Abstract
An increasing number of studies show that an altered epigenetic landscape may cause impairments in regulation of learning and memory-related genes within the aged hippocampus, eventually resulting in cognitive deficits in the aged brain. One such epigenetic repressive mark is trimethylation of H3K9 (H3K9me3), which is typically implicated in gene silencing. Here, we identify, for the first time, an essential role for H3K9me3 and its histone methyl transferase (SUV39H1) in mediating hippocampal memory functions. Pharmacological inhibition of SUV39H1 using a novel and selective inhibitor decreased levels of H3K9me3 in the hippocampus of aged mice, and improved performance in the objection location memory and fear conditioning tasks and in a complex spatial environment learning task. The inhibition of SUV39H1 induced an increase in spine density of thin and stubby but not mushroom spines in the hippocampus of aged animals and increased surface GluR1 levels in hippocampal synaptosomes, a key index of spine plasticity. Furthermore, there were changes at BDNF exon I gene promoter, in concert with overall BDNF levels in the hippocampus of drug-treated animals compared with control animals. Together, these data demonstrate that SUV39H1 inhibition and the concomitant H3K9me3 downregulation mediate gene transcription in the hippocampus and reverse age-dependent deficits in hippocampal memory.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Cognitive decline is a debilitating condition associated with not only neurodegenerative diseases but also aging in general. However, effective treatments have been slow to emerge so far. In this study, we demonstrate that epigenetic regulation of key synaptic proteins may be an underlying, yet reversible, cause of this decline. Our findings suggest that histone 3 trimethylation is a probable target for pharmacological intervention that can counteract cognitive decline in the aging brain. Finally, we provide support to the hypothesis that, by manipulating the enzyme that regulates H3K9me3 (using a newly developed specific inhibitor of SUV39H1), it is possible to alter the chromatin state of subjects and restore memory and synaptic function in the aging brain.
Introduction
Learning and memory decline with age as a consequence of multiple different mechanisms, many of which target the hippocampus. A growing body of evidence suggests that epigenetic regulation of genes and proteins is fundamental to these age-related changes in the hippocampus. In the context of aging and neurodegenerative conditions, histone acetylation has been extensively studied and is unmistakably associated with facilitating learning and memory (Mai et al., 2009; Gräff et al., 2012). On the other hand, changes in histone methylation states in the brain, relative to cognitive functions, is a relatively unexplored area of research (Gupta et al., 2010; Jarome and Lubin, 2014; Morse et al., 2015). Methylation of the histone tail typically occurs at specific lysine (K) residues, such as H3K4, H3K9, H3K27, H3K36, H3K79, and H4K20, and can either activate or repress transcription (Vakoc et al., 2006). In particular, trimethylation of H3K9 (H3K9me3) is an important repressive histone mark, mainly involved in the formation and maintenance of silent heterochromatin state.
Several studies point to a pivotal role of H3K9me3 in aging, including vascular inflammation and diabetes (Sedivy et al., 2008; Villeneuve et al., 2008, 2010). More recently, in an elegant new study, Zhang et al. (2015b) also demonstrate how alterations in the heterochromatin state may drive human aging. The histone methyl transferase suppressor of variegation 3–9 homolog 1 (SUV39H1) is the principle enzyme responsible for the trimethylation of H3K9 (Stewart et al., 2005), and Zhang et al. (2015b) demonstrate how knockdown of SUV39H1 leads to a reduction of both overall H3K9me3 and induction of cellular senescence. However, to date, there has not been any study that directly addresses the effect of SUV39H1 inhibition and the corresponding downregulation of H3K9me3 on memory and cognitive function in the aging brain. Recent progress in the development of small-molecule inhibitors of methyltransferases is a powerful means to change this.
