Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Disrupted Saccadic Corollary Discharge in Schizophrenia

Katharine N. Thakkar, Jeffrey D. Schall, Stephan Heckers and Sohee Park
Journal of Neuroscience 8 July 2015, 35 (27) 9935-9945; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0473-15.2015
Katharine N. Thakkar
1Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee 37240,
2Department of Psychiatry, University Medical Center Utrecht, 3584 CX Utrecht, The Netherlands,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Katharine N. Thakkar
Jeffrey D. Schall
1Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee 37240,
3Center for Integrative Cognitive Neuroscience and
4Vanderbilt Vision Research Center, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee 37240, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Jeffrey D. Schall
Stephan Heckers
1Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee 37240,
3Center for Integrative Cognitive Neuroscience and
5Departments of Psychiatry and
6Radiology and Radiological Sciences, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee 37232
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Stephan Heckers
Sohee Park
1Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee 37240,
3Center for Integrative Cognitive Neuroscience and
5Departments of Psychiatry and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Sohee Park
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Disruptions in corollary discharge (CD), motor signals that send information to sensory areas and allow for prediction of sensory states, are argued to underlie the perceived loss of agency in schizophrenia. Behavioral and neurophysiological evidence for CD in primates comes largely from the saccadic double-step task, which requires participants to make two visually triggered saccadic eye movements in brief succession. Healthy individuals use CD to anticipate the change in eye position resulting from the first saccade when preparing the second saccade. In the current study with human participants, schizophrenia patients and healthy controls of both sexes performed a modified double-step task. Most trials required a saccade to a single visual target (T1). On a subset of trials, a second target (T2) was flashed shortly following T1. Subjects were instructed to look directly at T2. Healthy individuals also use CD to make rapid, corrective responses following erroneous saccades to T1. To assess CD in schizophrenia, we examined the following on error trials: (1) frequency and latency of corrective saccades, and (2) mislocalization of the corrective (second) saccade in the direction predicted by a failure to use CD to account for the first eye movement. Consistent with disrupted CD, patients made fewer and slower error corrections. Importantly, the corrective saccade vector angle was biased in a manner consistent with disrupted CD. These results provide novel and clear evidence for dysfunctional CD in the oculomotor system in patients with schizophrenia. Based on neurophysiology work, these disturbances might have their basis in medial thalamus dysfunction.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT According to the World Health Organization, acute schizophrenia carries more disability weight than any other disease, but its etiology remains unknown. One promising theory of schizophrenia highlights alterations in a sense of self, in which self-generated thoughts or actions are attributed externally. Disruptions in corollary discharge (CD), motor signals sent to sensory areas that allow for the prediction of impending sensations, are proposed to underlie these symptoms. Direct physiological evidence, however, is limited. In nonhuman primates, inactivation of mediodorsal thalamic neurons disrupts CD associated with eye movements. Using the same task, we show similar impairments in schizophrenia patients, consistent with disrupted CD. These findings allow us to link clinical phenomenology to primate neurophysiology and interpret findings within a biological framework.

  • corollary discharge
  • double-step
  • efference copy
  • saccade
  • schizophrenia

Introduction

Altered sense of self is a defining feature of schizophrenia (Sass and Parnas, 2003; Park and Nasrallah, 2014). Self-disturbances manifest in, for example, delusions of being controlled by an outside force and anomalous source monitoring of internal speech resulting in auditory hallucinations (Frith, 1987). Such experiences have been hypothesized to be associated with abnormal corollary discharge (CD; Feinberg, 1978), the motor signals sent to sensory areas that allow for the prediction of future sensory states and the ability to distinguish self-generated from externally generated events (Wolpert and Ghahramani, 2000). Accordingly, disrupted CD could be one of the physiological underpinnings of the subjective loss of agency and self-other boundary in psychosis (Schneider, 1959).

Robust evidence for the central role of CD in internal monitoring and correction of movement plans has come from the saccadic double-step task in which two visual targets are flashed sequentially in darkness, and subjects are instructed to make a saccade to the locations of the first, and then the second target (Hallett and Lightstone, 1976; Becker and Jürgens, 1979). To shift gaze to the second target accurately, the subject must account for the first saccade from the fixation point without spatial references or visual feedback. In the double-step task, the measure of interest is spatial accuracy of the second saccade. If the participant fails to compensate for the first saccade due to disrupted CD, the second saccade vector deviates from the location of the second target (Fig. 1B). Healthy humans and nonhuman primates can accurately localize the second target, even when the interval between the first and second saccade is very brief. Further, subjects can also compensate for variations in amplitude of the first saccade (Collins et al., 2009; Joiner et al., 2010). That is, despite making a hypometric or hypermetric saccade to the first target, an accurate saccade to the second target can be executed. This task provides a robust translational bridge for understanding neurophysiological mechanisms of self disturbances in schizophrenia (Sommer and Wurtz, 2002, 2008; Murthy et al., 2007, 2009). To our knowledge, no published studies have investigated the role of CD in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia with the double-step paradigm.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

A, Modified double-step task. Arrows indicate the direction of the saccade. Trials began with the presentation of a central fixation spot. After the fixation spot disappeared, a target flashed at a noncentral location. Subjects were instructed to saccade to this location as quickly as possible. On double-step trials, a second target flashed at some variable delay following onset of the first target (target step delay). A target step was the cue for the subject to withhold a saccade to the first target and instead redirect gaze toward the second target. On most double-step error trials, a corrective saccade was made to the second target location. On single-step trials, a second target did not appear. Trial types were randomly interleaved. B, CD Schematic. Left, Schematic of not compensating for the change in eye position brought about by the first saccade. The gray arrow is the saccade vector from fixation to T2. The dotted arrow represents the saccade that would be made from T1 if the subject had not adjusted for change in position brought about by the first saccade (CD loss-saccade occurrence). Angle θ1 represents the bias in the second saccade vector if the subject had not compensated at all for the first eye movement. Any saccade that fell in the direction between the ideal saccade and saccade indicating no CD was assigned a positive value. Right, Schematic of not compensating for variability in amplitude of the first saccade. The gray arrow is the saccade vector from T1 to T2. The dotted arrow represents the saccade that would be made from the endpoint of the first saccade if the subject had not adjusted for error in the endpoint of the first saccade (CD loss-saccade amplitude). Angle θ2 represents the bias in the second saccade vector if the subject had not compensated at all for spatial error in the first saccade endpoint. Any saccade that fell in the direction between the ideal saccade and saccade indicating no CD was assigned a positive value.

Therefore, we used a modified double-step paradigm (Murthy et al., 2007), which allowed us additional measures of CD beyond mislocalization of the second saccade. In this modified task, we instructed participants to ignore the first target if the second appeared and look only at the second target. The instruction to redirect gaze rather than follow the targets modulates performance of this task (Ray et al., 2004). We manipulated task difficulty such that on approximately one-half of the double-step trials, subjects made rapid saccades to the two targets in succession, as in a typical double-step paradigm. Importantly, on the trials when the first saccade was an error and the second saccade was corrective, we could investigate the quality of CD to monitor actions in several ways. First, we measured the frequency of error detection and correction. Second, we measured the latency of corrective saccades. CD allows individuals to “know” or sense an impending error before movement initiation. Accordingly, a corrective movement can be executed at very short latencies following an error (Rabbitt, 1966). We expected disrupted CD to result in slower corrective reaction times (RTs). Finally, we asked whether patients with schizophrenia would fail to use CD to localize the second target. These data implicate functional pathology in very specific neural circuits that give rise to disrupted CD in schizophrenia.

Materials and Methods

Participants.

