Abstract
The brain constantly creates perceptual predictions about forthcoming stimuli to guide perception efficiently. Abundant studies have demonstrated that perceptual predictions modulate sensory activities depending on whether the actual inputs are consistent with one particular prediction. In real-life contexts, however, multiple and even conflicting predictions might concurrently exist to be tested, requiring a multiprediction coordination process. It remains largely unknown how multiple hypotheses are conveyed and harmonized to guide moment-by-moment perception. Based on recent findings revealing that multiple locations are sampled alternatively in various phase of attentional rhythms, we hypothesize that this oscillation-based temporal organization mechanism may also underlie the multiprediction coordination process. To address the issue, we used well established priming paradigms in combination with a time-resolved behavioral approach to investigate the fine temporal dynamics of the multiprediction harmonization course in human subjects. We first replicate classical priming effects in slowly developing trends of priming time courses. Second, after removing the typical priming patterns, we reveal a new theta-band (∼4 Hz) oscillatory component in the priming behavioral data regardless of whether the prime was masked. Third, we show that these theta-band priming oscillations triggered by congruent and incongruent primes are in an out-of-phase relationship. These findings suggest that perceptual predictions return to low-sensory areas not continuously but recurrently in a theta-band rhythm (every 200–300 ms) and that multiple predictions are dynamically coordinated in time by being conveyed in different phases of the theta-band oscillations to achieve dissociated but temporally organized neural representations.
Introduction
We do not live in a static and invariable world; rather, external information dynamically evolves over time. Therefore, to cope adaptively with an ever-changing environment, the brain constantly creates perceptual predictions about forthcoming stimuli to guide perception efficiently (Rao and Ballard, 1999; Engel et al., 2001; Summerfield and Egner, 2009; Bastos et al., 2012; den Ouden et al., 2012). Meanwhile, in most real-world contexts, multiple and even conflicting predictions might exist concurrently to be tested against the current inputs (Carpenter and Grossberg, 1987; Mumford, 1991, 1992), requiring a multiprediction coordination process. However, the mechanisms by which the brain dynamically sends and coordinates multiple predictions to guide and modulate perception remain unknown.
Neuronal oscillations may act as a temporal organization infrastructure (Klimesch, 1999; Ward, 2003; Buzsaki, 2006) to mediate dynamically and harmonize multiple perceptual hypotheses (Roelfsema et al., 1996; von Stein et al., 2000; Engel et al., 2001; Arnal et al., 2011). Recently, several psychophysical studies using a time-resolved behavioral measurement revealed rhythmic fluctuations (Landau and Fries, 2012; Fiebelkorn et al., 2013a; Song et al., 2014) directly in behavioral performances (‘behavioral oscillations’) and demonstrated a novel oscillation-based temporal mechanism in spatial attention such that multiple locations are sampled alternatively in various phase of attentional rhythms. Based on these findings, we posit that this oscillation-based temporal organization mechanism may also underlie the multiprediction coordination process.
Here, we used priming paradigms (Eimer and Schlaghecken, 2003; Sumner, 2007; Huang et al., 2011) in combination with a time-resolved behavioral approach to address the issue. In a typical priming trial, an initial feedforward input (i.e., a prime) rapidly activates one perceptual prediction, which then descends through re-entrant pathways to lower levels, where they are matched to ongoing sensory inputs (i.e., a probe that is either congruent or incongruent with the perceptual prediction triggered by the prime; Enns and Di Lollo, 2000). As a result, we can plot the fine temporal course of the prediction re-entrance process by measuring performance of the subsequent probe at variable time lags after prime presentation. Moreover, by examining the temporal relationship in the priming courses between congruent and incongruent probe, we could also study how multiple perceptual predictions interact dynamically with each other, given that equally likely predictions about the probe have to be held simultaneously.
