the gamble and certain option. Deciles 4–10 corresponded to higher expected reward for the gamble than the certain option. As gain gamble value increased, subjects gambled more on both placebo and l-DOPA, as expected. D, Average model fit across subjects for the prospect theory model, which cannot account for the observed difference in choice behavior on l-DOPA compared with placebo.

  • Figure 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 3.

    Approach–avoidance model fits. A, Our approach–avoidance model incorporates an additional value-independent, valence-dependent effect on choice probability that accounts for the increased gambling on l-DOPA across gamble value deciles. B, Risk aversion parameters αgain and αloss for both placebo and l-DOPA sessions were <1, indicating risk aversion in the gain domain and risk seeking in the loss domain. Approach–avoidance parameter βgain was positive (“approach”) and βloss was negative (“avoid”) in placebo and l-DOPA sessions. Error bars indicate SEM. *p < 0.05. C, βgain was higher on l-DOPA than placebo, indicating an increased tendency to choose gain gambles independent of value. D, Subjects that received higher effective drug doses had a greater increase in βgain on l-DOPA than placebo (p < 0.05).

  • Figure 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 4.

    Choice behavior and approach–avoidance model fits for mixed and loss trials. A, Mixed gamble value was determined by the ratio of the potential gain to the potential loss. As mixed gamble value increased, subjects gambled more, as expected. B, Average model fit across subjects for the approach–avoidance model. C, Subjects who received higher effective doses did not have a larger change in the number of mixed gambles chosen on l-DOPA than placebo (Spearman's ρ = 0.02, p = 0.91), a relationship that would be negative if l-DOPA decreased risk taking. D, Loss gamble value was determined relative to the value of the certain option. As loss gamble value increased, subjects gambled more, as expected. E, Average model fit across subjects for the approach–avoidance model, which can account for the low probability of gambling for even the highest gamble values in placebo and l-DOPA sessions. F, Subjects who received higher effective doses did not have a larger change in the number of loss gambles chosen on l-DOPA than placebo (Spearman's ρ = 0.19, p = 0.32), a relationship that would be negative if l-DOPA decreased risk taking.

  • Figure 5.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 5.

    Happiness ratings across sessions. A, l-DOPA did not affect the mean or SD of happiness ratings. Error bars indicate SEM. B, Mean happiness ratings were uncorrelated across placebo and l-DOPA sessions (p = 0.27). C, SD of happiness ratings was correlated across placebo and l-DOPA sessions (p < 0.001). D, l-DOPA did not affect initial or final happiness ratings or how happy subjects remembered being the day after the session (all p > 0.2). Error bars indicate SEM. E, Mean happiness ratings were correlated with how happy subjects remembered being the day after the session (both Spearman's ρ > 0.5, p < 0.01). F, Remembered happiness was uncorrelated between placebo and l-DOPA sessions (Spearman's ρ = −0.21, p = 0.27).

  • Figure 6.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 6.

    Rewards and expectations explain momentary subjective well-being. A, B, Happiness ratings and cumulative task earnings across subjects (n = 30) on placebo (A) and l-DOPA (B) (placebo: r2 = 0.49 ± 0.25; l-DOPA: r2 = 0.45 ± 0.25). Happiness model fits are displayed for the model in C. Subjects completed 300 choice trials and made a rating after every 3–4 trials for a total of 90 ratings. C, The computational model that best explained momentary happiness had positive weights for certain rewards, gamble EVs, and gamble RPEs. Error bars indicate SEM. D, An alternative computational model included separate positive and negative RPE terms. E, F, In trials with potential gains but not losses, happiness was higher after gamble wins than losses on both placebo and l-DOPA (both p < 0.05). Happiness was higher after the small rewards from low-value gain gambles on l-DOPA compared with placebo (E), but not for the large rewards from high-value gain gambles (F). Error bars indicate SEM. *p < 0.05.

Tables

  • Figures
    • View popup
    Table 1.