Demographic information is presented in Table 1. Sixteen individuals who met the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) criteria for schizophrenia (SZPs) were recruited from outpatient psychiatric facilities in Nashville, TN. Diagnoses were confirmed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (First et al., 1995). All patients were taking either atypical (olanzapine, n = 3; aripiprazole, n = 2; clozapine, n = 2; risperidone, n = 5; paliperidone, n = 1; quetiapine, n = 1; asenapine, n = 1) or typical (thiotixene, n = 1; haloperidol, n = 1) antipsychotic medication. Some patients were also using antidepressants (citalopram, n = 2; sertraline, n = 2; trazadone, n = 4), lithium (n = 2), and/or anticonvulsants (lamictal, n = 1; oxcarbazepine, n = 1). Chlorpromazine (CPZ) equivalent antipsychotic dosages were calculated for each patient (Woods, 2003). Eighteen healthy, unmedicated control subjects (HCs) without a personal and family history of DSM-IV Axis I disorders were recruited from the same community by advertisements.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Demographic characteristics of the patient and control groups

Clinical symptoms were assessed with the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (Overall and Gorham, 1962), the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (Andreasen, 1984), and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (Andreasen, 1983). Additionally, the Scale for Passivity Phenomena (Spence et al., 1997) was administered to assess current and lifetime passivity symptoms. Social and occupational functioning was assessed by the 79-item Social Functioning Scale (Birchwood et al., 1990), which has seven subscales: social engagement, interpersonal communication, frequency of daily living activities, competence of daily living activities, recreational activities, social activities, and occupational activity. The North American Adult Reading Test (Blair and Spreen, 1989) was used to estimate premorbid IQ. The Modified Edinburgh Handedness Inventory was used to assess handedness (Oldfield, 1971). All participants were screened to exclude substance use, neurological disorders, history of head injury, inability to fixate, and excessive sleepiness. All subjects had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Groups were matched for age, sex, and handedness. All subjects gave written informed consent approved by the Vanderbilt Institutional Review Board and were reimbursed.

Apparatus and stimuli.

Eye position was monitored using the EyeLink II eyetracker (SR Research,) at a sampling rate of 250 Hz with average gaze position error <0.5°, noise limited to <0.01° root mean square. Saccades were detected on-line using a velocity criterion (35°/s). Subjects were seated 57 cm from the computer monitor with their head in a chinrest. One aim of this experiment was to examine the integrity of CD signals by measuring the spatial accuracy of the corrective saccade to the final target on double-step trials (Fig. 1B). To eliminate the effect of visual reference cues on saccade localization, the task was performed in darkness by reducing the stimulus presentation computer monitor brightness to the minimum value and using a three-stop neutral density gel filter to block stray monitor light. This technique has been used in prior studies (Park et al., 2003). Subjects were instructed to notify the experimenter, who was seated outside the testing room, if they saw any stray monitor light that revealed the monitor outline as they began to dark-adapt; however, no subjects reported seeing any visual reference cues.

Design and procedure.

Subjects performed the saccadic double-step task (Fig. 1A). We used this task to explore both cognitive control of gaze as well as CD in the oculomotor system. Results from our analysis of cognitive control are reported by Thakkar et al. (2015). Sixty percent of the trials were single-step trials. These trials required subjects to fixate on the central fixation spot (white square subtending 0.5°) until it disappeared (after a random delay between 500 and 1000 ms) and a peripheral target (T1), subtending 1°, flashed for 94 ms at an alternate location. The first target could be presented at one of eight positions 12° equidistant from fixation. Subjects were instructed to look directly at the target as quickly as possible. The remaining 40% of trials were step trials. These trials were initially identical to the no-step trials, but after a variable delay (target step delay; TSD) following initial target presentation, a second target (T2) flashed for 94 ms at an alternate location. (If the TSD was <94 ms, T1 was only presented for the length of the TSD. At TSDs of 47 or 94 ms, T1 offset and T2 onset were simultaneous.) T1 and T2 were separated by at least 90° to minimize the number of saccades landing midway between T1 and T2, which occurs more frequently as the angle decreases (for review, see Van der Stigchel and Nijboer, 2011). We further constrained target locations such that T1 and T2 were never separated by 180° (i.e., could not be presented at opposite sides of fixation) as there would be no difference in the saccade vector angle when the subject did or did not use CD to accurately localize the second target. Thus, because targets were presented at one of eight equidistant locations on a circular array, T1 and T2 location could be separated by either 90° or 135°.

The target step instructed subjects to inhibit a saccade to the initial target and instead look toward the second target as quickly as possible. The first and second targets were different isoluminant colors (cyan and magenta, 2.06 cd/m2, presented on a black background), making it easier to distinguish target order at short TSDs. Color mapping was counterbalanced across subjects. Accuracy (the likelihood of initially directing gaze to the second target location) reduced with increasing TSDs. To manipulate task difficulty TSDs were adjusted trialwise using two independent, interleaved tracking procedures, 2-up/1-down (converging near 71% successful inhibition) and 1-up/2-down (converging near 29% successful inhibition). This procedure ensures accurate performance on ∼50% of the step trials. The initial TSD was 94 ms. If a particular step trial was part of the 2-up/1-down staircase, TSD increased by 47 ms if the previous two step trials that were part of that staircase were accurate (first saccade made to T2) and decreased by 47 ms if the previous step trial in that staircase was inaccurate (first saccade made to T1). Otherwise, the TSD was held constant. Likewise, for trials that were part of the 1-up/2-down staircase, TSD increased by 47 ms if the previous step trial was accurate, decreased by 47 ms if the two previous step trials were inaccurate, and was otherwise held constant. The testing session consisted of a practice block of 60 trials, and four experimental blocks of 120 trials each.

For the analysis of CD, we were interested in double-step error trials. These erroneous saccades to the first target are nearly always followed by corrective saccades to the final target. Accordingly, as indices of CD, we measured the incidence and speed of corrective saccades as well as the spatial accuracy of the second saccade, as one would in a standard double-step paradigm. To distinguish corrective saccades from saccades that return the eyes to the central fixation point, the following criteria were used to define corrective saccades: (1) the endpoint of the saccade is closer to T2 than the start of the saccade, (2) the endpoint of the saccade is closer to T2 than to the central fixation location, and (3) the direction of the saccade is closer to the direction necessary for gaze to land on T2 than the direction necessary for gaze to land on the central fixation. Saccadic RT on double-step error trials was calculated as the time between initial target onset and saccade initiation. Corrective saccade latency was measured as the interval between the initiation of the error saccade and corrective saccade. RT values were log-transformed, using the natural log (loge), to account for non-normality of data.

To examine the spatial accuracy of corrective saccades, we first tested for general directional inaccuracy of the second saccade vector (Joiner et al., 2010). On each trial in which a corrective saccade was produced, we calculated the actual direction of the second saccade and the ideal direction of the second saccade that would have brought the eye upon the second target, based on the endpoint of the initial saccade. For each subject, we plotted the ideal versus actual direction (in polar angle) of each corrective saccade, fit a linear regression model, and compared the slope and R2 values across groups. A failure to use CD to correctly localize T2 can be examined in two ways. First, subjects may have failed to take into account that gaze has moved from the fixation point when initiating the second saccade. To measure the degree to which subjects took into account the anticipated change in eye position following a saccade to T1, we measured the angle of deviation between the endpoint of the corrective saccade and the location of T2. Angular deviations in the direction that would be expected if the subject was not using CD to compensate for any change in eye position (i.e., as if making a saccade to T2 from fixation) were assigned a positive value (Fig. 1B, left). We refer to these positive deviations as CD loss-saccade occurrence.

