Abstract
Posterior cortical atrophy (PCA) is a rare focal neurodegenerative syndrome characterized by progressive visuoperceptual and visuospatial deficits, most often due to atypical Alzheimer's disease (AD). We applied insights from basic visual neuroscience to analyze 3D shape perception in humans affected by PCA. Thirteen PCA patients and 30 matched healthy controls participated, together with two patient control groups with diffuse Lewy body dementia (DLBD) and an amnestic-dominant phenotype of AD, respectively. The hierarchical study design consisted of 3D shape processing for 4 cues (shading, motion, texture, and binocular disparity) with corresponding 2D and elementary feature extraction control conditions. PCA and DLBD exhibited severe 3D shape-processing deficits and AD to a lesser degree. In PCA, deficient 3D shape-from-shading was associated with volume loss in the right posterior inferior temporal cortex. This region coincided with a region of functional activation during 3D shape-from-shading in healthy controls. In PCA patients who performed the same fMRI paradigm, response amplitude during 3D shape-from-shading was reduced in this region. Gray matter volume in this region also correlated with 3D shape-from-shading in AD. 3D shape-from-disparity in PCA was associated with volume loss slightly more anteriorly in posterior inferior temporal cortex as well as in ventral premotor cortex. The findings in right posterior inferior temporal cortex and right premotor cortex are consistent with neurophysiologically based models of the functional anatomy of 3D shape processing. However, in DLBD, 3D shape deficits rely on mechanisms distinct from inferior temporal structural integrity.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Posterior cortical atrophy (PCA) is a neurodegenerative syndrome characterized by progressive visuoperceptual dysfunction and most often an atypical presentation of Alzheimer's disease (AD) affecting the ventral and dorsal visual streams rather than the medial temporal system. We applied insights from fundamental visual neuroscience to analyze 3D shape perception in PCA. 3D shape-processing deficits were affected beyond what could be accounted for by lower-order processing deficits. For shading and disparity, this was related to volume loss in regions previously implicated in 3D shape processing in the intact human and nonhuman primate brain. Typical amnestic-dominant AD patients also exhibited 3D shape deficits. Advanced visual neuroscience provides insight into the pathogenesis of PCA that also bears relevance for vision in typical AD.
Introduction
Visual neuroscience has made big strides over the past decades. The translation of these basic advances into a better understanding of neurological phenotypes remains an important area of research. One of the most complex visual disorders in neurology is posterior cortical atrophy (PCA) (Benson et al., 1988; Berthier et al., 1991; Graff-Radford et al., 1993; Caine, 2004; Tang-Wai et al., 2004; Lehmann et al., 2011a, 2011b; Crutch et al., 2012), a rare syndrome that is characterized by progressive visuoperceptual disturbances and is most often due to Alzheimer's disease (AD). Markers of cerebral amyloidosis are most often indistinguishable from what is seen in typical AD (Rosenbloom et al., 2011; de Souza et al., 2011; Lehmann et al., 2013a). The predominance of visual complaints and symptoms is attributable to the unusual distribution of neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs) and neuronal loss in primary and secondary visual areas (Hof et al., 1990; Hof and Bouras, 1991; Levine et al., 1993; Hof et al., 1997; Tang-Wai et al., 2004; von Gunten et al., 2006; Kas et al., 2011; Rosenbloom et al., 2011; Lehmann et al., 2013a, 2013b; Ossenkoppele et al., 2015). The topography of the neuropathological damage is heterogeneous, with variable involvement of dorsal or ventral areas of the visual processing stream (Levine et al., 1993; Hof et al., 1997; Migliaccio et al., 2012). Although deficits in 3D perception have been mentioned in the context of PCA (Rogelet et al., 1996), to the best of our knowledge, 3D shape-processing deficits in PCA have not been analyzed in a systematic and neuroscientifically grounded manner.
The way in which the brain computes 3D shape from a 2D retinal image has been largely elucidated based on single-cell recordings in nonhuman primates combined with functional imaging of the intact human and nonhuman primate brain (for review, see Orban, 2011). The 3D shape-processing system involves both the dorsal and ventral processing stream. The relative contribution of each of these streams depends on the cue defining 3D shape: 3D shape-from-shading relies more heavily on the inferior temporal cortex and 3D shape-from-motion mainly on more dorsal processing areas (Georgieva et al., 2008; Orban, 2011), whereas 3D from texture and disparity activate both dorsal and ventral processing stream areas (Orban, 2011).
Within the visual processing hierarchy, the input to the 3D shape-processing system relies on the output of areas extracting elementary features. These features are also used to extract contours. For the luminance cue, the orientation of these contours is extracted at the level of V1 (De Valois et al., 1982) and, for nonluminance cues, at higher levels of the ventral stream, as shown in nonhuman primate (Sáry et al., 1995; Mysore et al., 2006; Mysore et al., 2010; El-Shamayleh and Movshon, 2011) and human (DuPont et al., 1997; Mendola et al., 1999; Tyler et al., 2006) studies. We therefore applied a hierarchical experimental design with three levels: (1) 3D shape processing from shading, texture, motion, and binocular disparity; (2) 2D shape processing from the same cues; and (3) processing of the elementary features.
We included two patient control groups for two distinct reasons. The clinical phenotype of diffuse Lewy body dementia (DLBD) is known to be associated with visuoperceptual deficits (Mori et al., 2000; Calderon et al., 2001; Mosimann et al., 2004). This allowed us to assess how disease specific the visuoperceptual deficits observed in PCA and their neuroanatomical underpinnings were. As our second patient control group, we included a group of patients with typical AD (Dubois et al., 2014). By “typical AD,” we refer to the most common form of AD that initially presents with an amnestic syndrome (McKhann et al., 2011; Dubois et al., 2014). Including a typical AD group allowed us to ascertain the relevance of the findings obtained in the PCA for the far more common amnestic AD phenotype.
We used 3D stimuli and behavioral tasks similar to those used in the previous studies (Todd, 2004; Georgieva et al., 2008; Nelissen et al., 2009) and related the deficits to regional measures of cerebral volume loss. The anatomical correlations may provide a better understanding of the pathogenetic mechanisms leading to visual dysfunction in PCA and shed light on the contribution of cortical regions to 3D shape processing in the intact brain.
Material and Methods
Subjects
The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of University Hospitals Leuven. All participants provided written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision except for PCA Case 6, who had a congenital left monocular blindness.