Specifically, as illustrated in Figure 1A, participants fixed their gaze on the central spot and made speeded responses to orientation of the probe stimuli (pointing to left or right) with their reaction time (RT) recorded. The 100 ms probe was preceded by a 20 ms prime stimulus, which was either backward masked (Fig. 1A, left) or unmasked (Fig. 1A, right). The probe and the prime stimuli had either the same (congruent condition) or different (incongruent condition) orientations (Fig. 1B). Critically, to assess the fine temporal dynamics of priming courses, we ranged the stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs; masked priming: mask-to-probe SOA; unmasked priming: prime-to-probe SOA) from 0 to 0.8 s in steps of 0.02 s (Fig. 1A), corresponding to a sampling frequency of 50 Hz.
Materials and Methods
Ethics statement.
The study was approved by the ethics committee of Institute of Biophysics at Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing.
Subjects.
Fifty-six human subjects (18 for the masked priming experiment, 8 males; 16 for the unmasked priming experiment, 6 males; 16 for the neutral priming experiment, 6 males; 6 for the single-subject experiment, 4 males; all right handed, average age 24 years) participated in the experiments. All subjects had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity, provided written informed consent, and were paid to compensate them for their time.
Experimental procedures.
Subjects sat in a dark room 57 cm in front of a CRT monitor (100 Hz) with their heads stabilized in a chin rest and their responses were recorded using a parallel port response keypad. Visual stimuli were presented in black (0 cd/m2) on a gray background (0.99 cd/m2). As shown in Figure 1, the prime stimulus was a left- or right-pointing arrow (2.86° × 1.21°), fitting into the inner cutout of a mask with a rectangular outer shape (3.43° × 1.79°). The neutral prime stimulus was a rectangle of the same size as the arrow prime. The probe stimulus was a left- or right-pointing hollow arrow (6.21° × 2.29°).
In each trial, subjects were requested to maintain visual fixation at a central spot and make speeded response to the orientation of a probe stimulus (pointing left or right). The RTs in each trial were calculated from the offset of the probe stimulus. In masked and unmasked priming experiments, the prime and probe are either congruent (pointing in the same direction) or incongruent (pointing in a different direction). In the neutral masked priming experiment, the prime is a “neutral” rectangle figure that did not contain relevant orientation information for the subsequent probe stimulus. The masked priming experiment consisted of a 20 ms prime stimulus, a 60 ms blank screen, a 100 ms mask stimulus, and a 100 ms probe stimulus (Fig. 1A, left), whereas in the unmasked priming experiment (Fig. 1A, right), the stimulus sequence consisted of a 20 ms prime stimulus and a 100 ms probe stimulus without a mask stimulus in between. The intertrial interval ranged from 1 to 2 s. Critically, to achieve dense temporal assessment of behavioral performance, we used time-resolved measurement such that the probe stimuli appeared at one of 40 temporal intervals from 0 s to 0.8 s in steps of 20 ms (masked priming experiment: mask-probe SOA; unmasked priming experiment: prime-probe SOA), corresponding to a sampling frequency of 50 Hz. Each subject completed 16 trials for each condition (congruent and incongruent) at each of the 40 SOA intervals. The SOA of each trial was pseudorandom from 0 to 0.8 s in steps of 20 ms, balanced across trials to have exactly the same likelihood. For the single-subject study, each subject completed 64 trials for each condition (congruent and incongruent) at each SOA interval. For the masked priming task, the mask-probe SOA ranged from 0 to 0.4 s in steps of 20 ms and, for the unmasked priming task, the prime-probe SOA ranged from 0 to 0.5 s in steps of 20 ms. The reason we shortened the SOA range in the single-subject study was to obtain enough trials (64 instead of 16 trials in previous experiments) at each SOA interval to get a good approximation of the behavioral time courses in single subject.
Data analyses.
Behavioral RT data were analyzed with MATLAB (The MathWorks) using functions from the EEGLAB toolbox and CircStat toolbox. For each subject, we excluded RTs >3 SDs from the mean cross all trials. We calculated the temporal profile of RTs as a function of SOA from 0 to 0.8 s in steps of 0.02 s (50 Hz sampling frequency) for each condition (congruent vs incongruent).
For each subject, we first extracted a slowly developing trend representing the classical priming effect time course by calculating the 240 ms (12 time bins) moving average of RT time courses for each condition (congruent and incongruent), respectively. Next, for each subject, we subtracted the slow trends from corresponding RT time courses to obtain detrended RT time courses separately for each condition to remove possible interferences from classical priming and expectancy effects (Fiebelkorn et al., 2013a; Song et al., 2014).