Integrity of the CD mechanisms that support accurate saccades can be measured in a second way on double-step error trials. Saccades to T1 are often slightly hypometric or hypermetric. Both humans and nonhuman primates use CD to adjust for these inaccuracies in the saccade to T1 when making their saccade to T2. We calculated the degree to which subjects compensated for variations in amplitude of the first saccade by measuring the angle of deviation between the endpoint of the corrective saccade and T2. Angular deviations in the direction that would be expected if the subject were not compensating for spatial inaccuracy in the saccade to T1 (i.e., making a saccade to T2 from T1) were assigned a positive value (Fig. 1B, right). We refer to these positive deviations as CD loss-saccade amplitude.

Given the literature documenting asymmetries in spatial attention in schizophrenia, we also examined the influence of T1 hemifield on measures of CD integrity. For this analysis, we excluded trials in which the initial saccade was vertical. Analyses of RT and biases in the second saccade endpoint toward CD loss were conducted with the hemifield of T1 as a factor in the model.

Finally, to control for the possibility that putative group differences in the endpoint accuracy of the saccade to T2 are accounted for by differences in the dynamics of the initial saccade to T1, we calculated the amplitude of T1 and the degree to which the direction of the saccade to T1 was biased in the direction of T2. Saccades biased in the direction of T2 were assigned a positive value.

Statistical methods.

Fisher's exact tests, independent t tests, and repeated-measures ANOVAs were used where appropriate. Spearman rank-correlation coefficients were calculated between symptoms and task performance. All tests were two-tailed.

Results

Error correction rate

An independent t test was used to compare the percentage of corrected double-step error trials across groups. SZPs corrected fewer errors than HCs (HC: mean = 88.1%, SD = 13.4; SZP: mean = 78%, SD = 13.8; t(32) = 2.2, p = 0.04), suggesting impaired action monitoring.

Error correction speed

Cumulative RT distributions are presented in Figure 2. To examine corrective saccade latency compared with error saccade latency, as well as putative group differences in corrective saccade latency, a repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted on mean log-transformed RT, with response type (corrective, error) and angle between targets (90°, 135°) as within-subjects variables and diagnostic group as a between-subjects variable. We found no main effect of either group (F(1,32) = 2.2, p = 0.15) or response type (F(1,32) = 3.1, p = 0.09). Importantly, however, we found a significant group-by-response type interaction effect (F(1,32) = 4.2, p = 0.05). In HCs, corrective saccades were significantly faster than the initial error saccade (corrective: mean = 239 ms, SD = 41 ms; error: mean = 256 ms, SD = 36 ms; t(17) = 3.7, p = 0.002), but we found no difference between saccade types in SZPs (corrective: mean = 279 ms, SD = 53 ms; error: mean = 269 ms, SD = 88 ms; t(15) = 0.2, p = 0.81). Further, we found no difference in error saccade latency between groups (t(32) < 0.1, p > 0.99), but corrective saccades were significantly slower in SZPs than HCs (t(32) = 2.2, p = 0.04). We found no effect of angle of separation between targets. These data suggest that SZPs fail to use CD to plan a corrective movement in anticipation of making an error.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Error and corrective saccade RTs. RTs were transformed using the natural log (loge). Vincentized reaction time distributions for error (dotted lines) and corrective saccades (solid lines) on double-step trials. HCs are depicted in gray and SZPs are depicted in orange. Error saccades were measured as the time between T1 onset and the onset of the first saccade. Corrective saccades were measured as the time between the onset of the error saccade and the onset of the corrective saccade. For each participant, RTs for error and corrective saccades were binned into deciles. Decile means were averaged across subjects to create the group-averaged vincentized RT distributions. Inset, Mean RTs for corrective (solid bars) and error (striped bars) saccades are presented.

Corrective saccade spatial accuracy

Eye position traces from two representative participants for all double-step trials in which a saccade to T1 was followed by a saccade to T2 are presented in Figure 3.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Eye position traces for a representative HC (A) and SZP (B). The eye position between the initiation of the first saccade toward T1 (empty circle) and the termination of the second saccade toward T2 (filled circle) are depicted for each trial in which successive saccades to T1 and T2 were produced. Plots are separated for 90° (left) and 135° (right) T1–T2 angles. Saccade vectors to different target locations were normalized into the same space.

Ideal versus observed saccade direction

For each subject, the relationship between the direction of the ideal saccade to the second target compared with the observed saccade direction was quantified by fitting a linear model (Fig. 4). Goodness of fit (R2) and slope values were submitted to separate repeated-measures ANOVAs with angle between T1 and T2 entered as within-subject variables and group entered as a between-subjects variable. We found no effect of group (F(1,32) = 0.3, p = 0.60), angle between targets (F(1,32) = 2.9, p = 0.10), or group-by-separation angle interaction (F(1,32) = 0.5, p = 0.48) on slope. We found, however, a significant effect of group (F(1,32) = 4.1, p = 0.05) and marginal effect of angle (F(1,32) = 3.2, p = 0.08) on R2 values. Importantly, we found also a significant group-by-separation angle effect (F(1,32) = 8.8, p = 0.006). R2 values (noted in Fig. 4) were lower in SZPs than HCs when targets were separated by 90° (t(32) = 2.6, p = 0.01), but not 135° (t(32) = 0.2, p = 0.81). These data indicate more variability in degree to which SZPs use CD to accurately localize the second target.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Ideal versus actual saccade vector. For each participant, regression lines were fit to actual corrective saccade vector angle plotted as a function of ideal saccade vector angle when targets were separated by 90° (left) and 135° (right) for HCs (gray) and SZPs (orange). Individual regression lines are depicted. The black line represents the line of unity. Accompanying the regression lines is a histogram of the signed differences between the ideal and actual saccade direction, separately for controls and patients.

CD influence on corrective saccade endpoint

Figure 5 shows single trial examples of biases in the second saccade endpoint toward not compensating for the first saccade (CD loss-saccade occurrence; Fig. 5A) and not compensating hypo- or hypermetricity of the first saccade (CD loss-saccade amplitude; Fig. 5B) in one SZP. Figure 6 quantifies the magnitude of CD guiding corrective saccades. We submitted both CD loss-saccade occurrence and CD loss-saccade amplitude to two separate repeated-measures ANOVAs with angle of separation between T1 and T2 entered as a within-subjects variable and group entered as a between-subjects variable. Significance was evaluated at a Bonferroni-corrected α-level of 0.025 (0.05/2), to correct for multiple testing.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Single trial examples. Eye movement samples (orange dots) from single trials in one schizophrenia patient. A, Example of a trial in which the subject almost completely fails to compensate for the initial saccade to T1 when making a saccade to T2 (i.e., nearly total CD loss-saccade occurrence). B, Example of a trial in which the subject almost completely fails to compensate for hypometricity of the saccade to T1 when making a saccade to T2 (i.e., nearly total CD loss-saccade amplitude).

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Loss of CD. A, Mean CD loss-saccade occurrence (±SE) and (B) mean CD loss-saccade amplitude (±SE) separated by target separation angle for HCs (gray) and SZPs (orange). n.s., p > 0.05; *p < 0.05.

For CD loss-saccade occurrence (Fig. 6A), we found a significant effect of target separation (F(1,32) = 0.02). Even though the main effect of group did not reach significance after Bonferroni correction (F(1,32) = 4.4, p = 0.04), we found a significant group-by-target separation interaction (F(1,32) = 9.3, p = 0.004). Planned independent t tests indicated that SZPs showed less CD compensation than HCs when targets were separated by 90° (HC: mean = 0.5°, SD = 2.1°; SZP: mean = 3.1°, SD = 2.6°; t(32) = 3.1, p = 0.004) but not 135° (t(32) = 0.4, p = 0.67).