In this study, we investigated, for the first time, the role of H3K9me3 in learning and memory in aged mice treated with the SUV39H1 inhibitor ETP69, a newly developed analog of the epidithiodiketopiperazine alkaloid chaetocin A, the first reported inhibitor of SUV39H1 (Fig. 1) (Greiner et al., 2005; Cherblanc et al., 2013; Overman et al., 2014; Baumann et al., 2015). ETP69, initially developed for use in cancer treatment, shows significantly greater selectivity against a panel of 17 human histone methytransferases than chaetocin A, and exhibits no inhibitory activity toward histone acetyltransferase p300 and DNA methyltransferase DNMT1 (Overman et al., 2014). It is well accepted that hippocampal impairments result in severe deficits in spatial memory (Duva et al., 1997; Bachevalier and Nemanic, 2008). Accordingly, we used a spatial memory task (object location memory task), known to be hippocampal dependent, to evaluate the effects of H3K9me3 manipulation in aged animals. This test has been used extensively to evaluate novel compounds for their effects on cognitive deficits (Haettig et al., 2011; Intlekofer et al., 2013). In addition, we used the fear conditioning task and a spontaneous activity task, referred to as the unsupervised learning (USL) task, to extend and confirm our findings. The USL and fear conditioning tasks both measure hippocampal-dependent learning, and the USL task is known to be highly correlated with synaptic changes in the hippocampus (Cox et al., 2014), and are susceptible to age-related deficits.
Structure of ETP69 (Rac-(3S,6S,7S,8aS)-6-(benzo[d][1,3]dioxol-5-yl)-2,3,7-trimethyl-1,4-dioxohexahydro-6H-3,8a-epidithiopyrrolo[1,2-a]pyrazine-7-carbonitrile).
Then to identify the downstream regulatory mechanisms underlying memory improvements by H3K9me3 inhibition, we evaluated cellular/molecular events, including epigenetic and protein changes on BDNF and synapse growth. We identified the effect of ETP69 on hippocampal spine formation using Golgi staining and flow synaptometry, a new method that can be used to profile synapses using isolated synaptosomes (Prieto et al., 2015). Overall, our data reveal that ETP69 has procognitive benefits for the aging brain and that this effect supports a role of H3K9me3 in memory function.
Materials and Methods
Animals
Aged (18–20 months, n = 48) or young (3–4 months, n = 18) C57BL/6J male mice were group housed with food and water ad libitum and were acclimated to the vivarium for 1 week before experimental procedures. Lights were maintained on a 12:12 light/dark cycle, and all behavior testing was performed during the light phase of the cycle.
Compound
ETP69 (Rac-(3S,6S,7S,8aS)-6-(benzo[d][1,3]dioxol-5-yl)-2,3,7-trimethyl-1,4-dioxohexahydro-6H-3,8a-epidithiopyrrolo[1,2-a]pyrazine-7-carbonitrile) is a racemic analog of epidithiodiketopiperazine alkaloids, such as chaetocin A, and was prepared as described previously (Overman et al., 2014) and recrystallized from methanol. Compound was dissolved in a mixture of 50% DMSO-based saline solution. Control subjects received DMSO saline solution.
Testing
Object location memory (OLM) task.
Training and testing procedures were performed using a standard OLM protocol. Briefly, 22 aged mice and 18 young mice were handled for ∼2–3 min per day for 5 d, followed by habituation to the experimental apparatus (white rectangular open field measuring 30 × 23 × 21.5 cm) for 5 min per day for 5 consecutive days before training.
Dose–response in OLM.
On the test day, mice were given 3 min of habituation (in an empty test arena) followed by a single intraperitoneal injection of ETP69 (10 mg/kg, n = 8; or 20 mg/kg, n = 7; or vehicle, n = 7). Mice were then given a 3 min acquisition trial (with 2 similar objects placed in the arena opposite each other) 30 min after the injection of drug or vehicle, and all animals were then returned to their home cages for a 24 h intertrial interval. Twenty-four hours later, a 3 min retention test was administered, where one object was moved to a novel location and the amount of time the animals spent exploring the novel versus familiar location was recorded to evaluate the dose–response in the OLM task. Because animals that had received 10 mg/kg performed better than other groups in the OLM task, we used 10 mg/kg for the remainder of the study. This OLM paradigm has previously been shown to be subthreshold for learning (Stefanko et al., 2009; Intlekofer et al., 2013). Locomotor activity was also measured in this paradigm using the line crossings method (Snigdha et al., 2011).