A consecutive academic memory clinic-based series of 16 patients who received a diagnosis of PCA due to AD were screened for participation in this research study. Based on performance of prior test runs (see below), three patients were excluded, leaving 13 patients who were eligible to undergo the full protocol (two men, 11 women). The study started as a monocentric study run at the academic memory clinic of University Hospitals Leuven (n = 8, Cases 1–7 and 13). In a second phase, additional subjects were recruited via the Liège University Hospital (n = 4, Cases 8–11) and the University Hospital Brussels (n = 1, Case 12) (Table 1). The clinical diagnosis was based on the standard procedures as implemented at the different academic centers. In all cases, the presenting symptom consisted of visuoperceptual complaints with insidious onset and slow progression and preserved insight. The impact on instrumental activities of daily living was caused by the visuoperceptual disturbances (such as reduced speed of reading, difficulties with car driving under perceptually degraded conditions such as rainy weather, falls due to misperception of uneven surfaces such as stairs, etc.). Apart from a clinical evaluation by an academic neurologist (R.V., E.S., or J.V.), investigations consisted of a clinical structural MRI, a conventional neuropsychological assessment and, in 11 of 13 cases, a clinical [18F]-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography ([18F]-FDG-PET) scan. The clinical diagnosis of PCA was confirmed by the distribution of FDG-PET hypometabolism. In all patients who received an [18F]-FDG-PET, hypometabolism was located predominantly in posterior cortex. The laterality of the lesions varied between cases, as well as their extent. In Cases 6–9 and 13, the premotor cortex was also involved. The individuals' MRI volume loss was consistent with the FDG-PET hypometabolism confirming the mainly posterior involvement, but with a lower sensitivity. In the two patients in whom no FDG-PET was available (Cases 5 and 12), posterior gray matter loss was present on the MRI scan. CSF AD biomarkers (ELISA; Fujirebio Europe) and Pittsburgh Compound B ([11C]-PIB) amyloid PET were performed in 7 and 3 patients, respectively, and confirmed AD as etiology (Table 1). In all patients who received a lumbar puncture, amyloid-beta 42 (Aβ42) was <500 pg/ml, except Case 5, in whom Aβ42 = 581 pg/ml with a pathological tau level of 498 pg/ml, which was therefore clearly also within the AD range. Clinical follow-up currently extends 3–6 years after the study and the clinical evolution was fully compatible with the diagnosis of PCA in all cases, with the development of severe disability due to virtual blindness in a more advanced stage. None of these subjects have developed a corticobasal or other extrapyramidal neurodegenerative syndrome over time.
Thirty cognitively intact age-matched controls (17 men, 13 women, age 61 ± 5 years) participated in the psychophysical experiment. Controls were recruited through advertisement in a regional newspaper asking for volunteers above the age of 55 years for participation in a scientific study. Control participants did not have any subjective memory or visual complaints or any history of significant neurological or psychiatric illness. None of the control participants took any psychotropic or vasoactive medication.
Twenty-four of these controls and 94 additional cognitively intact age-matched controls (50 at the KU Leuven site, 44 at the Liège site) also received a volumetric MRI (60 men, 58 women, age 65 ± 7.32 years, range 52–85).
As a patient control group, we included a consecutive series of 13 patients who fulfilled the consensus criteria for clinically probable dementia with Lewy bodies (McKeith et al., 2005) [11 men, 2 women, age 69 ± 6.1 years, disease duration 2.4 ± 1.5 years, Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score 24.5 ± 3.1].
As a further patient control group, we included a consecutive series of 14 patients with amnestic MCI due to AD or early-stage clinically probable AD (11 men, 3 women, 66 ± 6.8 years, MMSE score 25.1 ± 3.7) who were recruited through the memory clinic of University Hospitals Leuven.
According to one-way ANOVA, the 3 patient groups did not differ in MMSE score (F(2,39) = 1.95, p = 0.16). Age differed significantly between the 4 groups (F(3,66) = 8.04, p < 0.001): PCA patients did not differ in age from the healthy controls (p = 0.70), but were younger than the typical AD patients (p = 0.01) and DLBD patients (p < 0.001).
Psychophysical experiments
All 13 PCA patients, the 13 DLBD and 14 AD patients, as well as 30 healthy controls, participated in the psychophysical experiments. All controls underwent the full hierarchical design except for four of them, who only received the 3D tests.
Stimulus presentation and response registration were controlled by a PC running Presentation 11.3 (Neurobehavioral Systems) and in-house software written in C. Participants were seated 50 cm from a cathode ray tube monitor (diagonal measurement 48.26 cm, resolution 1024 × 768 pixels, refresh rate 75 Hz).
The psychophysical experiments were organized according to a hierarchical design with three levels. At the 3D level, we evaluated the processing of 3D shape-from-shading, texture, motion, or binocular disparity cue (Fig. 1A–C). At the 2D level, we evaluated orientation discrimination of rectangular gratings defined by luminance, texture, motion, or disparity cues (Fig. 1D). At a third, basic level, we evaluated elementary feature processing of the four cues (Fig. 1E).
3D shape perception.
The visual stimuli were created and rendered in four modalities (3D shape-from-shading, shape-from-texture, shape-from-motion, and shape-from-disparity) using 3D Studio Max by Autodesk. The 3D shape-from-shading and the 3D shape-from-texture stimuli (Fig. 1A–C) were created as described in Georgieva et al. (2008). The 3D shape-from-motion and 3D shape-from-disparity stimuli (Fig. 1A) were filled with an isotropic and statistically homogeneous texture pattern. The 3D shape-from-motion stimuli oscillated back and forth around a vertical axis at a rate of 87.5°/s (Norman et al., 2004). The 3D shape-from-disparity stimuli were displayed binocularly through red/blue anaglyph glasses.
The stimuli depicted 10 randomly generated complex 3D surfaces, representing the front surface of meaningless 3D objects, with a large assortment of variably shaped hills, ridges, valleys, and dimples at multiple scales (Fig. 1B,C) (Norman and Todd, 1995; Fleming et al., 2004; Norman et al., 2004; Todd, 2004; Georgieva et al., 2008). Because the surfaces were smoothly curved, they did not provide information from configurations of edges and vertices (Moore and Engel, 2001; Kourtzi et al., 2003). Depth maps corresponding to three representative surfaces indicate that, relative to the global maximum of the 3D surface (point nearest to the subject), the border was located ∼5 cm further back in depth (Fig. 1B). Compared with the global maximum of each 3D surface, the depth values present in the surfaces averaged 1.89 cm (SD across all points of all 3D surfaces, 1.03 cm), with the extremes of the distribution corresponding to the border of the surfaces (Fig. 1C).
Before initiation of the experimental runs, four test trials were provided per cue. In the first two trials, the experimenter pointed the five fingers of her left or right hand to the subject and the subject had to indicate which fingers were closest to him/her (i.e., the middle finger). These test trials did not require accurate depth perception because they could be solved based on prior knowledge and were meant to clarify the concept of proximity. In the last two test trials, subjects performed the 3D shape task on a 3D surface on the computer screen. The 3D surfaces used for the test trials differed from those used in the subsequent experimental runs. Subjects who failed the first two test trials, who were not sure about the location of the local maxima in the last two trials, or were unable to verbally repeat the task instructions for the last two test trials were excluded from the study.