To quantify the degree to which subjects fully compensated for the occurrence of the first saccade, for each trial we calculated the angle between the vector of the ideal saccade and the vector resulting from no CD (Fig. 1B, θ1). We calculated percentage compensation as the angle between the actual saccade vector and the vector resulting from no CD divided by θ1; 100% compensation indicates complete CD producing an accurate saccade to T2, and 0% compensation indicates no CD compensating for the first saccade. When targets were separated by 90°, we found nearly complete compensation in HCs (mean = 99.8%, SD = 7.4%), but not in SZPs (mean = 91.6%, SD = 8.8%). When targets were separated by 135°, CD compensation was more similar between HCs (mean = 95.8%, SD = 10.0%) and SZPs (99.9%, SD = 10.4%). When targets were separated by 90°, SZPs compensated ∼8% less than HCs for the occurrence of the first saccade.

The robust group-by-target separation interaction was unexpected. Given the greater distance between targets separated by 135°, we tested whether schizophrenia patients might have adjusted saccade trajectory midflight to direct gaze closer to T2. We measured the initial saccade trajectory between fixation and instantaneous eye position 12 ms following saccade initiation (Van der Stigchel et al., 2006). Although the initial saccade trajectory reflected less CD compensation, the initial trajectory still showed a significant group-by-angle interaction effect (F(1,32) = 5.2, p = 0.03). That is, even at initiation of the corrective saccade, CD had reduced influence when targets were separated by 90° compared to 135° in SZPs.

For CD loss-saccade amplitude (Fig. 6B) we found no significant effect of group (F(1,32) = 0.05, p = 0.82), separation angle (F(1,32) = 0.59, p = 0.44), or group-by-separation angle interaction (F(1,32) = 2.2, p = 0.15). Thus, SZPs showed a loss of CD for making any saccade toward T1, but did not show evidence of a loss of CD for not having made the ideal saccade to T1.

Spatial asymmetries in CD

To examine saccade latencies as a function of T1 hemifield, a repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted on log-transformed RTs, with hemifield (left, right), response type (corrective, error) and angle between targets (90°, 135°) entered as within-subjects as factors and group entered as a between-subjects factor. We found no significant effect of saccade direction, nor any interaction effects involving saccade direction.

We also examined spatial accuracy of the corrective saccade endpoint as a function of hemifield. Both CD loss-saccade occurrence and CD loss-saccade amplitude were submitted to a repeated-measures ANOVA with initial saccade direction and angle between targets entered as within-subjects factors, and group entered as a between-subjects factor. Significance of both of these ANOVAs was evaluated at a Bonferroni-corrected α-level of 0.025 (0.05/2), to correct for multiple testing. For CD loss-saccade occurrence (Fig. 7A), we found no main effect of saccade direction, and the group-by-saccade direction interaction effect did not meet significance after correcting for multiple testing (F(1,32) = 4.1, p = 0.05). We were intrigued, however, by this trend toward a significant interaction and performed planned between-group comparisons. In SZPs, we found a greater bias in second saccade endpoint toward no influence of CD to compensate for the first saccade relative to controls when the first saccade was rightward (HC: mean = −0.7°, SD = 2.3°; SZP: mean = 2.1°, SD = 3.0°; t(32) = 3.1, p = 0.004) but not leftward (t(32) = 0.4, p = 0.69).

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

Loss of CD as a function of target hemifield presentation. A, Mean CD loss-saccade occurrence (±SE) and (B) mean CD loss-saccade amplitude (±SE) separated by visual hemifield of T1 for HCs (gray) and SZPs (orange). n.s., p > 0.05; *p < 0.05.

For CD loss-saccade amplitude (Fig. 7B), we found a significant group-by-saccade direction interaction effect (F(1,32) = 6.8, p = 0.01). In SZP, bias in second saccade endpoint was greater when T1 was in the left versus right hemifield (left: mean = 1.4°, SD = 3.5°; right: mean = −0.5°, SD = 3.3°; t(15) = 2.3, p = 0.04). This effect was not present in HCs (t(17) = 0.8, p = 0.4). However, there were no significant group differences in bias for either T1 hemifield (right: t(32) = 1.8, p = 0.09; left: t(32) = 0.9, p = 0.36).

Control analyses

To ensure that group differences in the bias of the corrective saccade endpoint in the direction predicted by loss of CD were not due to group differences in the dynamics of the saccade to T1, we compared the degree to which the first saccade was biased toward T2 (Fig. 8A) and the first saccade amplitude (Fig. 8B) across groups. Repeated-measures ANOVAs were conducted on each of these variables, including separation angle between T1 and T2 as within-subjects variables and group as a between-subjects variable. For saccade amplitude, we found no effect of group (F(1,32) = 1.4, p = 0.24), separation angle (F(1,32)<0.1, p > 0.99), or group-by-separation angle interaction (F(1,32) = 0.5, p = 0.49). For directional error in the first saccade, we found a significant effect of separation angle (F(1,32) = 5.9, p < 0.0001). Saccades to T1 were more biased in the direction of T2 when targets were separated by 90° (mean = 9.9°, SD = 5.8°) versus 135° (mean = 6.0°, SD = 5.8°). Importantly, however, we found no effect of group (F(1,32) = 0.1, p = 0.75), or group-by-separation angle interaction (F(1,32) = 1.9, p = 0.18).

Figure 8.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 8.

Dynamics of first saccade on error double-step trials, separated by angle of target separation. A, Mean bias (±SE) in the first saccade vector angle in the direction of T2. B, Mean amplitude (±SE) of the first saccade in degrees of visual angle (right) for HCs (gray) and SZPs (orange). n.s., p > 0.05.

Symptom, social functioning, and medication correlations

Correlations were performed between measures of symptom severity, social functioning, and standardized medication dose and indices of CD: percentage corrected responses, log-transformed corrective saccade RT, the R2 and slope values for the linear function plotting ideal versus observed second saccade direction, CD loss-saccade occurrence, and CD loss-saccade amplitude. Correlations with the latter two measures were performed separately for the 90° and 135° target configurations. No significant correlations were observed between symptom severity and social functioning and any of double-step task performance measures. We found a modest positive correlation between CPZ dose and R2 value of the ideal-actual saccade direction function (r = 0.6, p = 0.03), but only in the 135° target configuration, in which we did not observe any group differences.

Discussion

Using a double-step saccade task, we report multiple lines of evidence demonstrating disrupted saccadic CD in schizophrenia. Most crucially, when schizophrenia patients make saccades in rapid succession, they fail to compensate for the displacement of the first saccade. This systematic mislocalization has never been shown before in schizophrenia and provides clear evidence of disrupted oculomotor CD.

This mislocalization of the second target cannot be explained by differences in the dynamics of the saccade to the first target. Schizophrenia patients were unimpaired in the amplitude of the first saccade or in the degree to which the first saccade was biased toward the second target.

Likewise, this mislocalization is unlikely to be due only to the documented working memory deficits in schizophrenia (Park and Holzman, 1992) because the task included no delay period. Also, unlike symmetrically greater variability of saccade endpoints around the second target predicted by impaired working memory (White et al., 1994), we saw a systematic bias in the mislocalization of T2 in the direction predicted by an impaired CD.