Acquisition (pretraining) or consolidation (post-training).
In another test, aimed to determine the efficacy of the compound in pretraining and post-training paradigms, the same group of 22 aged animals was retested on the OLM task 3 weeks after the first test. To determine whether the drug impacted acquisition or consolidation mechanisms, mice were injected (i.p.) with 10 mg/kg compound (n = 7) or vehicle (n = 8) either right before or after acquisition phase. At 24 h later, animals were killed after retention testing, and hippocampi were removed, rapidly frozen on dry ice, and stored at −80°C until processing for trimethylation levels and BDNF assay.
Eighteen young mice were also tested in the same OLM paradigm as described above. To determine whether the drug impacted acquisition or consolidation mechanisms, the young mice were injected with the 10 mg/kg compound or vehicle (i.p., n = 6/group) either just before or right after the acquisition trial, and tested 24 h after acquisition.
USL.
A new group of 12 aged animals were used for the unsupervised learning task. Procedures were adapted from published work (Fedulov et al., 2007; Chen et al., 2010a, b). The USL behavioral apparatus consisted of a large open field divided by walls into 4 chambers, all accessible by small entrances in each dividing wall (see Fig. 5a). The animals can also access a smaller attached enclosed dark compartment by an open entrance. After 5 d of handling, animals were injected with ETP69 (10 mg/kg, i.p.) or vehicle and placed in the video-monitored USL box for 30 min, and then returned to the home cage. At 24 h later, the animals were placed in the USL box again and killed immediately afterward. Their brains were rapidly removed, hippocampus dissected rapidly, frozen on dry ice, and stored at −80°C until further processing.
Contextual fear conditioning.
Fourteen aged (18–20 months) male mice were placed in the fear-conditioning chamber and allowed to explore for 2 min before receiving one electric foot shocks (2 s, 0.2 mA). Animals were returned to the home cage 2 min after the foot shock. Twenty-four hours later, behavior in the conditioning chamber was observed for 5 min and subsequently was analyzed for freezing behavior, which was defined as the absence of all movement except for respiration.
Histone trimethylation.
Total histone was extracted from frozen hippocampi of 3 young and 3 old mice using an EpiQuik extraction kit (OP-0006–100) following the manufacturer's protocol. In brief, tissue was weighed and cut into small pieces and homogenized in 1× prelysis buffer, transferred in a 2 ml tube, and centrifuged at 10,000 × g for 1 min at 4°C. The supernatant was removed; tissue pellet was resuspended in 3 volumes of lysis buffer, incubated on ice for 30 min, and centrifuged at 12,000 × g for 5 min at 4°C. Balance-DTT buffer (0.3 volumes) was added to the supernatant, which was stored at −80°C. The protein concentration of the eluted histone was estimated using a Bradford protein detection kit (Bio-Rad) using BSA as a standard. Histone (H3K9) trimethylation analysis was performed according to the manufacturer's information (ab115064). Briefly, trimethylated histones were captured using specific antibody and detected with a labeled detection antibody, followed by a color development reagent. Absorbance was read at 450 nm, and results were calculated using a standard curve following the manufacturer's instructions.
Spine counts.
In another series of experiments, 12 aged mice (20–22 months) were injected with ETP69 (10 mg/kg; i.p) or vehicle (i.p.); and 24 h later, the animals were killed, and the brains were separated into two hemispheres. The right hemisphere was used for Golgi staining, and the left was processed for flow synaptometry. Staining was conducted according to the manufacturer's information (Golgi-Cox, Bioenna), and spine densities were calculated relative to the 3D length of the sampled segments of stained dendrites.
Flow synaptometry.