In the experimental runs, the four cues (shading, texture, motion, and disparity) were presented in a blocked design, one block per cue. The 10 3D surfaces were presented in random order in a block. The order of the blocks was counterbalanced across participants. When 3D structure was induced by a monocular cue (shading, texture, or motion), participants wore a patch over the nondominant eye. When the 3D structure was induced by binocular disparity, the participants wore red/blue anaglyph glasses. Each trial was initiated by pressing “RETURN” on the keyboard. An image of a 3D shape (average size, 12°) appeared at the center of the screen on a blue (motion, shading, texture) or black (disparity) background until the next trial was initiated. Participants were allowed to make eye movements and were instructed to indicate the global maximum of the 3D structure (point nearest to them) as accurately as possible by displacing a red cross using the mouse. If the participant was not able to manipulate the mouse, he/she could indicate the global maximum with a pen and the experimenter would move the red cross to that location until the participant had approved it verbally.
As our primary outcome analysis, we calculated the error-in-depth (in centimeters) as follows. The difference between the global maximum indicated by the subject and the true global maximum can be mathematically described as a 3D vector that represents the error in the 3D shape perception (the 3D shape error vector). Error-in-depth is the projection of this vector onto the sagittal plane. We then conducted a two-way mixed ANOVA with group (four levels: healthy control participants, PCA, DLBD, and AD) as the between-subjects factor and cue (four levels: shading, texture, motion, and disparity) as the within-subjects factor (threshold: p < 0.05). When sphericity could not be assumed (Mauchly's sphericity test: p < 0.05), p values were adjusted using the Greenhouse–Geisser correction. Post hoc comparisons consisted of the comparison between PCA and each of the control groups for each of the cues corrected for the number of cues and contrasts (n = 12).
A complementary and well established way of analyzing the responses per group for this type of 3D shape-processing task relies on the in-plane distribution of the responses (Todd et al., 2001; Koenderink et al., 2002). This amounts to an analysis of the frontoparallel components of the 3D shape error vector. We calculated covariance ellipses encompassing 95% of the responses per group for each 3D surface (see Fig. 3A for an illustration of these ellipses) and averaged these ellipses over the 10 3D surfaces. We analyzed the location of the ellipse center (x- and y-coordinates), the size of the ellipse (mean of the two eigenvalues), and its elongation (largest eigenvalue divided by smallest eigenvalue) (Fig. 3B). The x- and y-coordinates correspond to the group average of the global maxima indicated by the members of the group projected in the frontoparallel plane (as opposed to the sagittal plane) and has to be referenced to the projection of the true global maximum in the frontoparallel plane. Size is a measure of response variability within a group; elongation is a measure of stimulus-induced bias in the distribution of the responses, for example, that induced by ridges. These parameters were defined for the single-3D-surface ellipses. Each of the parameters was analyzed using a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA across 3D surfaces with group (four levels) and cue (four levels) as factors at a significance threshold of p < 0.05, corrected for the number of ellipse parameters analyzed; therefore, the uncorrected p < 0.01. Post hoc comparisons consisted of the comparison between PCA and each of the control groups. The significance level for the post hoc comparisons was corrected for the number of comparisons per parameter (n = 12).
The above analyses allowed us to determine differences between PCA and the control groups, but it is also pertinent to ascertain whether performance in PCA differed from random. We randomly selected 1000 locations on the 3D surface within an interior zone, avoiding the edges of the surface, and calculated the distribution of the distances in depth. We compared the random error-in-depth to the error-in-depth obtained from the PCA patients for each cue using a Mann–Whitney U test (threshold: uncorrected p < 0.05). We also derived from the random distributions a covariance ellipse and compared the parameters of this covariance ellipse with that obtained in PCA.
Furthermore, we evaluated whether performance in PCA differed from what one would expect if the deviation of responses in PCA compared with controls were due to a random motor error rather than a perceptual error. We randomly generated 30 data points on a 3D surface (equaling the sample size of the group of healthy controls) and adjusted their x- and y-coordinates such that the center of the corresponding covariance ellipse equaled that of the healthy controls (simulating normal detection of the global maximum), but its elongation and size matched those of PCA patients (simulating the variance observed in PCA under the assumption that this variance would be fully due to hypothetical motor problems). We used a Mann–Whitney U test (threshold: uncorrected p < 0.05) to compare the error-in-depth (relative to the global maxima) of the 30 data points and that obtained in the PCA patients. This procedure was repeated 1000 times for each cue. We calculated in which percentage of iterations the responses in PCA differed from the simulated data points.
2D orientation discrimination and elementary feature extraction.
In an orientation discrimination task, observers had to decide whether a rectangular grating shown in a circular aperture (diameter 6°, spatial frequency 0.8°, random phase) at the center of the screen was tilted 45° clockwise or anticlockwise from the vertical meridian in a two-alternative forced-choice procedure. The orientation of the grating was defined by discontinuities in luminance, texture coarseness, speed of motion (Sáry et al., 1994), or disparity (Fig. 1D).
To test elementary feature extraction, subjects performed a 2-alternative forced choice task. Two squares (size 6° × 6°) differing in one feature (luminance, coarseness of texture, speed of motion, or disparity) were presented simultaneously 6.8° to the left and to the right on the horizontal meridian (Fig. 1E). Participants were instructed to indicate the brightest shape, the shape filled with the coarsest texture, the shape with the fastest motion, or the shape closest in depth.
Before initiation of the experimental runs, three test trials with maximum contrast and unlimited exposure were provided for the 2D shape-processing and feature extraction task for each of the four cues. Subjects who failed the test trials were excluded. In the experimental runs, the four cues (luminance, texture, motion, and disparity) were presented in a blocked design. In a trial, stimuli were presented for 800 ms after an auditory warning cue (50 ms) and a brief interval (400 ms). For the tasks with the disparity cue, the stimuli were repeatedly presented for 800 ms until the participant made a response. Participants held a response box in their preferred hand and were instructed to press one of two keys depending on the target orientation or the position of the target square and depending on the task. In patients who, according to the test trials, were unable to press a key (left or right), the following options were evaluated for response modality: to say left or right aloud, to lift their left or right hand, or to point to one or the other stimulus shown on a piece of paper in front of them. A new trial was initiated after a response or after 4 s if no response was made. A block ended after 60 trials with responses.