Interestingly, mislocalization of T2 was highly dependent on the arrangement of T1 and T2. Loss of CD was clearer when targets were separated by 90° than 135°. We are unaware of any studies that have investigated the influence of angle or distance between targets on second saccade accuracy in such tasks. Thus, the basis of this target separation effect remains unclear. In previous double-step experiments, targets were separated by <90° or positioned on the horizontal meridian. We considered the hypothesis that greater distance between T1 and T2 when targets were separated by 135° allowed subjects to correct midflight an initially errant saccade trajectory (Minken et al., 1993); however, an effect of target separation was present even in the initial saccade trajectory. The difference in the direction of the CD-compensated saccade from T1 to T2 and the CD-uncompensated saccade was greater when targets were separated by 90° than 135° (45° vs 22.5°), so measurements may have been more sensitive to deviations in the second saccade vector with the 90° target configuration.

We also observed a statistical trend toward spatial asymmetry of CD, in schizophrenia. Patients showed attenuated influence of CD when T1 was in the right visual field. Right visual hemifield abnormalities have been reported for response latencies (Gur, 1978), spatial attention (Posner et al., 1988), and memory-guided saccades (Park, 1999). Interestingly, among right-handed schizophrenia patients, visual hallucinations are most often seen in the right visual field (Bracha et al., 1985).

We observed other evidence for abnormal CD in schizophrenia. On double-step trials, subjects were instructed to look immediately at the second target. Trials in which subjects first looked at T1 were errors. CD is the purported mechanism for internally monitoring motor plans to predict an upcoming erroneous response and plan corrections in parallel with the error response (Coles et al., 1985; Ray et al., 2004; Murthy et al., 2007; Sharika et al., 2008). Consistent with this notion, corrective responses in healthy controls were faster than errors. In patients, however, we found no difference between errors and corrective response latencies, suggesting reduced influence of CD. Thus, despite equal error saccade latencies across groups, patients were slower at correcting these errors. Providing further evidence for disrupted CD, schizophrenia patients corrected fewer errors.

Findings regarding post-error behavioral adjustments in schizophrenia are mixed; both intact (Kopp and Rist, 1994, 1999; Brownstein et al., 2003; Morris et al., 2006; Polli et al., 2006, 2008; Reuter et al., 2006) and impaired adjustments during cognitive tasks have been reported (Malenka et al., 1982, 1986; Frith and Done, 1989; McDowell and Clementz, 1997; Turken et al., 2003; Waters et al., 2009). One explanation for our findings is that we removed all visual reference cues, thus placing particularly strong demands on internal monitoring via CD. Thus, in contrast with previous studies, subjects could not rely on sensory information to shift gaze in space.

Combined, these findings are consistent with past reports of impaired generation, transmission, or use of CD in schizophrenia to modulate visual (Thaker et al., 1996; Lindner et al., 2005; Spering et al., 2013; Richard et al., 2014), auditory (Ford et al., 2001a,b,c, 2004; Ford and Mathalon, 2004), and tactile (Blakemore et al., 2000) information processing.

The significance of our study is the precision with which CD can be mapped to specific circuits in the primate brain. Accordingly, these findings offer a direct translational link between neurophysiological data in animals and behavioral data in patient populations. Strong evidence demonstrates that neurons in the mediodorsal (MD) thalamus convey CD signals generated by movement neurons in the superior colliculus (SC) to the frontal eye fields (FEFs; Sommer and Wurtz, 2002, 2004, 2008). Following inactivation of MD thalamus, nonhuman primates performing a double-step task showed reduced influence of CD through systematic mislocalization of the second target, paralleling our observation in schizophrenia patients. Double-step task performance is also impaired in thalamic lesion patients (Gaymard et al., 1994; Bellebaum et al., 2005; Ostendorf et al., 2010). Thus, functional alterations in this SC-MD-FEF pathway could result in abnormal incidence, latency, or precision of saccadic CD signals in schizophrenia patients. One very important property of signals sent to the thalamus either from cortex or lower brain regions is that they are most often also sent to motor centers (Guillery and Sherman, 2002). Accordingly, thalamocortical relays have been conceptualized as “monitoring” ongoing motor commands. Although speculative, the thalamus might be a relay hub for transmitting CD signals across the cortex, and dysfunctions in modality-specific thalamocortical pathways might give rise to the heterogeneity of psychotic experiences.

Given the role of the thalamus in relaying information linking multiple brain regions, altered thalamic function would contribute to abnormal functional connectivity (Andreasen et al., 1998; Stephan et al., 2006). Interestingly, there are recent reports of abnormal functional connectivity of the MD thalamus in schizophrenia that are related to symptoms theoretically linked to abnormal CD. Woodward et al. (2012) observed hypoconnectivity between MD thalamus and prefrontal cortex at resting state in schizophrenia. Attesting to the clinical relevance of such corticothalamic connectivity, decreased functional connectivity has been shown between auditory cortex and MD thalamus in individuals that experience auditory hallucinations (Shinn et al., 2013), individuals with psychotic bipolar disorder show greater alterations in functional connections between MD thalamus and cortex than those with nonpsychotic bipolar disorder, (Anticevic et al., 2014), and altered microstructural integrity of the thalamus has been related to alien control delusions in schizophrenia patients (Sim et al., 2009). Thus, abnormal thalamocortical connections that disrupt CD may contribute to blurred self–other boundaries and vulnerability to psychosis.

Neurophysiological evidence also points to the importance of parietal cortex for the spatial updating needed for double-step task performance. Visually responsive neurons in lateral intraparietal sulcus predictively update their receptive field properties based on an impending saccade (for review, see Hall and Colby, 2011), modulated by CD signals from MD thalamus (Wurtz, 2008). Parietal lesions disturb the second saccade trajectory during the double-step task (Duhamel et al., 1992; Heide et al., 1995; Pisella et al., 2011), which suggests the important role of the parietal visual neurons in updating their receptive properties based on CD signals.

These results have caveats. First, the influence of antipsychotic medication on CD is unclear; however, it is unlikely that CD disruptions were caused by medication; none of the CD measures varied with medication dose. Moreover, disrupted CD is argued to engender the psychotic symptoms that antipsychotics attenuate (Feinberg, 1978; Frith, 1992). Thus, antipsychotics, if anything, should improve CD. This point, however, brings us to our second limitation. Although we conjecture a link between CD disturbances and psychotic symptoms, we observed no correlations between symptoms and CD measures. This is not unprecedented (Ford and Mathalon, 2012; Ford et al., 2012). The absence of symptom correlations could indicate that CD disturbances reflect symptom vulnerability, rather than expression. Alternatively, symptoms interviews may be inadequate; they lack key questions about psychotic experiences that are closely aligned to CD abnormalities, such as body aberrations (Chapman et al., 1978; Thakkar et al., 2011). Finally, individuals with schizophrenia did not show evidence for a complete loss of CD. When targets were separated by 90°, patients showed, on average, an 8% loss of CD compared with HCs. Although the magnitude of this effect is modest, primates with pharmacological MD thalamus inactivation performing the double-step task also showed only a partial loss of CD (19%; Sommer and Wurtz, 2004). To explain these partial deficits, Sommer and Wurtz (2004) highlight the multiple potential thalamic and nonthalamic routes for CD signals to reach cortex and the possible compensatory use of proprioception to guide saccadic eye movements.