Next, to investigate the spectral characteristics of the detrended RT temporal profiles, we conducted spectrum analysis on the detrended RT time courses separately for each subject. Specifically, we performed fast Fourier transform (FFT) to convert the priming time courses into the frequency domain (after zero padding and application of a Hanning window) separately for each condition (congruent and incongruent). To examine the phase relationship between congruent and incongruent conditions, we calculated the phase difference between congruent and incongruent conditions as a function of frequency from 0 to 20 Hz (sampling frequency of 50 Hz here) separately for each subject, then we calculated cross-subject coherence in the phase difference values, resulting in a congruent-incongruent (C-IC) phase difference coherence pattern as a function of frequency. Testing for nonuniformity for C-IC phase differences in the theta-band (3–5 Hz) across subjects involved use of circular statistics (Rayleigh test for nonuniformity for circular data in CircStats toolbox).
We further performed a randomization procedure by shuffling the RT time series for congruent and incongruent conditions separately within each subject to assess statistical significance of observed priming oscillations at theta band as well as the C-IC phase relationship. After each randomization, we conducted FFT on surrogate signals, similar to that of the original RT data analysis; we repeated this procedure 200 times, arriving at a distribution of spectral power for each frequency point from which we obtained the p < 0.05 threshold (uncorrected). We applied multiple-comparison correction to the uncorrected randomization threshold spectrum profile by setting the maximum across all frequency bins as the threshold (Nichols and Homes, 2002; Song et al., 2014). Similarly, for each randomization, we conducted the same phase analysis on the surrogate signals by calculating cross-subject coherence in the C-IC phase difference, resulting in a distribution of C-IC phase coherence for each frequency point, from which we obtained the p < 0.05 threshold (uncorrected). Similar multiple-comparison correction analysis was applied by setting the maximum threshold values across all frequency bins as the final permutation threshold for cross-subject coherence in the C-IC phase difference.
Correlation analysis was performed to examine the relationship between the theta-band priming oscillations and the typical priming effects. Specifically, for each subject, we calculated the spectral amplitude of the theta-band rhythm for Congruent and Incongruent conditions respectively (i.e., Congruent, Incongruent). Furthermore, to characterize the typical priming effects[(i.e., positive compatibility effect (PCE), negative compatibility effect (NCE)], the slow trends for Congruent and Incongruent conditions were compared, resulting in C-IC difference waveforms in each subject. The PCE effects were calculated by averaging the C-IC differences within PCE time range (for masked priming, SOA within 0-100 ms; for unmasked priming, SOA within 100-800 ms) and the NCE effects were calculated by averaging within NCE time range (for masked priming, SOA within 200–800 ms; none for unmasked priming). The correlation analysis was then performed between the amplitude of the theta-band rhythms (Congruent and Incongruent) and the typical priming effects (PCE and NCE).
Results
Alternating theta-band oscillations in masked priming time courses
Eighteen subjects participated in the masked priming task (Fig. 1A, left) and performed well in the probe orientation discrimination task (percentage correct: 0.99 ± 0.0018). Only small number of trials were discarded due to either no response or out-of-range RTs (trial discarding percentage: 0.02 ± 0.002). Figure 2A illustrates the raw RT time courses as a function of mask-to-probe SOAs under prime-probe congruent (red) and incongruent (black) conditions averaged across all subjects. Generally, the RT traces showed classical “facilitation followed by inhibition” priming patterns, as revealed in previous masked priming studies (Eimer and Schlaghecken, 2003). Specifically, for short SOAs (< 100 ms), the congruent condition elicited faster RTs than the incongruent condition (i.e., PCE), whereas for longer SOAs, the priming effects reversed (i.e., NCE) such that the congruent condition was slower than the incongruent condition. Furthermore and critically, the RT time courses displayed an oscillatory pattern in addition to the slowly developing “PCE followed by NCE” trend. Specifically, the NCE was not sustained but revealed an intermittent rhythmic pattern; in fact, statistically significant NCE effects occurred in several nonconsecutive temporal intervals and not at SOAs around 300 and 600 ms (paired t test, Fig. 2A). To better examine the oscillatory pattern embedded in RT time courses, we extracted slow trends of RT time courses in each subject by calculating 240 ms smoothed RT traces for the congruent and incongruent conditions separately (similar slow trends were obtained when using least-squares polynomial fitting method). The slowly developing trends exhibited a “PCE followed by NCE” pattern (paired t test, Fig. 2B) resembling classical masked priming effects (Eimer and Schlaghecken, 2003). To remove possible interferences from the classical priming and expectancy patterns, we next removed the slow trends from the raw RT courses separately for each subject. Detrended RT time courses demonstrated a clear periodic pattern under both congruent and incongruent conditions (Fig. 2C). Moreover, the oscillations under the two conditions were in a type of temporally switching relationship (Fig. 2C), suggesting that the rhythms initiated by the congruent and incongruent primes might have a consistent phase lag relationship in the theta-band.