Fresh synaptosome P2 fractions were obtained from whole-mouse hippocampus using our long-standing protocol (Sandoval et al., 1978). Briefly, the fractions were obtained from the hippocampas by homogenizing tissue (1:10 w/v) in ice-cold sucrose 320 mm. All the steps were performed at 4°C; sucrose buffer, grinder, pestle, and microfuge tubes were all precooled on ice. Hippocampi were rapidly dissected form a single mouse and homogenized in 320 mm sucrose (1.5 ml) containing HEPES (10 mm) and a protease/phosphatase inhibitor mixture (Pierce), pH 7.4. Homogenization consisted of 6–8 manual strokes in a glass-Teflon grinder, clearance (between plunger and glass): 0.15–0.25 mm. The plunger was gently rotated during strokes while the grinder was kept on ice. The homogenate was centrifuged at 1200 × g for 10 min. Supernatant (S1, containing mitochondria and synaptosomes) was transferred into two clean microfuge tubes and centrifuged at 12,000 × g for 20 min. Supernatants (S2) were carefully removed using a plastic tip and vacuum. Pellets (P2, corresponding to the crude synaptosome fraction) were resuspended by gently pipetting up and down in 1.5 ml of PBS. Protein concentration was determined using the BCA assay using BSA as standard. Before immunolabeling, we adjusted all samples to the same protein concentration using PBS as diluent. Immunolabeling for flow synaptometry analysis was performed according to a method for staining of extracellular/intracellular antigens using 200 μl of synaptosomal fractions (50–100 μg protein). Antibodies included the following: GluR1 (extracellular) (Millipore, ABN241, DIL = 1:400), pAkt-ser473 (Cell Signaling Technology, 4060, DIL = 1:400), PSD95 (Millipore, MAB1598, DIL = 1:400), anti-rabbit IgG Alexa-488 and anti-mouse IgG Alexa 647 (Invitrogen, DIL = 1:400). Data were acquired using a FACS Calibur flow cytometer (BD Biosciences) equipped with argon 488 nm and helium-neon 635 nm lasers. Relative size and granularity were determined by forward (FSC) and side scatter (SSC) properties. FSC, SSC, FL1, and FL4 signals were collected using log amplification. Alexa-488 and Alexa-647 fluorochromes were detected by the FL1 and FL4 detectors, respectively. FSC-SSC plots were used to select particles matching the size of synaptosomes (0.5–3.0 μm) using calibrated beads (see Fig. 9a), as previously described (Fein et al., 2008). Identical FSC settings were used for acquiring data on bead standards and samples. Small fragments and debris were excluded by establishing a FSC-H threshold (325). A total of 10,000 size-gated particles were collected and analyzed for each sample. Analysis was performed using the CellQuest Pro software (BD Biosciences).
ELISA.
ELISA was performed using the BDNF Emax Immunoassay System (G7610, Promega) according to the manufacturer's instructions. Two sets of samples were prepared from the hippocampus of each animal, and all reactions were performed in duplicate.
In the first series of experiments, we had established that BDNF was a target of ETP69; then we used brains obtained from a second set of experiments (USL task) to detect which BDNF exons were methylated and which downstream targets of BDNF showed changes following ETP69 treatment.
ChIP for BDNF promoter regions.
After cross-linking with 1% formaldehyde, chromatin was sheared to fragments of 200–500 bp. Immunoprecipitation was realized overnight at 4°C with an antibody directed against histone H3K9me3 (Millipore). After washes, elution from beads, and reversal of the cross-link, immunoprecipitated DNA was purified and analyzed in triplicate by qRT-PCR with an internal standard curve prepared from pooled input samples. Each sample was normalized with the respective input value. Primers for bdnf promoters for exon I, IV, and VI correspond to each unique exon sequence (Aid et al., 2007; Intlekofer et al., 2013). qRT-PCR primer sets were designed using the Roche Universal Probe Library Assay Design Center and obtained from Integrated DNA Technologies. qRT-PCR was run in a Stratagene MX3005P thermocycler at 95°C for 3 min, followed by 45 cycles of 95°C for 10 s and 58°C for 15 s. Each qRT-PCR run included all samples run in triplicate and a standard curve. Data were analyzed by the 2−ΔΔCt method and expressed as fold change over control after normalizing with input samples, as described previously (Sahar et al., 2007).