For each cue, we determined the logarithm of the Michaelson contrast (Michaelson, 1927) between luminance levels, texture coarseness, speed of motion, and disparity needed for 82% accuracy using a single QUEST procedure (Watson and Pelli, 1983). The thresholds, expressed as log of these contrasts, were submitted to an ANOVA with group (four levels: healthy control participants, PCA patients, DLBD patients, and typical AD patients) as the between-subjects factor and cue as the within-subjects factor.
Behavioral analysis at the individual level.
Apart from the between-group comparisons, we also determined which of the individual PCA patients had a pathological error-in-depth score for the 3D tasks compared with healthy controls using a one-tailed modified t test (Crawford and Garthwaite, 2002), as well as how many of the individual PCA patients showed a dissociation between 3D tests, a dissociation meaning that the difference in performance between two tasks in an individual patient was larger than that expected based on the difference in healthy controls (p < 0.05) (Crawford and Garthwaite, 2005).
In addition, we calculated diagnostic power for each 3D test by means of area under the curve (AUC) values obtained with receiver operator curves to discriminate between PCA and controls and between PCA and each of the patient control groups (DLBD and typical AD patients).
Relationship between 3D and lower-order deficits.
To evaluate to which degree lower-order deficits contributed to the 3D shape deficit in PCA patients, we performed for each cue a hierarchical multiple linear regression with error-in-depth from the global maximum as the dependent variable and the contrast thresholds on the lower-order tasks as the independent variables. Probability of F to enter the model was set at <0.05 with probability to remove set to >0.10.
For those cues for which lower-order deficits significantly contributed to the 3D shape-processing deficit, we investigated whether impairment on 3D tasks remained significant in PCA patients after adjusting for performance on these 2D and elementary feature tasks. We performed an ANCOVA with performance of the 3D task (error-in-depth) as the dependent variable, group (PCA, healthy controls) as the between-subjects variable, and performance on the lower-order task as the covariate.
Volumetric MRI
Image acquisition.
A high-resolution T1-weighted structural MRI was obtained in patients and healthy controls. In the nine PCA patients, 11 DLBD cases (the two remaining cases had medical contraindications for MRI), 14 AD cases, and the 74 controls scanned at the Leuven site, the scans were acquired on a 3T Philips Intera system equipped with an eight-channel receive-only head coil (Philips SENSitivity Encoding head coil) using a 3D turbo field echo sequence [coronal inversion recovery prepared 3D gradient-echo images, inversion time (TI) 900 ms, shot interval = 3000 ms, echo time (TE) = 4.6 ms, field of view (FoV) = 250 × 250 mm2, voxel size 0.98 × 0.98 × 1.2 mm3]. In the four PCA patients and 44 healthy controls from Liège, a high-resolution T1-weighted anatomical image was acquired on a 3T head-only scanner (Magnetom Allegra; Siemens) operated with the standard transmit-receive quadrature head coil (T1-weighted 3D magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo sequence, shot interval = 1960 ms, TI = 1100 ms, TE = 4.35 ms, FoV = 230 × 173 mm2, voxel size = 0.9 × 0.9 × 0.9 mm3). The maximum interval between the structural MRI and the psychophysical experiments was 7 d.
Preprocessing.
Preprocessing and statistical analysis were performed with Statistical Parametric Mapping 8 (SPM8, Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) and the Voxel-Based Morphometry 8 toolbox (VBM8, http://dbm.neuro.uni-jena.de/vbm). High-resolution T1-weighted images were corrected for bias-field inhomogeneities, registered to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space using linear (12-parameter affine) and nonlinear transformations, and segmented into gray matter, white matter, and CSF within the same generative model (Ashburner and Friston, 2005). Voxel values were modulated for volumetric changes and smoothed with a 8 × 8 × 8 mm3 full-width at half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel.
Voxel-based morphometry.
To identify the principal areas of cortical atrophy, we conducted a voxelwise ANCOVA with four groups (PCA, healthy controls, DLBD, and typical AD) as between-subjects factor, and age, sex, and scanner site as nuisance variables using the VBM8 toolbox with the “nonlinear only modulation” option (threshold: cluster-level FWE-corrected p < 0.05 for a voxel-level threshold of p < 0.001 uncorrected; Poline et al., 1997).
To investigate the correlation between regional gray matter volume loss and 3D shape processing, individual error-in-depth scores of the 13 PCA patients on the 3D shape-processing tasks were entered as independent variables in voxelwise linear regression analyses with modulated gray matter volume as the dependent variable. The threshold for significance was set to a cluster-level FWE-corrected p < 0.05 for a voxel-level threshold of uncorrected p < 0.005 (Poline et al., 1997).
We conducted a similar voxelwise linear regression analysis in the patient control groups and evaluated the overlap between the significance map obtained in PCA at the preset threshold (see above) and that obtained in the patient control groups. When assessing the overlap, the latter maps were thresholded at voxelwise uncorrected p < 0.01 because we mainly wanted to determine how specific the correlation was for PCA.
We assessed the effect of scanner site in a voxelwise analysis of covariance with scanner site as covariate of interest, age, and sex as covariates of no interest and the modulated gray matter images from the 118 healthy controls as dependent variables. The threshold for significance was set to a cluster-level FWE-corrected p < 0.05 for a voxel-level threshold of uncorrected p < 0.005.
fMRI
To delineate the functional anatomy network in healthy controls, we conducted an fMRI study in 18 cognitively intact older adults for 3D shape-from-shading and 3D shape-from-motion using appropriate 2D control stimuli (see Fig. 11A). We only selected monocular cues for fMRI for practical reasons and left out texture as a cue because of the number of control conditions needed for 3D shape-from-texture (Georgieva et al., 2008).
The fMRI substudy was only offered to those PCA patients who underwent scanning at the Leuven site (n = 9) to avoid between-scanner effects on the BOLD signal. Cases 1, 2, 6, 7, and 12 participated and the remaining 4 subjects refused, principally for the following practical reasons: to avoid an extra visit, long-distance travel, or additional stress for patient or because of unwillingness to lie still in the MRI scanner for 1.5 h.
Experimental design.
Visual stimuli were projected from a liquid crystal display projector (Barco Reality 6400im 1024 × 768, 75 Hz refresh frequency) onto a translucent screen positioned in the bore of the magnet at a distance of 36 cm from the point of observation. Participants viewed the stimuli through a tilted mirror attached to the head coil and wore an eye patch over the right eye. Participants were asked to maintain fixation on a small red target (0.45° × 0.45°) in the center of the screen. Fixation was monitored online using an MR-compatible ASL eye tracking system 5000 (Applied Science Laboratories).
The 3D shape-from-shading condition was identical to those used by Georgieva et al. (2008). In the two control conditions (see Fig. 11A), we eliminated appearance of depth by randomly repositioning the pixels within the boundary of each object (2D pixel-scrambled) or by mimicking the pattern of shading gradients with three to five randomly shaped ovals with blurred edges on a light background (2D shaded-blobs) (Georgieva et al., 2008).