To further examine the characteristics of oscillatory components in masked priming, we next performed spectrum analyses on the detrended priming traces separately for each subject. Moreover, to determine whether they reflected simply random fluctuations in time, we performed permutation tests to examine statistical significances. Figure 2D shows the grand average (n = 18) power spectrum of the detrended RT traces for the congruent (red) and incongruent (black) conditions. Both congruent and incongruent conditions showed significant (p < 0.05, corrected for multiple comparison) activations in the theta-band (∼4 Hz). We next examined the phase relationship between the congruent (red) and incongruent (black) conditions by calculating cross-subject consistency in the C-IC phase differences as a function of frequency. As shown in Figure 2E, the theta-band (∼4 Hz, marked by red star) showed significant (p < 0.05, permutation test corrected for multiple comparison) cross-subject coherence in the C-IC phase difference. The C-IC theta-band phase difference, representing the phase lag between congruent and incongruent conditions, clustered around 108.4° (Fig. 2E, inset small figure) and was statistically significant (Rayleigh test, p = 0.03), supporting the consistent phase relationship in the theta-band rhythms between the congruent and incongruent conditions (Fig. 2C). Finally, we found no correlations (Pearson's correlation, n = 18) between theta-band oscillatory components and classical priming effects (congruent vs PCE: r = 0.05, p = 0.86; incongruent vs PCE: r = 0.12, p = 0.65; congruent vs NCE: r = −0.31, p = 0.2; incongruent vs NCE: r = −0.09, p = 0.73). In other words, the theta-band periodic pattern seems to be a separable component embedded in time-resolved priming time courses that is not attributable to classical priming effects.
In sum, we demonstrated that masked priming traces (Fig. 2A) actually consist of two concurrent components, a well established “PCE followed by NCE” pattern (Fig. 2B) widely reported in previous masked priming studies and, more critically, a new theta-band (∼4 Hz) oscillatory component (Fig. 2C) driven in an out-of-phase manner by congruent and incongruent primes.
Unmasked priming also reveals alternating theta-band oscillations
The logical next question is, do theta-band oscillations only occur in masked priming or do they reflect a general phenomenon (i.e., prediction re-entrance and coordination) in priming? Unlike masked priming (Fig. 1A, left), which is associated with a “PCE-followed-by-NCE” pattern, unmasked priming (Fig. 1A, right) reveals a “PCE-only” pattern. Theories abound to explain the dissociations between masked and unmasked priming (e.g., motor inhibition, perceptual interactions, mask-triggered inhibition; for review, see Sumner, 2007; Sohrabi and West, 2009). Taking a different perspective here, we focused on examining the generality of rhythmic fluctuations in priming because both masked and unmasked priming are associated with the generative process with two concurrent perceptual predictions. Specifically, we investigated whether theta-band oscillations observed in masked priming also occur in unmasked priming despite their dissociated classical priming effects (PCE-followed-by-NCE vs sustained PCE only).
