Figure 2. RGFP966 enables highly specific memory and A1 plasticity. A, Behavior. The latency to bar press for each test tone frequency was determined in each group to indicate which frequencies best elicited reward-related behavior. Rats treated with RGFP966 (N = 6) were faster to respond than performance-matched rats treated with vehicle alone (N = 6) to frequencies associated with reward (Signal A) and reward delivery [Signal B; Z-score for paired difference in latency to respond to each test frequency; calculated as RGFP966 minus vehicle, responses to 1.1 kHz (Signal B) were on average 1.25 ± 0.7 s significantly faster, p = 0.030; responses to 2.4 kHz were 0.26 ± 0.5 s faster, p = 0.280; responses to 5.0 kHz (Signal A) were 0.75 ± 0.3 s significantly faster, p = 0.003; responses to 10.6 kHz were 0.75 ± 0.3 s faster, p = 0.380; responses to 22.4 kHz were 0.42 ± 0.5 s slower, p = 0.830), which indicates the formation of a more highly specific memory for acoustic frequency with RGFP966. Inset, Mean (±SE) bar-press latencies show group response gradients for the RGFP966 (N = 6) and vehicle (N = 6) groups, without respect to performance-matched pairs. B, Sound frequency representation in A1. Rats treated with RGFP966 had greater expansions of best frequency areas in A1 to overrepresent sound frequencies near the reward, Signal A (5.0 kHz; in the 4.0–6.3 kHz frequency band) and reward delivery signal, Signal B (1.1 kHz; in the 1.0–1.6 kHz frequency band). Asterisks indicate frequency bands that were significant with both paired (sign test) and unpaired (Mann–Whitney–Wilcoxon) one-sided tests after Holm–Bonferroni correction (p values for sign test/Mann–Whitney–Wilcoxon tests are indicated in parentheses): 1.0–1.6 kHz, 4.8 ± 2.6% significant increase in A1 area (p = 0.002/p = 0.004); 1.6–2.5 kHz, 6.9 ± 4.8% less A1 area (p = 0.984/p = 0.479); 2.5–4.0 kHz, 3.7 ± 2.4% less A1 area (p = 0.891/p = 0.418); 4.0–6.3 kHz, 4.1 ± 2.4% significant increase in A1 area (p = 0.022/p = 0.010); 6.3–10 kHz, 3.5 ± 3.2% increase in A1 area (p = 0.328/p = 0.016); 10–15.9 kHz, 0.7 ± 1.5% increase in A1 area (p = 0.219/p = 0.366); 15.9–25.2 kHz, 1.2 ± 3.1% increase in A1 area (p = 0.086/p = 0.116); 25.2–39.9 kHz, 0.9 ± 3.2% less A1 area (p = 0.445/p = 0.334). C, Sound level representation in A1. Analysis of best frequency areas of A1 representation between groups (y-axis) across various sound levels (x-axis) revealed that frequency-specific expansion for Signal A in rats treated with RGFP966 occurred only in the representation of the best frequency determined at 70 dB SPL, i.e., at the unique sound level of Signal A. The expansion of Signal B occurred across all sound levels. Asterisks indicate that sound levels that were significantly increased in RGFP966-treated rats using both paired (sign test) and unpaired (Mann–Whitney–Wilcoxon) one-sided tests after Holm–Bonferroni correction (p values for the sign test/Mann—Whitney–Wilcoxon tests are indicated in parentheses): for Signal A, 70 dB, 4.1 ± 2.4% significant increase in A1 area (p = 0.027/p = 0.030); 60 dB, 4.2 ± 2.3% significant increase in A1 area (p = 0.027/p = 0.044); 50 dB, 0.02 ± 2.5% nonsignificant change in A1 area (p = 0.055/p = 0.089); 40 dB, 2.9 ± 2.2% decrease in A1 area (p = 0.056/p = 0.084); for Signal B: 70 dB, 4.8 ± 2.6% significant increase in A1 area (p = 0.004/p = 0.004); 60 dB, 8.0 ± 5.0% significant increase in A1 area (p = 0.041/p = 0.041); 50 dB, 4.7 ± 3.6% significant increase in A1 area (p = 0.029/p = 0.036); 40 dB, 4.3 ± 3.8% significant increase in A1 area (p = 0.014/p = 0.041). Mean differences between six pairs of RGFP966- and vehicle-treated animals (total N = 12) are shown (±SE). Note that the study design did not permit detection of significant differences between Signal A and Signal B in the identified behavioral and neural response changes between groups. Thus, the report focuses on RGFP966 versus vehicle treatment effects with respect to frequency specificity per se.