The fMRI experiment used a block design with 20 s epochs in which we presented 15 stimuli for 1333 ms without gaps. Conditions were presented in different epochs and repeated three times in a time series. Six time series were recorded in each participant and presentation order of the conditions was randomized between the six time series. The fMRI experiment also contained three further conditions related to 3D shape-from-motion and two control conditions, but for the sake of brevity these results will not be reported here.
Image acquisition and analysis.
The whole-brain functional scans consisted of T2*-weighted gradient-echo echoplanar images obtained on a 3 T Philips Intera scanner equipped with an 8-channel receive-only head coil (6 time series, 212 scans per time series, repetition time = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, 2.75 × 2.75 mm2 in-plane resolution, 36 3.75 mm thick axial slices without gap). The T2*-weighted images were realigned and coregistered to the T1-weighted image. The T1-weighted image was warped into the MNI space using the procedures implemented in the VBM8 toolbox (see above) and the resulting deformation field was used to spatially normalize the functional images. The voxel size of the functional images in MNI space was 3 × 3 × 3 mm3. The images were spatially smoothed with a 6 × 6 × 6 mm3 FWHM Gaussian kernel before statistical analysis.
We examined in each PCA patient whether his/her activity pattern in the 3D shape-from-shading network differed from that seen in the control group, controlling for the effect of regional gray matter volume. To this end, we determined the effect size for contrast 1 (3D shape-from-shading minus 2D pixel-scrambled) and 2 (3D shape-from-shading minus 2D shaded-blobs) in the different clusters of the 3D shape-from-shading network. For each control, this network was defined using a random-effects analysis (conjunction of contrasts 1–2) in the remaining 17 healthy controls (Georgieva et al., 2008). With this leave-one-out approach we avoided a bias in favor of the control group that might arise had we defined the network in the control group and examined activity levels in patients in that predefined network (Gillebert et al., 2011). For each PCA patient, the effect size for the two contrasts in the 3D shape-from-shading network was compared with that obtained in healthy controls using regional gray matter volume as a covariate of no interest (Crawford et al., 2011) (threshold: two-tailed p < 0.05).
Results
The primary purpose of this study was to determine whether and for which cues 3D shape processing was affected in PCA, whether this could be explained by deficits at lower levels of processing, and which regions were implicated structurally. Furthermore, we overlaid the fMRI activity pattern for 3D shape processing in healthy controls directly with the volumetric changes that correlate with 3D shape processing in PCA and also evaluated how functional activity levels in the 3D shape network were altered in a subset of PCA patients. Finally, we also evaluated how disease specific the behavioral deficits and the correlations with gray matter volume were by comparing our findings to two patient control groups, DLBD and typical AD.
Behavioral comparison between PCA and controls
3D shape perception
The error-in-depth for the four cues and the four groups is shown in Figure 2. Repeated-measures ANOVA (4 × 4 design) with group (healthy controls, PCA, DLBD, and AD patients) and cue (shading, texture, motion, and binocular disparity) as factors and error-in-depth as an dependent variable revealed a significant main effect of group (F(3,65) = 17.09, p < 0.001) and of cue (F(2.18,141.42) = 7.44, G-G adjusted p = 0.001) and a significant group-by-cue interaction (F(6.53,141.42) = 3.29, G-G adjusted p = 0.004; Fig. 2, Table 2). According to post hoc comparisons, error-in-depth was larger in PCA than in healthy controls (p < 0.001) for each of the monocular cues. Error-in-depth tended to be larger in PCA than in AD for texture (p = 0.005). Error-in-depth did not differ between PCA and DLBD for any of the cues (p > 0.31; Fig. 2), nor was there any difference between PCA and any of the control groups for binocular disparity (p ≥ 0.28; Fig. 2).
The average ellipse parameters representing the in-plane distribution of the responses of PCA and normal subjects are shown in Figure 3C for each of the cues. In healthy controls, the angle and elongation is very similar between cues because the underlying 3D structure is the same. The ellipses in PCA are systematically larger and wider than those of healthy controls. The main parameters of these ellipses are shown in Figure 3, D–G. According to the repeated-measures ANOVA with group and cue as factors, there was a significant main effect of group on all 4 ellipse parameters (G-G adjusted p ≤ 0.01; Fig. 3C–G), a main effect of cue on location (x and y) and on size (p ≤ 0.002), and a group-by-cue interaction on location (y-coordinate, p = 0.006; Fig. 3D–G). According to post hoc comparisons, ellipse size differed significantly between PCA and healthy controls for all 4 cues (p ≤ 0.003). The location of the ellipse center differed between PCA and healthy controls for shading (y-coordinate p = 0.004) and for texture (y-coordinate p = 0.003). No other differences between PCA and the patient groups reached our stringent corrected significance threshold.
In PCA, error-in-depth and in-plane distribution of responses differed from random for all cues (p ≤ 0.01) except texture (p = 0.48; Fig. 2). Performance of 3D shape-from-shading, motion, and disparity in PCA also differed from a distribution based on random motor control errors for shading, motion, and disparity (p <0.05 in >99% iterations; Fig. 2).
At an individual level (Table 2), all PCA patients except Case 4 performed worse than healthy controls (p < 0.05; Crawford and Garthwaite, 2002) on 3D shape processing for monocular cues, with 8 patients performing worse than healthy controls for all 3 monocular cues. Two of the PCA patients were significantly impaired on the 3D shape-from-disparity task (Table 2). There were a number of dissociations between 3D tasks in PCA patients: 3D shape-from-motion was significantly more affected than 3D shape-from-disparity or 3D shape-from-shading in 9 and 5 cases, respectively. 3D shape-from-texture was significantly more affected than 3D shape-from-disparity and 3D shape-from-shading in 8 and 2 cases, respectively. 3D shape-from-shading was significantly more affected than 3D shape-from-disparity in 5 cases.
The AUC of error-in-depth for discriminating between PCA and controls was 0.97 for texture, 0.94 for shading, 0.93 for motion, and 0.70 for disparity. For PCA compared with AD patients, the AUC was 0.81 for texture, 0.69 for motion, 0.60 for shading, and 0.54 for disparity. The AUC of error-in-depth for discriminating between PCA and DLBD patients was 0.58 for texture, 0.45 for shading, 0.54 for motion, and 0.51 for disparity.
2D orientation discrimination
We investigated whether the 2D processing level was also affected (Figs. 1D, 4A,B). However, we were unable to obtain reliable thresholds for the disparity cue in four of the PCA cases (Table 2) and, because of these missing data, we analyzed performance for the monocular cues separately from that for the binocular cue.