Sixteen subjects participated in the unmasked priming task (percentage correct: 0.99 ± 0.002; trial discarding percentage: 0.025 ± 0.002). As shown in Figure 3A, the unmasked priming traces also displayed an oscillatory pattern in addition to a slowly developing trend. Specifically, the congruent condition (red) elicited faster RTs than the incongruent condition (black) on average, consistent with the typical PCE pattern in unmasked priming (for review, see Sumner, 2007; Sohrabi and West, 2009); the fine temporal structure of the unmasked priming traces, again, showed a rhythmic pattern. After extracting and removing slow RT trends (PCE-only course, similar slow trends were obtained when using least-squares polynomial fitting method) representing the classical unmasked priming effect (Fig. 3B), the detrended RT time courses (Fig. 3C) demonstrated clear rhythmical fluctuations for both congruent (red) and incongruent (black) conditions. Initiated theta rhythms under both conditions were also in an out-of-phase relationship. Spectrum analysis and permutation testing supported significant theta-band (∼4 Hz) activations (Fig. 3D) and consistent theta-band phase differences between congruent and incongruent conditions (Fig. 3E); the C-IC phase lag clustered around 152° (Rayleigh test, p = 0.002). We found no correlations (Pearson's correlation, n = 16) between theta-band oscillatory components and classical PCE effects in unmasked priming (congruent vs PCE: r = −0.03, p = 0.9; incongruent vs PCE: r = −0.29, p = 0.28).
Taking these two experiments together, in addition to replicating classical priming effects (PCE-followed-by-NCE in masked priming; PCE-only in unmasked priming) in slowly developing trends, we discovered a general theta-band oscillation component in priming behavior regardless of whether the prime is masked. Furthermore, both congruent and incongruent conditions underwent these rhythmic modulations in an out-of-phase manner.
Neutral primes do not initiate priming oscillations
Previous work has shown rhythmic patterns in visual detection after a salient event (e.g., an auditory sound; Fiebelkorn et al., 2011; Romei et al., 2012), we next conducted a control masked priming task to determine whether a neutral prime (Fig. 1B, bottom), which is a salient but irrelevant event, could also initiate theta-band oscillations in subsequent probe performances. Sixteen subjects participated in the study and performed the same task (percentage correct: 0.99 ± 0.001; trial discarding percentage: 0.023 ± 0.002). As illustrated in Figure 4A, the detrended RT time courses (middle) with neutral prime revealed smaller (cf. Figs. 2D, 3D) and no significant spectral peaks in the theta band (right), indicating that salient but uninformative primes do not effectively initiate priming oscillations, possibly because of their failure to trigger particular perceptual predictions. These results also suggest that onset-triggered periodicities in detection performances cannot account for the observed theta-band oscillations in the present studies.
Priming oscillations in individual subjects
Finally, we investigated whether theta-band rhythms and the C-IC phase relationship in priming could be reliably demonstrated in individual subjects. We recruited additional subjects (three for masked priming and three for unmasked priming) to perform the priming task for 64 trials at each SOA value under each condition (congruent and incongruent). Figure 4B plots the raw RT time courses as a function of SOA under the congruent (red) and incongruent (black) conditions for each of the three subjects in a masked priming (Fig. 4B, left) and an unmasked priming task (Fig. 4B, right). Similar to the group results (see Fig. 2C for masked priming; Fig. 3C for unmasked priming), every individual subject showed reliable priming oscillations directly, as well as the C-IC switching patterns in raw RT traces. Note that despite the priming periodicities consistently shown in all subjects, the fine temporal structures varied to some extent across subjects. For example, in masked priming (Fig. 4B, left), subject 3 began to leave “PCE” and enter the “NCE” pattern around 300 ms, whereas subject 1 and subject 2 were still in the “PCE” phase. Rhythmic fluctuations were most consistent across subjects during early SOA periods, likely reflecting temporary intersubject alignment by the resetting event (i.e., the prime stimuli) and became gradually variable across subjects thereafter.