In conclusion, using the double-step saccade task used for single-cell neurophysiological studies of CD transmission, we found strong evidence for abnormal CD in patients with schizophrenia. Our findings provide a direct translational bridge to understanding the biological underpinnings of psychotic experiences in schizophrenia and highlight the role of CD beyond basic sensorimotor processing.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported in part by F31-MH085405-01 (K.N.T.), Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research Rubicon Grant (K.N.T.), NARSAD (S.P.), R01-MH073028 (S.P.), R01-MH70560 (S.H.), P30-HD015052 (S.P., J.D.S., K.N.T.), P30-EY08126 (J.D.S.), R01-MH055806 (J.D.S.), and the E. Bronson Ingram Chair in Neuroscience (J.D.S.). We thank Lindsay Gilling McIntosh and Heathman Nichols for their assistance with clinical interviews and subject recruitment.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to either of the following: Dr Katharine Thakkar, Department of Psychiatry, University Medical Center Utrecht, Heidelberglaan 100, 3584 CX Utrecht, The Netherlands, k.n.thakkar{at}umcutrecht.nl; or Dr Sohee Park, Wilson Hall 213, Department of Psychology, 111 21st Avenue South, Nashville, TN 37240, sohee.park{at}vanderbilt.edu

References

  1. ↵
    1. Andreasen NC
    (1983) The scale for the assessment of negative symptoms (SANS) (University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA).
  2. ↵
    1. Andreasen NC
    (1984) The scale for the assessment of positive symptoms (SAPS) (University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA).
  3. ↵
    1. Andreasen NC,
    2. Paradiso S,
    3. O'Leary DS
    (1998) “Cognitive dysmetria” as an integrative theory of schizophrenia: a dysfunction in cortical-subcortical-cerebellar circuitry? Schizophr Bull 24:203–218, doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033321, pmid:9613621.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Anticevic A,
    2. Yang G,
    3. Savic A,
    4. Murray JD,
    5. Cole MW,
    6. Repovs G,
    7. Pearlson GD,
    8. Glahn DC
    (2014) Mediodorsal and visual thalamic connectivity differ in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder with and without psychosis history. Schizophr Bull 40:1227–1243, doi:10.1093/schbul/sbu100, pmid:25031221.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    1. Becker W,
    2. Jürgens R
    (1979) An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli. Vision Res 19:967–983, doi:10.1016/0042-6989(79)90222-0, pmid:532123.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Bellebaum C,
    2. Daum I,
    3. Koch B,
    4. Schwarz M,
    5. Hoffmann KP
    (2005) The role of the human thalamus in processing corollary discharge. Brain 128:1139–1154, doi:10.1093/brain/awh474, pmid:15758033.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    1. Birchwood M,
    2. Smith J,
    3. Cochrane R,
    4. Wetton S,
    5. Copestake S
    (1990) The social functioning scale: the development and validation of a new scale of social adjustment for use in family intervention programmes with schizophrenic patients. Br J Psychiatry 157:853–859, doi:10.1192/bjp.157.6.853, pmid:2289094.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    1. Blair JR,
    2. Spreen O
    (1989) Predicting premorbid IQ: a revision of the national adult reading test. Clin Neuropsychol 3:129–136, doi:10.1080/13854048908403285.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  9. ↵
    1. Blakemore SJ,
    2. Wolpert D,
    3. Frith C
    (2000) Why can't you tickle yourself? Neuroreport 11:R11–R16, doi:10.1097/00001756-200008030-00002, pmid:10943682.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Bracha HS,
    2. Cabrera FJ Jr.,
    3. Karson CN,
    4. Bigelow LB
    (1985) Lateralization of visual hallucinations in chronic schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 20:1132–1136, doi:10.1016/0006-3223(85)90013-7, pmid:4041515.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Brownstein J,
    2. Krastoshevsky O,
    3. McCollum C,
    4. Kundamal S,
    5. Matthysse S,
    6. Holzman PS,
    7. Mendell NR,
    8. Levy DL
    (2003) Antisaccade performance is abnormal in schizophrenia patients but not in their biological relatives. Schizophr Res 63:13–25, doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(02)00438-3, pmid:12892854.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Chapman LJ,
    2. Chapman JP,
    3. Raulin ML
    (1978) Body-image aberration in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol 87:399–407, doi:10.1037/0021-843X.87.4.399, pmid:681612.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Coles MG,
    2. Gratton G,
    3. Bashore TR,
    4. Eriksen CW,
    5. Donchin E
    (1985) A psychophysiological investigation of the continuous flow model of human information processing. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 11:529–553, doi:10.1037/0096-1523.11.5.529, pmid:2932529.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Collins T,
    2. Rolfs M,
    3. Deubel H,
    4. Cavanagh P
    (2009) Post-saccadic location judgments reveal remapping of saccade targets to non-foveal locations. J Vis 9(5):29, 1–9, doi:10.1167/9.5.29, pmid:19757907.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Duhamel JR,
    2. Goldberg ME,
    3. Fitzgibbon EJ,
    4. Sirigu A,
    5. Grafman J
    (1992) Saccadic dysmetria in a patient with a right frontoparietal lesion. Brain 115:1387–1402, doi:10.1093/brain/115.5.1387, pmid:1422794.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. ↵
    1. Feinberg I
    (1978) Efference copy and corollary discharge: implications for thinking and its disorders. Schizophr Bull 4:636–640, doi:10.1093/schbul/4.4.636, pmid:734369.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    1. First MB,
    2. Spitzer RL,
    3. Gibbon M,
    4. Williams JBW
    (1995) Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV Axis I disorders (Biometrics Research, New York).
  18. ↵
    1. Ford JM,
    2. Mathalon DH
    (2004) Electrophysiological evidence of corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia during talking and thinking. J Psychiatr Res 38:37–46, doi:10.1016/S0022-3956(03)00095-5, pmid:14690769.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Ford JM,
    2. Mathalon DH
    (2012) Anticipating the future: automatic prediction failures in schizophrenia. Int J Psychophysiol 83:232–239, doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.09.004, pmid:21959054.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Ford JM,
    2. Mathalon DH,
    3. Heinks T,
    4. Kalba S,
    5. Faustman WO,
    6. Roth WT
    (2001a) Neurophysiological evidence of corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat 158:2069–2071, doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.158.12.2069, pmid:11729029.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Ford JM,
    2. Mathalon DH,
    3. Kalba S,
    4. Whitfield S,
    5. Faustman WO,
    6. Roth WT
    (2001b) Cortical responsiveness during inner speech in schizophrenia: an event-related potential study. Am J Psychiat 158:1914–1916, doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1914, pmid:11691701.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Ford JM,
    2. Mathalon DH,
    3. Kalba S,
    4. Whitfield S,
    5. Faustman WO,
    6. Roth WT
    (2001c) Cortical responsiveness during talking and listening in schizophrenia: an event-related brain potential study. Biol Psychiatry 50:540–549, doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01166-0, pmid:11600107.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Ford JM,
    2. Gray M,
    3. Whitfield SL,
    4. Turken AU,
    5. Glover G,
    6. Faustman WO,
    7. Mathalon DH
    (2004) Acquiring and inhibiting prepotent responses in schizophrenia: event-related brain potentials and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry 61:119–129, doi:10.1001/archpsyc.61.2.119, pmid:14757588.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Ford JM,
    2. Dierks T,
    3. Fisher DJ,
    4. Herrmann CS,
    5. Hubl D,
    6. Kindler J,
    7. Koenig T,
    8. Mathalon DH,
    9. Spencer KM,
    10. Strik W,
    11. van Lutterveld R
    (2012) Neurophysiological studies of auditory verbal hallucinations. Schizophr Bull 38:715–723, doi:10.1093/schbul/sbs009, pmid:22368236.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    1. Frith C
    (1992) The cognitive neuropsychology of schizophrenia (Lawrence Erlbaum, Hove, UK).
  26. ↵
    1. Frith CD
    (1987) The positive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia reflect impairments in the perception and initiation of action. Psychol Med 17:631–648, doi:10.1017/S0033291700025873, pmid:3628624.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Frith CD,
    2. Done DJ