For the monocular cues, a 4 × 3 ANOVA analysis revealed main effects of group (F(3,59) = 17.64, p < 0.001) and of cue (F(1.53,90.4) = 112.52, G-G adjusted p < 0.001), as well as a significant group-by-cue interaction (F(4.6,90.4) = 2.53, G-G adjusted p = 0.039; Fig. 4A, Table 2). PCA patients needed a larger orientation difference than healthy controls to reach criterion for all monocular cues (p < 0.001). For motion, they needed a larger orientation difference than AD (p = 0.005; Fig. 4A). No other differences were found between PCA and the patient control groups (p > 0.06).
No differences between groups were found for binocular disparity (F(3,52) = 2.40, p = 0.08).
Elementary feature extraction
We also evaluated whether elementary feature extraction was affected (Figs. 1E, 4C,D). However, we were unable to obtain reliable thresholds for the disparity cue in five of the PCA cases (Table 2) and therefore analyzed performance for the monocular cues separately from that for the binocular cue.
A 4 × 3 ANOVA revealed significant main effects of group (F(3,62) = 24.7, p < 0.001) and of cue (F(2,124) = 36.85, p < 0.001), as well as a significant group-by-cue interaction (F(6,124) = 3.48, p = 0.003; Fig. 4C, Table 2). To reach criterion, PCA patients needed a larger contrast than healthy controls (p < 0.001) regardless of cue type (Fig. 4C). They also needed a significantly larger contrast than DLBD and AD patients for luminance (p = 0.001; all others p >0.02).
No differences between groups were found for binocular disparity (F(3,56) = 1.93, p = 0.14).
Relationship between 3D and lower-order deficits
We investigated whether the deficits for 2D-level processing or elementary feature extraction could account for the 3D shape-processing deficit. According to a stepwise multiple linear regression analysis, performance of PCA patients on the 3D shape-from-shading task could be partly predicted by performance at the 2D-processing level (R2 adjusted = 0.46, p = 0.007). Performance on the 3D shape-from-texture task could also be partly explained by performance at the elementary feature extraction level (R2 adjusted = 0.28, p = 0.037). Performance on the 3D shape-from-motion and 3D-shape-from-disparity task could not be predicted by performance on the lower-order tasks (p > 0.1).
When performance at the 2D-processing level was included as covariate in a one-way ANOVA with group (healthy controls, PCA patients) as the factor and performance on the 3D shape-from-shading task as the dependent variable, the effect of group remained significant (F(1,36) = 6.99, p = 0.01). The difference in performance between healthy controls and PCA patients on the 3D shape-from-texture task also remained significant when performance at the elementary feature extraction level was included as the covariate of no interest (F(1,36) = 14.39, p < 0.001).
Volumetric comparison between PCA patients and control groups
The volumetric comparison of the PCA group with the controls confirmed the predominantly posterior localization of the atrophy (Fig. 5A–C). In the group analysis, primary visual cortex was relatively spared, at least the part representing central vision, whereas lateral occipital and medial occipital cortex were involved together with occipitotemporal, occipitoparietal, and posterior parietal cortex. Dorsal premotor cortex was also atrophic (Fig. 5A–C). Medial pulvinar and lateral geniculate nuclei also exhibited significant volume loss (Fig. 5A–C).
Compared with DLBD and AD, extrastriate cortex showed significant volume loss in PCA (Fig. 6A–D,F). Inversely, the anterior portion of the hippocampal formation was atrophic in AD compared with PCA (Fig. 6E). We did not find any regions of greater volume loss in DLBD compared with PCA.
Relationship between volume loss and 3D-processing deficits
In PCA, scores on the 3D shape-from-shading task correlated significantly with gray matter volume loss in right posterior inferior temporal cortex extending into lateral temporal and inferior parietal cortex (extent total cluster 2394 1.5 × 1.5 × 1.5 mm3 voxels, cluster-level corrected p = 0.010; Fig. 7). The highest-significance peak was in the right inferior temporal gyrus (ITG; 44, −63, −6, z = 3.71), followed by a peak in the right inferior occipital gyrus (36, −72, −5, z = 3.59) and a peak in the right middle temporal gyrus (63, −45, 3, z = 3.33). The correlation between 3D-from-shading and right inferior temporal volume loss was also present when we excluded the monocularly blind Case 6 (extent total cluster 8414 cluster-level p < 0.001, r = −0.96).
We also obtained a significant correlation between volume loss and 3D shape-from-disparity in PCA in right inferior temporal cortex (50, −51, −27, z = 4.20, extent 1958, corrected p = 0.017; Fig. 8A,B) and right premotor cortex (44, 9, 34, z = 4.58, extent 5558, corrected p < 0.001; Fig. 8A,C). The inferior temporal 3D shape-from-disparity cluster lay anterior to the 3D shape-from-shading cluster and there was some overlap between the right occipitotemporal cluster obtained for 3D shape-from-shading (0.08% of the total cluster extent) at our preset threshold and that obtained for 3D shape-from-disparity (0.10% of the total cluster extent; Fig. 9A, yellow).
A whole-brain analysis did not reveal significant correlations between volume loss and 3D shape processing for motion and texture. Even if we lowered the threshold for these maps to voxel-level p < 0.01, we did not find any overlap between the right posterior inferior temporal cluster obtained for shading and disparity and 3D shape-from-motion or 3D shape-from-texture.
We overlaid the inferior temporal cluster obtained for 3D shape-from-shading with a probabilistic retinotopic map (Abdollahi et al., 2014; Fig. 7C): visual areas V4t and LO1, MST, MT, FST, and the putative human homolog of the posterior inferior temporal region (phPIT; Kolster et al., 2010) overlapped with the posterior part of this region. A large proportion of this region lay anterior to retinotopically organized cortex.
For shading, we evaluated whether the right inferior temporal cluster was specific for the 3D level or overlapped with the maps obtained for the lower-order processes. At a lower threshold, performance on 2D shape-from-shading correlated with volume loss posteriorly to the 3D shape-from-shading cluster and there was some overlap between the right occipitotemporal cluster obtained for 3D-shape-from-shading at our preset threshold (voxel-level p < 0.005, cluster-level p < 0.05 corrected) and 2D shape-from-shading (voxel-level p < 0.005 without cluster-level threshold; Fig. 9B, yellow). We did not find overlap with elementary feature extraction for luminance.
Next, we evaluated whether the volumetric correlations obtained for 3D shape-from-shading and 3D shape-from-disparity were specific for PCA compared with the two other patient groups. There was limited overlap in the anterior portion of the inferior temporal cluster with the map obtained in typical AD patients for the correlation between brain volume and 3D shape-from-shading if we lowered the threshold in the AD patients to uncorrected p < 0.01 (Fig. 10, yellow). We also investigated whether performance on 3D shape-from-shading and 3D shape-from-disparity in DLBD and AD patients correlated with gray matter volume in the regions defined independently in PCA for these cues. A significant correlation was found in AD between error-in-depth for 3D shape-from-shading and gray matter volume in the right inferior temporal region obtained in PCA for this cue (r = −0.626, p = 0.017).