Discussion
Using a fine-grained behavioral measurement, we assessed priming performances in human subjects to investigate the fine temporal structure of the multiprediction coordination processes during which the brain generates, sends, and harmonizes multiple and conflicting perceptual hypotheses about forthcoming inputs. Typical priming effects (“PCE followed by NCE” in masked priming and “PCE-only” in unmasked priming) were replicated in slowly developing trends. Critically, after removing classical priming patterns, we demonstrated a new theta-band oscillatory component driven in an out-of-phase manner by congruent and incongruent primes in both masked and unmasked priming tasks. Together, these results suggest that perceptual predictions descend to sensory areas not continuously but recurrently in a theta-band rhythm and multiple predictions are carried by feedback signals in a temporally dissociated “competition-like” manner (i.e., conveyed in different phases of theta-band rhythms).
Perception has been viewed as an inferential process of hypothesis testing (i.e., predictive coding; Rao and Ballard, 1999) during which generated hypotheses descend to lower areas to be matched against sensory inputs. This process has been suggested to be mediated by ongoing interactions between feedback connections carrying perceptual hypotheses and bottom-up inputs (Harth et al., 1987; Mumford, 1991, 1992). Notably, an appropriate match may not be accomplished in just one pass, thus requiring iterative interactions and matching between the perceptual hypotheses and sensory events (Carpenter and Grossberg, 1987; Harth et al., 1987; Mumford, 1992; Ullman, 1995; Enns and Di Lollo, 2000; Ahissar and Hochstein, 2004; McMains and Kastner, 2011). Actually, several proposed models suggest iterative looping feedback that finally culminates in confirmation of the perceptual hypothesis that best fits input data (Carpenter and Grossberg, 1987; Harth et al., 1987; Mumford, 1992; Enns and Di Lollo, 2000; Engel et al., 2001). In the present study, we demonstrated a general theta-band oscillation component in priming behavioral time courses, suggesting that perceptual predictions, initially triggered by prime events, return to lower-order areas not continuously but recurrently and periodically in a theta-band rhythm (every 200–300 ms). Our results thus provide novel evidence supporting the iterative dynamic characteristics of the “predictive coding” process in visual perception.
We demonstrated that the fine priming time course actually consist of two components: a slowly developing trend that resembles a classical priming pattern and a new theta-band (∼4 Hz) rhythmic component. Replication of classical priming effects in slow trends is consistent with coarse temporal measurements (at SOAs every several hundred milliseconds) used in previous studies, thus suggesting little influence of the present design on classical priming behavior. Conversely, theta-band oscillatory patterns, given their presence in both masked and unmasked priming tasks, cannot be explained in terms of previous priming theories (for review, see Sumner, 2007; Sohrabi and West, 2009) that account for the dissociations between masked and unmasked priming in typical priming effects (i.e., “PCE followed by NCE” in masked priming and “PCE-only” in unmasked priming). Moreover, the theta-band oscillatory did not correlate with the slow trends, further suggesting that priming oscillation might be a new and separable component in priming behavior. Therefore, we suggest that overall priming patterns are concurrently mediated through two processes, one based on classical priming models and one presumably related to the theta-band prediction waves.
Transition from PCE to NCE in masked priming but not in unmasked priming was originally suggested to represent automatic motor inhibition that suppresses partial activation initially caused by the prime (i.e., “motor self-inhibition”; Eimer and Schlaghecken, 2003). Previous studies reported reciprocal inhibitory interactions in movement-related EEG potentials after masked primes (Praamstra and Seiss, 2005), suggesting response competition taking place in motor cortex. Moreover, additional PCE occurs after the “PCE followed by NCE” pattern in masked priming behavior (Sumner and Brandwood, 2008), also consistent with this interpretation. We postulate that the observed priming oscillations are not solely based on motor priming explanations. First, we observed theta-band oscillations in unmasked priming as well, which would be inconsistent with “motor self-inhibition” because that is triggered by partial motor activation caused by masked primes only. Second, there were no correlations between priming oscillations and the classical priming effects in the masked priming task; this would not be expected if theta-band rhythms reflect reciprocal motor inhibitions initiated by masked primes. Third, recent EEG studies reported that the inhibitory EEG movement-related component was actually triggered and modulated by the mask stimulus after prime rather than being automatically initiated by the masked prime (Jaśkowski et al., 2008). Further studies using both masked and unmasked priming paradigms are needed to elucidate the neural correlates of priming oscillations.