Statistical analysis.
Mann–Whitney test was used as nonparametric t test for paired and unpaired data, and Student's t test were used for parametric data. One-way ANOVAs were followed by post hoc Tukey's test for mean comparisons of three or more groups; whereas two-way ANOVAs were followed by Bonferroni's post hoc test. All statistical tests and the nonlinear fit for Figure 9b were performed using GraphPad Prism 6. Data are presented as mean ± SEM. p < 0.05 was considered significant.
Results
Acute administration of ETP69 improved performance of aged mice in the OLM task
We first conducted a dose–response study to test the effects of intraperitoneally administered ETP69 in the spatial OLM task. The dose range included amounts known to be well tolerated in mice (Overman et al., 2014). Our results show that 10 mg/kg, but not 20 mg/kg, of ETP69 induced memory improvements in the OLM task in aged mice (Fig. 2b,c). This was demonstrated using a two-way ANOVA (interaction effect: F(2,19) = 10.25, p < 0.01; Fig. 2b) on the exploration times, with post hoc testing showing that the group receiving 10 mg/kg of the drug performed significantly better compared with controls (p < 0.05), and further confirmed by one-way ANOVA of the discrimination index (F(2,18) = 6.48) followed by post hoc testing (p < 0.05; Fig. 2c). The acquisition phase of the testing showed no significant interaction effect (F(2,19) = 2.21, p = 0.139; Fig. 2a) or main effect of object location (F(1,19) = 1.83, p = 0.19). However, there was a significant treatment effect (F(2,19) = 3.18, p < 0.05), with animals that had been given a higher dose of the drug (20 mg/kg) showing a decrease in total exploration time (p < 0.05). This suggests that higher doses of the H3K9me3 downregulation may impair overall exploration and possibly impair locomotor activity in aged animals. This was confirmed by measuring locomotor activity of animals (Fig. 2c). A two-way ANOVA on the locomotor activity levels showed no interaction effect (F(2,18) = 1.22, p = 0.31) or time effect (F(1,18) = 1.16, p = 0.213), but there was a significant effect of treatment (F(2,18) = 14.2, p < 0.01; Fig. 2d). Accordingly, we selected the lower dose for all further testing.
a, Mice treated with ETP69 at 10 mg/kg and 20 mg/kg (i.p.) administered 30 min before acquisition phase in the OLM showed no difference in object exploration (measured in seconds) in the acquisition phase. Data are mean ± SEM; n = 7 or 8/group. b, In the retention phase of the task (24 h after drug administration) mice treated with 10 mg/kg of ETP69 (i.p.) explored the novel object significantly (measured in seconds) more than the familiar object (**p ≤ 0.01, Bonferroni t test). Mice treated with 20 mg/kg of ETP69 performed similar to aged controls. Data are mean ± SEM; n = 7 or 8/group. c, Effect of ETP69 treatment on discrimination index in aged animals. Mice treated with 10 mg/kg of ETP69 (i.p.) showed improved discrimination index [time spent at novel object location − time spent at familiar object location]/[total time exploring both objects × 100] compared with aged controls. *p < 0.05 (Bonferroni t test). Data are mean ± SEM; n = 7 or 8/group. d, Effect of ETP69 treatment on locomotor activity (measure by line crossings) in aged animals. Mice treated with 20 mg/kg (but not 10 mg/kg) of the compound showed significant reduction in locomotor activity compared with aged control. **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001 (Bonferroni t test). Data are mean ± SEM; n = 7 or 8/group.