We also evaluated in DLBD and typical AD patients whether any regions outside of the regions involved in 3D shape-from-shading and 3D shape-from-disparity in PCA correlated with 3D shape-from-shading and 3D shape-from-disparity at FWE corrected cluster-level p < 0.05. The only such region was found in AD in the anterior cingulate for 3D shape-from-disparity (12, 42, 19, z = 4.22, extent 2491, corrected p = 0.003).
Finally, we evaluated whether the effect of scanner site on brain volumetry overlapped with the region where we found a correlation between 3D shape-from-shading and 3D shape-from-disparity and brain volume. This was restricted to a very minor overlap for 3D shape-from-shading (0.005% of the inferior temporal cluster) and for 3D shape-from-disparity (0.42% of the inferior temporal cluster).
fMRI activity levels in the 3D-processing network
Next, we overlaid directly the volumetric analysis of 3D shape-from-shading in PCA with the known fMRI activity pattern for 3D shape from shading versus 2D controls in healthy controls. According to online infrared eye tracking, participants kept stable gaze fixation on the central fixation cross. In healthy controls, 3D shape-from-shading activated the caudal ITG bilaterally (left ITG: −45, −73, −8; z = 6.86; extent 599 3 × 3×3 mm3 voxels; cluster-level p < 0.001; right ITG: 45, −70, −11; z = 5.28; 346 voxels; Fig. 11B). The right ITG activation cluster overlapped with the region described previously by Georgieva et al. (2008) (marked by black outline in Fig. 11B) and with the region where volume loss correlated with performance of 3D shape-from-shading by PCA subjects (Fig. 11C, overlap marked in yellow). In the controls, another extensive activity cluster for 3D shape-from-shading was present in the left anterior intraparietal sulcus (IPS) extending into the supramarginal gyrus (−48, −34, 43; z = 4.92; 447 voxels; p < 0.001; Fig. 11B).
Finally, we looked for functional alterations in the 3D shape-from-shading network due to PCA over and above what one would expect based on brain volume. We compared the average fMRI activity levels in each of the clusters that were activated in healthy subjects, controlling for gray matter volume as a covariate of no interest (Crawford et al., 2011). In 4 of 5 PCA patients, the effect size for contrast 1 (3D shape-from-shading minus 2D pixel-scrambled) in left posterior ITG was significantly smaller than in healthy controls (p < = 0.05). In right ITG, the effect size was reduced in 2 of 5 patients (p < = 0.05; Crawford et al., 2011). No significant effects were found in the IPS cluster. Overall, compared with baseline, the response in PCA patients compared with controls was reduced in both ITGs, in particular in the right ITG (Fig. 11D,E).
Discussion
In PCA patients, deficits on 3D shape-from-shading were associated with volume loss in right posterior inferior temporal cortex and deficits on 3D shape-from-disparity with volume loss slightly more anteriorly and in the right premotor cortex (Fig. 7). These findings obtained in PCA can be readily understood in the light of current neurophysiologically based models of 3D shape processing developed based on nonhuman primate research and fMRI in the intact human brain for these cues (Orban, 2011). They may also bear relevance for the far more common amnestic predominant forms of AD.
Potential limitations
PCA is characterized by a complex constellation of visuoperceptual and visuospatial disturbances (Benson et al., 1988; Hof et al., 1990; Graff-Radford et al., 1993; Levine et al., 1993; Giovagnoli et al., 2009). Nevertheless, we are confident that our psychophysical estimates reliably reflected the perceptual processes of interest rather than dysfunction in other domains: Subjects could only take part if they had passed a test session and proven that they were able to perform the 3D shape task reliably, leading to the exclusion of three patients. Furthermore, the response modality was flexibly adapted to the individual's capabilities based on the test session (key press, pointing, and vocal responses). Third, in 11 of 13 PCA patients, the 3D shape threshold was within normal limits for at least one cue (Table 2), most frequently the disparity cues (6/11 patients). The relative preservation of 3D shape from any cue provides evidence that the deficits observed were not caused by generic task demands. Fourth, the choice of global maximum by the PCA patients differed significantly from a random distribution or from what one would obtain in case of a random motor control error. For the lower-order tasks, further support is provided by the average staircase curves in the different populations. A reliable plateau level was reached, except for 2D-from-motion, in which the curve indicated a floor effect (Fig. 4). As a final argument, our main result, inferior temporal volume loss in relation to 3D shape processing (Fig. 7), is unlikely to be explained by manuomotor or visuomotor deficits (such as optic ataxia or gaze apraxia), which are typically localized to the dorsal stream.
Error-in-depth was abnormal only for monocular cues. For 3D shape-from-disparity, between-subjects variance in healthy controls was larger than for the other cues (Fig. 2). The carrier texture for 3D from disparity had the same resolution as that used for motion so that carrier texture cannot account for this difference. A possible explanation is the deterioration and increased variability in stereoacuity with normal aging (Bohr and Read, 2013). The analysis of the in-plane distribution of responses (Todd et al., 2001; Koenderink et al., 2002) clearly had added value in this respect and also revealed abnormal dispersion of responses in PCA patients for disparity.
Neuroanatomical correlates of deficits in 3D shape perception
Performance on 3D shape-from-shading correlated with right posterior inferior temporal volume loss (Fig. 7A). A separate fMRI experiment that we conducted in healthy older adults established the overlap between the fMRI focus activated by 3D shape-from-shading and the correlation between volume and 3D shape-from-shading performance in PCA patients (Fig. 11C). This cluster encompassed a caudal inferior temporal region that has been implicated previously in 3D shape-from-shading and shape-from-disparity by fMRI in the intact brain (Georgieva et al., 2008).
Comparison between this ITG region obtained in patients and the maximum probability map of retinotopic areas (Fig. 7C; Abdollahi et al., 2014) localizes the peak of this cluster to the rostral tip of the putative human homolog of the dorsal phPIT (phPITd; Kolster et al., 2010; Abdollahi et al., 2014) and neighboring retinotopic areas. In healthy human subjects, the local maximum of the 3D shape-from-shading activation is also located close to the rostral tip of the phPITd (Orban et al., 2014). Anteriorly, the volumetric cluster extended beyond retinotopic areas. In monkey fMRI, 3D shape-from-shading specifically activates V4 bilaterally and the dorsal portion of TEO (Boussaoud et al., 1991; Distler et al., 1993; Nelissen et al., 2009), now recognized as retinotopic PITd (Kolster et al., 2009, 2014). However, more widespread activation in FST and surrounding STS regions has also been reported using more naturalistic stimuli (Sereno et al., 2002).