    (1989) Experiences of alien control in schizophrenia reflect a disorder in the central monitoring of action. Psychol Med 19:359–363, doi:10.1017/S003329170001240X, pmid:2762440.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Gaymard B,
    2. Pierrot-Deseilligny C,
    3. Rivaud S
    (1994) Impairment of extraretinal eye position signals after central thalamic lesions. Exp Brain Res 102:1–9, pmid:7895785.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Guillery RW,
    2. Sherman SM
    (2002) The thalamus as a monitor of motor outputs. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 357:1809–1821, doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1171, pmid:12626014.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  30. ↵
    1. Gur RE
    (1978) Left hemisphere dysfunction and left hemisphere overactivation in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol 87:226–238, doi:10.1037/0021-843X.87.2.226, pmid:649861.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Hall NJ,
    2. Colby CL
    (2011) Remapping for visual stability. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 366:528–539, doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0248, pmid:21242141.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    1. Hallett PE,
    2. Lightstone AD
    (1976) Saccadic eye movements to flashed targets. Vision Res 16:107–114, doi:10.1016/0042-6989(76)90084-5, pmid:1258384.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Heide W,
    2. Blankenburg M,
    3. Zimmermann E,
    4. Kömpf D
    (1995) Cortical control of double-step saccades: implications for spatial orientation. Ann Neurol 38:739–748, doi:10.1002/ana.410380508, pmid:7486865.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Joiner WM,
    2. FitzGibbon EJ,
    3. Wurtz RH
    (2010) Amplitudes and directions of individual saccades can be adjusted by corollary discharge. J Vis 10(2):22, 1–12, doi:10.1167/10.2.22, pmid:20462323.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. ↵
    1. Kopp B,
    2. Rist F
    (1994) Error-correcting behavior in schizophrenic patients. Schizophr Res 13:11–22, doi:10.1016/0920-9964(94)90056-6, pmid:7947412.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Kopp B,
    2. Rist F
    (1999) An event-related brain potential substrate of disturbed response monitoring in paranoid schizophrenic patients. J Abnorm Psychol 108:337–346, doi:10.1037/0021-843X.108.2.337, pmid:10369044.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Lindner A,
    2. Thier P,
    3. Kircher TT,
    4. Haarmeier T,
    5. Leube DT
    (2005) Disorders of agency in schizophrenia correlate with an inability to compensate for the sensory consequences of actions. Curr Biol 15:1119–1124, doi:10.1016/j.cub.2005.05.049, pmid:15964277.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Malenka RC,
    2. Angel RW,
    3. Hampton B,
    4. Berger PA
    (1982) Impaired central error-correcting behavior in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 39:101–107, doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1982.04290010073013, pmid:6119966.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Malenka RC,
    2. Angel RW,
    3. Thiemann S,
    4. Weitz CJ,
    5. Berger PA
    (1986) Central error-correcting behavior in schizophrenia and depression. Biol Psychiatry 21:263–273, doi:10.1016/0006-3223(86)90047-8, pmid:3947708.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. McDowell JE,
    2. Clementz BA
    (1997) The effect of fixation condition manipulations on antisaccade performance in schizophrenia: studies of diagnostic specificity. Exp Brain Res 115:333–344, doi:10.1007/PL00005702, pmid:9224861.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Minken AW,
    2. Van Opstal AJ,
    3. Van Gisbergen JA
    (1993) Three-dimensional analysis of strongly curved saccades elicited by double-step stimuli. Exp Brain Res 93:521–533, doi:10.1007/BF00229367, pmid:8519341.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Morris SE,
    2. Yee CM,
    3. Nuechterlein KH
    (2006) Electrophysiological analysis of error monitoring in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol 115:239–250, doi:10.1037/0021-843X.115.2.239, pmid:16737389.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Murthy A,
    2. Ray S,
    3. Shorter SM,
    4. Priddy EG,
    5. Schall JD,
    6. Thompson KG
    (2007) Frontal eye field contributions to rapid corrective saccades. J Neurophysiol 97:1457–1469, doi:10.1152/jn.00433.2006, pmid:17135479.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  44. ↵
    1. Murthy A,
    2. Ray S,
    3. Shorter SM,
    4. Schall JD,
    5. Thompson KG
    (2009) Neural control of visual search by frontal eye field: effects of unexpected target displacement on visual selection and saccade preparation. J Neurophysiol 101:2485–2506, doi:10.1152/jn.90824.2008, pmid:19261711.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  45. ↵
    1. Oldfield RC
    (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9:97–113, doi:10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4, pmid:5146491.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Ostendorf F,
    2. Liebermann D,
    3. Ploner CJ
    (2010) Human thalamus contributes to perceptual stability across eye movements. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107:1229–1234, doi:10.1073/pnas.0910742107, pmid:20080657.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  47. ↵
    1. Overall JE,
    2. Gorham DR
    (1962) The brief psychiatric rating scale. Psychol Rep 10:799–812, doi:10.2466/pr0.1962.10.3.799.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  48. ↵
    1. Park J,
    2. Schlag-Rey M,
    3. Schlag J
    (2003) Spatial localization precedes temporal determination in visual perception. Vision Res 43:1667–1674, doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(03)00217-7, pmid:12798148.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Park S
    (1999) Hemispheric asymmetry of spatial working memory deficit in schizophrenia. Int J Psychophysiol 34:313–322, doi:10.1016/S0167-8760(99)00088-4, pmid:10610055.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Park S,
    2. Holzman PS
    (1992) Schizophrenics show spatial working memory deficits. Arch Gen Psychiatry 49:975–982, doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820120063009, pmid:1449384.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Park S,
    2. Nasrallah HA
    (2014) The varieties of anomalous self experiences in schizophrenia: splitting of the mind at a crossroad. Schizophr Res 152:1–4, doi:10.1016/j.schres.2013.11.036, pmid:24332407.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Pisella L,
    2. Alahyane N,
    3. Blangero A,
    4. Thery F,
    5. Blanc S,
    6. Pelisson D
    (2011) Right-hemisphere dominance for visual remapping in humans. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 366:572–585, doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0258, pmid:21242144.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  53. ↵
    1. Polli FE,
    2. Barton JJ,
    3. Vangel M,
    4. Goff DC,
    5. Iguchi L,
    6. Manoach DS
    (2006) Schizophrenia patients show intact immediate error-related performance adjustments on an antisaccade task. Schizophr Res 82:191–201, doi:10.1016/j.schres.2005.10.003, pmid:16448804.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Polli FE,
    2. Barton JJ,
    3. Thakkar KN,
    4. Greve DN,
    5. Goff DC,
    6. Rauch SL,
    7. Manoach DS
    (2008) Reduced error-related activation in two anterior cingulate circuits is related to impaired performance in schizophrenia. Brain 131:971–986, doi:10.1093/brain/awm307, pmid:18158315.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  55. ↵
    1. Posner MI,
    2. Early TS,
    3. Reiman E,
    4. Pardo PJ,
    5. Dhawan M
    (1988) Asymmetries in hemispheric control of attention in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 45:814–821, doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1988.01800330038004, pmid:3415424.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Rabbitt PM
    (1966) Error correction time without external error signals. Nature 212:438, doi:10.1038/212438a0, pmid:5970176.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. Ray S,
    2. Schall JD,
    3. Murthy A
    (2004) Programming of double-step saccade sequences: modulation by cognitive control. Vision Res 44:2707–2718, doi:10.1016/j.visres.2004.05.029, pmid:15358065.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Reuter B,
    2. Herzog E,
    3. Kathmann N
    (2006) Antisaccade performance of schizophrenia patients: evidence of reduced task-set activation and impaired error detection. J Psychiatr Res 40:122–130, doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2005.02.007, pmid:16459170.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Richard A,
    2. Churan J,