Here, priming oscillations were found to occur at the theta-band. Theta-band rhythms are known to mediate sensory stream processing (Luo and Poeppel, 2007, 2012; Luo et al., 2010; Kayser et al., 2012; Fiebelkorn et al., 2013b), attention (Schroeder and Lakatos, 2009; Busch and VanRullen, 2010; Landau and Fries, 2012; Fiebelkorn et al., 2013a; Song et al., 2014), and memory (Lisman and Idiart, 1995; Luo et al., 2013) by cyclically modulating cortical excitability and segmenting inputs into appropriate chunks in time (Giraud and Poeppel, 2012; Kayser et al., 2012). Several recent behavioral studies also showed theta-band fluctuations in spatial- or object-based attentional performance (Landau and Fries, 2012; Fiebelkorn et al., 2013a; Song et al., 2014). Specifically, after resetting attention to 1 of 2 spatial locations, visual performance at these locations underwent a 4 Hz fluctuation. However, in the present study, instead of being distributed at multiple spatial locations as in previous studies, both primes and probes occurred at the same location throughout the experiments. Therefore, theta-band priming oscillations here cannot be accounted for by spatial sampling rhythms that were suggested in previous attentional studies. Furthermore, the results substantially extend previous findings by suggesting that the theta rhythm is not limited to spatial attention, but may reflect a more general oscillatory mechanism in multi-item attention (e.g., location, feature, perceptual prediction, etc.).
Finally, our findings addressed an important but essentially unexplored question: how the brain dynamically manages and coalesces multiple perceptual predictions to guide moment-by-moment perceptions about the external environment. Although abundant studies have demonstrated that perceptual predictions modulate sensory activities (Hupé et al., 1998; Murray et al., 2002; Summerfield et al., 2008; Alink et al., 2010; Arnal et al., 2011; Costa-Faidella et al., 2011; Wacongne et al., 2011; den Ouden et al., 2012; Kok and de Lange, 2014), most of them examined how one particular perceptual prediction affects low-level sensory responses (i.e., consistent or inconsistent with the hypothesis). To our knowledge, there are virtually no experimental data revealing the dynamic structure of the multiprediction coordination process. Our results suggest that the re-entrance process actually fluctuates over time in a theta-band rhythm and the two predictions (i.e., congruent and incongruent) descend to lower sensory areas alternately in time. Such “competition-like” relationship between the two perceptual predictions might be mediated by an oscillation-based temporal coordination mechanism in higher brain regions. Specifically, different predictions are carried by feedback signals in different phases of theta-band oscillations to achieve organized and dissociated neural representations. Alternatively, it might be also accounted for by a “mutual inhibition” interpretation in that one perceptual prediction, activated by a prime, waxes and wanes in a theta-band rhythm, which would make the other prediction to be inhibited and disinhibited accordingly. Most interestingly, such oscillation-mediated population competition mechanisms have been proposed and demonstrated in spatial attention (Buschman and Miller, 2009; Fries, 2009; Landau and Fries, 2012; Fiebelkorn et al., 2013a; Song et al., 2014), working memory (Lisman and Idiart, 1995), consciousness (Doesburg et al., 2009), task rule switching (Buschman et al., 2012), discrete nature of perception (VanRullen et al., 2007), and interactions between feedforward and top-down processing (Fontolan et al., 2014). Together, our results, in combination with accumulating evidence, support an important role for neuronal oscillation in various cognitive processes, via dynamical organization and segregation of multiple neuronal populations over time.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the Ministry of Science and Technology of China (Grant 2012 CB825500), the National Nature Science Foundation of China (Grants 31171075 and 91120019 to H.L., Grant 31300836 to Y.H., and Grant 91132302 to L.C.), and the Chinese Academy of Sciences Strategic Priority Research Program B (Grants XDB02010100 and XDB02050001). We thank Sheng He, David Poeppel, Wu Zhou, Ming Meng, Fang Fang, and Ke Zhou for critical comments; Dan Zhang for statistics and data analysis consultation; and Zhe Wang and Kun Hu for technical support.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Huan Luo, Department of Psychology, Peking University, Beijing, 100871, P.R. China. huan.luo{at}pku.edu.cn or luohuan{at}gmail.com