    Decision model comparison results

    ModelParameters per subjectMean r2Median r2Model BICBIC- BICa-a
    Prospect theory70.510.5013620883
    Approach–avoidance110.570.58127370
    Dual-inverse-temperature100.540.5213363626
    Gain-loss learning110.520.51141601423
    Approach–avoidance-mixed130.600.5912596−141
    • BIC measures are summed across the 30 subjects. Parameters per subject are across both placebo and l-DOPA sessions. All models include separate parameters for placebo and l-DOPA sessions that capture the weighting of losses relative to equivalent gains (loss aversion, λ), risk aversion in the gain domain (αgain), and risk aversion in the loss domain (αloss). All models except the dual-inverse-temperature model included a shared parameter across sessions for stochasticity in choice (inverse temperature, μ). The final column is the difference between the model BIC and BICa-a, the BIC for the approach–avoidance model. The approach–avoidance model was preferred (lower BIC) to the prospect theory model, dual-inverse-temperature model, and gain-loss learning model. The more complex approach-avoidance-mixed model included additional approach–avoidance parameters for the mixed trials and had the lowest BIC of the models tested.

    • View popup
    Table 2.

    Subjective state questionnaire results

    Subjective state questionnairePlacebo startl-DOPA startp-valuePlacebo endl-DOPA endp-value
    Alert to drowsy0.181.200.150.120.820.62
    Calm to excited−0.29−0.220.42−0.36−0.060.72
    Strong to feeble−0.170.680.060.060.510.96
    Muzzy to clear headed0.04−0.940.12−0.60−1.120.86
    Coordinated to clumsy−0.750.250.11−0.340.330.78
    Lethargic to energetic−0.15−0.670.19−0.17−0.910.21
    Contented to discontented−0.050.650.200.400.510.85
    Troubled to tranquil−0.230.050.70−0.71−0.320.62
    Slow to quick witted−0.05−0.640.04−0.44−0.680.40
    Tense to relaxed−0.04−0.540.22−0.74−0.930.44
    Attentive to dreamy0.110.830.140.010.940.09
    Incompetent to proficient−0.26−0.320.67−0.48−0.710.95
    Happy to sad−0.080.060.870.280.160.62
    Antagonistic to friendly−0.09−0.240.95−0.64−1.040.26
    Interested to bored0.140.400.530.520.641.00
    Withdrawn to sociable0.520.410.75−0.06−0.020.88
    • Scores represent differences in ratings in a subjective state questionnaire between baseline (before placebo or l-DOPA administration) and the start of the task or the end of the task. Questions were answered by marking a point on a line and responses were converted to a 0–10 scale. We used a Wilcoxon signed-rank test and the p-values listed are not corrected for multiple comparisons. We found no evidence for an overall effect of l-DOPA on any subjective state report because no test would survive any correction for multiple comparisons. However, many such corrections might be considered too conservative. For three variables, we identified trends at the 10% significance level (uncorrected) for a difference in subjective ratings between placebo and l-DOPA. For those three cases, we then tested for a dose-dependent relationship between l-DOPA and the size of the change in subjective responses because heavier subjects may not be affected and lighter subjects would be. We found no evidence for a relationship at the 10% significance level for these three tests, and therefore found no evidence supporting a dose-dependent effect of l-DOPA on these subjective state reports.

Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 35 (27)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 35, Issue 27
8 Jul 2015
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Dopaminergic Modulation of Decision Making and Subjective Well-Being
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Dopaminergic Modulation of Decision Making and Subjective Well-Being
Robb B. Rutledge, Nikolina Skandali, Peter Dayan, Raymond J. Dolan
Journal of Neuroscience 8 July 2015, 35 (27) 9811-9822; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0702-15.2015

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Dopaminergic Modulation of Decision Making and Subjective Well-Being
Robb B. Rutledge, Nikolina Skandali, Peter Dayan, Raymond J. Dolan
Journal of Neuroscience 8 July 2015, 35 (27) 9811-9822; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0702-15.2015
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • decision making
  • dopamine
  • reward prediction error
  • subjective well-being

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Articles

  • Choice Behavior Guided by Learned, But Not Innate, Taste Aversion Recruits the Orbitofrontal Cortex
  • Maturation of Spontaneous Firing Properties after Hearing Onset in Rat Auditory Nerve Fibers: Spontaneous Rates, Refractoriness, and Interfiber Correlations
  • Insulin Treatment Prevents Neuroinflammation and Neuronal Injury with Restored Neurobehavioral Function in Models of HIV/AIDS Neurodegeneration
Show more Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Disentangling Object Category Representations Driven by Dynamic and Static Visual Input
  • Irrelevant Threats Linger and Affect Behavior in High Anxiety
  • Multisession Anodal Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Enhances Adult Hippocampal Neurogenesis and Context Discrimination in Mice
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.