    3. Whitford V,
    4. O'Driscoll GA,
    5. Titone D,
    6. Pack CC
    (2014) Perisaccadic perception of visual space in people with schizophrenia. J Neurosci 34:4760–4765, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4744-13.2014, pmid:24695696.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  60. ↵
    1. Sass LA,
    2. Parnas J
    (2003) Schizophrenia, consciousness, and the self. Schizophr Bull 29:427–444, doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007017, pmid:14609238.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  61. ↵
    1. Schneider K
    (1959) Clinical psychopathology (Grune and Stratton, New York).
  62. ↵
    1. Sharika KM,
    2. Ramakrishnan A,
    3. Murthy A
    (2008) Control of predictive error correction during a saccadic double-step task. J Neurophysiol 100:2757–2770, doi:10.1152/jn.90238.2008, pmid:18815349.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  63. ↵
    1. Shinn AK,
    2. Baker JT,
    3. Cohen BM,
    4. Ongür D
    (2013) Functional connectivity of left Heschl's gyrus in vulnerability to auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 143:260–268, doi:10.1016/j.schres.2012.11.037, pmid:23287311.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Sim K,
    2. Yang GL,
    3. Loh D,
    4. Poon LY,
    5. Sitoh YY,
    6. Verma S,
    7. Keefe R,
    8. Collinson S,
    9. Chong SA,
    10. Heckers S,
    11. Nowinski W,
    12. Pantelis C
    (2009) White matter abnormalities and neurocognitive deficits associated with the passivity phenomenon in schizophrenia: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Psychiatry Res 172:121–127, doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.02.003, pmid:19297135.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Sommer MA,
    2. Wurtz RH
    (2002) A pathway in primate brain for internal monitoring of movements. Science 296:1480–1482, doi:10.1126/science.1069590, pmid:12029137.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  66. ↵
    1. Sommer MA,
    2. Wurtz RH
    (2004) What the brain stem tells the frontal cortex: II. Role of the SC-MD-FEF pathway in corollary discharge. J Neurophysiol 91:1403–1423, doi:10.1152/jn.00740.2003, pmid:14573557.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  67. ↵
    1. Sommer MA,
    2. Wurtz RH
    (2008) Brain circuits for the internal monitoring of movements. Annu Rev Neurosci 31:317–338, doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125627, pmid:18558858.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. Spence SA,
    2. Brooks DJ,
    3. Hirsch SR,
    4. Liddle PF,
    5. Meehan J,
    6. Grasby PM
    (1997) A PET study of voluntary movement in schizophrenic patients experiencing passivity phenomena (delusions of alien control). Brain 120:1997–2011, doi:10.1093/brain/120.11.1997, pmid:9397017.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  69. ↵
    1. Spering M,
    2. Dias EC,
    3. Sanchez JL,
    4. Schütz AC,
    5. Javitt DC
    (2013) Efference copy failure during smooth pursuit eye movements in schizophrenia. J Neurosci 33:11779–11787, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0578-13.2013, pmid:23864667.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  70. ↵
    1. Stephan KE,
    2. Baldeweg T,
    3. Friston KJ
    (2006) Synaptic plasticity and dysconnection in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 59:929–939, doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.10.005, pmid:16427028.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Thaker GK,
    2. Ross DE,
    3. Buchanan RW,
    4. Moran MJ,
    5. Lahti A,
    6. Kim C,
    7. Medoff D
    (1996) Does pursuit abnormality in schizophrenia represent a deficit in the predictive mechanism? Psychiatry Res 59:221–237, doi:10.1016/0165-1781(95)02759-9, pmid:8930028.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  72. ↵
    1. Thakkar KN,
    2. Nichols HS,
    3. McIntosh LG,
    4. Park S
    (2011) Disturbances in body ownership in schizophrenia: evidence from the rubber hand illusion and case study of a spontaneous out-of-body experience. PLoS One 6:e27089, doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0027089, pmid:22073126.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    1. Thakkar KN,
    2. Schall JD,
    3. Logan GD,
    4. Park S
    (2015) Response inhibition and response monitoring in a saccadic double-step task in schizophrenia. Brain Cogn 95:90–98, doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2015.01.007, pmid:25769133.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  74. ↵
    1. Turken AU,
    2. Vuilleumier P,
    3. Mathalon DH,
    4. Swick D,
    5. Ford JM
    (2003) Are impairments of action monitoring and executive control true dissociative dysfunctions in patients with schizophrenia? Am J Psychiatry 160:1881–1883, doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.10.1881, pmid:14514505.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  75. ↵
    1. Van der Stigchel S,
    2. Nijboer TCW
    (2011) The global effect: what determines where the eyes land? J Eye Mov Res 4:1–13.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  76. ↵
    1. Van der Stigchel S,
    2. Meeter M,
    3. Theeuwes J
    (2006) Eye movement trajectories and what they tell us. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 30:666–679, doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.12.001, pmid:16497377.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    1. Waters F,
    2. Price G,
    3. Dragović M,
    4. Jablensky A
    (2009) Electrophysiological brain activity and antisaccade performance in schizophrenia patients with first-rank (passivity) symptoms. Psychiatry Res 170:140–149, doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2008.10.033, pmid:19906434.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  78. ↵
    1. White JM,
    2. Sparks DL,
    3. Stanford TR
    (1994) Saccades to remembered target locations: an analysis of systematic and variable errors. Vision Res 34:79–92, doi:10.1016/0042-6989(94)90259-3, pmid:8116271.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  79. ↵
    1. Wolpert DM,
    2. Ghahramani Z
    (2000) Computational principles of movement neuroscience. Nat Neurosci 3:1212–1217, doi:10.1038/81497, pmid:11127840.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    1. Woods SW
    (2003) Chlorpromazine equivalent doses for the newer atypical antipsychotics. J Clin Psychiatry 64:663–667, pmid:12823080.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  81. ↵
    1. Woodward ND,
    2. Karbasforoushan H,
    3. Heckers S
    (2012) Thalamocortical dysconnectivity in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat 169:1092–1099, doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12010056, pmid:23032387.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  82. ↵
    1. Wurtz RH
    (2008) Neuronal mechanisms of visual stability. Vision Res 48:2070–2089, doi:10.1016/j.visres.2008.03.021, pmid:18513781.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 35 (27)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 35, Issue 27
8 Jul 2015
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Disrupted Saccadic Corollary Discharge in Schizophrenia
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Disrupted Saccadic Corollary Discharge in Schizophrenia
Katharine N. Thakkar, Jeffrey D. Schall, Stephan Heckers, Sohee Park
Journal of Neuroscience 8 July 2015, 35 (27) 9935-9945; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0473-15.2015

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Disrupted Saccadic Corollary Discharge in Schizophrenia
Katharine N. Thakkar, Jeffrey D. Schall, Stephan Heckers, Sohee Park
Journal of Neuroscience 8 July 2015, 35 (27) 9935-9945; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0473-15.2015
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • corollary discharge
  • double-step
  • efference copy
  • saccade
  • schizophrenia

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Articles

  • Choice Behavior Guided by Learned, But Not Innate, Taste Aversion Recruits the Orbitofrontal Cortex
  • Maturation of Spontaneous Firing Properties after Hearing Onset in Rat Auditory Nerve Fibers: Spontaneous Rates, Refractoriness, and Interfiber Correlations
  • Insulin Treatment Prevents Neuroinflammation and Neuronal Injury with Restored Neurobehavioral Function in Models of HIV/AIDS Neurodegeneration
Show more Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Disentangling Object Category Representations Driven by Dynamic and Static Visual Input
  • Irrelevant Threats Linger and Affect Behavior in High Anxiety
  • Multisession Anodal Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Enhances Adult Hippocampal Neurogenesis and Context Discrimination in Mice
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.