In the right ITG region, the correlation with volume loss was not limited to 3D shape-from-shading (Fig. 7). There was also a correlation between 2D luminance processing and volume in part of this cluster. This was mainly due to overlap between the posterior part of the cluster for 3D shape-from-shading and that obtained for 2D luminance (Fig. 9B). Areas involved in 3D shape-from-shading (principally area phPITd) may be close to areas involved in 2D orientation processing (such as the homolog of area V4; De Weerd et al., 1996), which may provide the input for the extraction of 3D shape (Fleming et al., 2011).
Slightly more anteriorly to the inferior temporal cluster obtained for 3D shape-from-shading, volume loss was associated with 3D shape-from-disparity. Performance on 3D shape-from-disparity correlated with volume loss, not only in right posterior inferior temporal, but also in right premotor cortex. Numerous previous studies have indicated that the 3D-processing network for binocular disparity is more extensive than the 3D networks for any of the monocular cues: in addition to ventral occipitotemporal areas (Janssen et al., 1999, 2000a, 2000b), it includes the IPS over its entire extent and even premotor cortex (Durand et al., 2007; Georgieva et al., 2009; Joly et al., 2009; Theys et al., 2012; Van Dromme et al., 2015). The premotor region where we find a correlation between volume loss and 3D shape-from-disparity fits well with these earlier observations (Georgieva et al., 2009) and likely encompasses the putative human homolog of F5 (Ferri et al., 2015). One of its subdivisions, area F5a, has been consistently found to be activated during 3D shape processing for disparity in fMRI in nonhuman primates (Joly et al., 2009; Theys et al., 2012), as well as in the intact human brain (Georgieva et al., 2009).
For two cues, motion and texture, 3D shape-processing deficits were present behaviorally, but without clear neuroanatomical substrate. These cues were among the most severely impaired. The areas implicated in 3D shape-from-motion, such as MT and FST (Vanduffel et al., 2002), were clearly affected in PCA patients (Fig. 5). The same holds true for the occipitotemporal regions involved in 3D shape-from-texture (Georgieva et al., 2009). Further work is needed to understand the lack of correlation for the motion and texture cues, which may reflect the organization (serial vs parallel) of the network extracting 3D shape from these cues, severity of atrophy, or the relatively small sample size combined with stringent thresholds applied to the whole-brain search volume.
Relevance for other patient groups
The atrophy pattern in extrastriate cortex was specific for PCA patients. Accordingly, the association between posterior inferior temporal volume loss and 3D shape-from-shading and shape-from-disparity deficits was also relatively specific for PCA. The profound 3D shape-processing deficits in DLBD patients (Figs. 2, 3) were not associated with posterior inferior temporal volume loss. This indicates that structural integrity of right posterior inferior temporal cortex is not the sole factor determining success on these 3D shape-processing tasks. A previous volumetric MRI study in DLBD failed to reveal neocortical volume loss (Whitwell et al., 2007). In contrast, functional changes as measured with FDG-PET (Minoshima et al., 2001) or task-related fMRI (Taylor et al., 2012) frequently have been reported posteriorly. The visuoperceptual deficits in DLBD patients have been hypothetically attributed to loss of dopaminergic or cholinergic input (Whitwell et al., 2007), disconnection of the inferior longitudinal fascicle (Nedelska et al., 2015), or dopaminergic deficits in the retina (Mori et al., 2000) rather than structural volume loss.
In patients with the amnestic-predominant presentation of AD, we found 3D shape perception deficits, although in a mitigated form (Figs. 2, 3). Conceivably, 3D-processing deficits may go unnoticed in AD patients because they are overshadowed by the concomitant cognitive deficits or because gradual changes in visual perception may remain below the awareness level as long as they do not affect instrumental activities directly, as they do in PCA patients. Gray matter volume loss in AD extended into the anterior border of the right inferior temporal region implicated in 3D shape-from-shading in PCA. The distribution of NFTs in typical AD may partly extend into the anterior portion of the inferotemporal region implicated in 3D shape perception in PCA (Lewis et al., 1987; Braak et al., 1989). Gray matter volume in the right inferior temporal region identified in PCA also correlated with performance on this task in AD. Accordingly, the 3D shape-from-shading deficit in AD may partly rely on a same substrate as that underlying 3D shape-processing deficits in PCA. However, other mechanisms may also contribute to the 3D shape-processing deficits in AD, for example, at a postperceptual level. The correlation between anterior cingulate volume loss and 3D shape-from-disparity scores in AD could point in that direction given the role of this region in decision making (Walton et al., 2007).
Conclusion
According to contemporary models of the functional anatomy of 3D processing, 3D shape-from-shading principally relies on posterior inferior temporal cortex. In contrast, 3D shape-from-disparity relies on widely distributed regions, not only in the ventral stream, but also in the dorsal stream extending into premotor cortex. The data that we obtained in patients with neurodegeneration targeting the ventral and dorsal processing stream are consistent with predictions based on the functional anatomical models derived from nonhuman primate research and fMRI of the intact brain. However, the 3D shape-processing deficits in DLBD remain unexplained at this stage and indicate that the structural integrity of right inferior temporal cortex cannot be the sole factor determining success on these 3D shape-processing tasks.
Footnotes
↵*C.R.G. and J.S. are co-first authors.
↵†G.A.O. and R.V. are co-senior authors.
This work was supported by Inter-University Attraction Pole P6/29 (R.V., E.S., G.A.O.) and P7/11 (R.V., E.S., G.A.O.), KU Leuven (Grant OT/12/097 and Programme Financing to R.V.), Fund for Scientific Research–Flanders (FWO; Grant G.0660.09N to R.V.), Fund for Scientific Research of the Wallonia-Brussels Federation (E.S.) and Stichting Alzheimer Onderzoek (Grant 11020 to R.V.), Vlaams Initiatief voor Netwerken voor Dementie Onderzoek (R.V.), and Transformationeel Geneeskundig Onderzoek BioAdaptAD (R.V.). R.V. and K.V.L. are senior clinical investigators of the FWO. C.R.G. holds a Sir Henry Wellcome Postdoctoral Fellowship funded by the Wellcome Trust (Grant 098771/Z/12/Z). We thank the patients and their families for their participation and Wouter Depuydt for technical support.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Rik Vandenberghe, Neurology Department, University Hospitals Leuven, Herestraat 49, Box 7003, 3000 Leuven, Belgium. rik.vandenberghe{at}uz.kuleuven.ac.be
This article is freely available online through the J Neurosci Author Open Choice option.