Next, we sought to determine whether ETP69 treatment differentially affects performance accuracy in the OLM task in a pre-trial versus a post-trial administration paradigm. The pre-trial versus post-trial paradigms for the OLM task allow for testing of two distinct components of memory formation. Specifically, the pre-training drug administration evaluates acquisition and recall of memory, whereas the post-training drug administration targets memory consolidation and recall. Thus, we administered the drug either 30 min before or immediately after the acquisition phase of the OLM test. We found that 10 mg/kg (i.p.) of the drug, administered either 30 min before acquisition phase or immediately after acquisition, improved performance in the OLM task (interaction effect: F(2,19) = 9.81, p < 0.01; Fig. 3b). Further post hoc testing showed a significant effect in both the pretrial and post-trial groups (p < 0.001). This was also confirmed by one-way ANOVA (F(2,19) = 3.67) of the discrimination index (p < 0.01 for pre-trial and p < 0.05 for post trial; Fig. 3c) The acquisition phase of the testing showed no significant group differences (interaction: F(2,19) = 0.26, p = 0.773; main effect of drug: F(2,19) = 0.01, p = 0.982; main effect of object location: F(1,19) = 0.01, p = 0.901; Figure 3a).
a, Effect of the drug at 10 mg/kg (i.p.) administered either 30 min before acquisition phase (pre-trial) or immediately after acquisition (post-trial) in the object location memory task in aged mice. No difference in object exploration (measured in seconds) in the acquisition phase. Data are mean ± SEM. Pre-trial, n = 6/group; post-trial, n = 8/group. b, In the retention phase of the task (24 h ± 30 min, after drug administration), mice treated with 10 mg/kg of ETP69 (i.p.) explored the novel location (measured in seconds) significantly more than the familiar location. ***p < 0.001 (Bonferroni t test). Data are mean ± SEM. Pre-trial, n = 6/group; post-trial, n = 8/group. c, Effect of ETP69 treatment on discrimination index in aged animals either 30 min before acquisition phase (pre-trial) or immediately after acquisition (post-trial). Mice treated with 10 mg/kg of ETP69 (i.p.) showed improved discrimination index [time spent at novel object location − time spent at familiar object location]/[total time exploring both objects × 100] compared with aged control in both conditions. *p < 0.05 (Bonferroni t test). **p < 0.01 (Bonferroni t test). Data are mean ± SEM; n = 7 or 8/group.
To test the hypothesis that H3K9me3 inhibition positively impacts memory in young and aged mice, we extended the behavioral testing to young mice. Unlike in aged animals, administration of ETP69 did not improve OLM performance in young mice. The 10 mg/kg (i.p.) of the drug, administered either 30 min before acquisition phase (pre-trial) or immediately after acquisition (post-trial), did not show any statistical differences compared with young controls (interaction effect: F(2,15) = 0.32, p = 0.728; main effect of drug: F(2,15) = 2.21, p = 0.143; main effect of object location: F(2,15) = 6.02, p < 0.05; Figure 4b). This was also confirmed by one-way ANOVA (F(2,17) = 0.035) of the discrimination index (Fig. 4c). The acquisition phase of the testing also showed significant group differences (interaction: F(2,15) = 0.44, p = 0.652; main effect of drug: F(2,15) = 25.01, p < 0.01) in the pre-trial versus post-trial groups, but no difference between exploration of Position 1 versus Position 2 (main effect of object location: F(2,15) = 1.56, p = 0.229; Fig. 4a). This suggests that H3K9me3 manipulation may have quite different effects in the young versus aged brain.
a, Effect of the drug at 10 mg/kg (i.p.) administered either 30 min before acquisition phase (pre-trial) or immediately after acquisition (post-trial) in the object location memory task in young mice. No difference in object exploration (measured in seconds) in the acquisition phase. Data are mean ± SEM; n = 6/group. b, In the retention phase of the task (24 h ± 30 min, after drug administration), drug-treated young animals did not show a preference for novel location (measured in seconds), performing similar to the young controls. Data are mean ± SEM; n = 6/group. c, Effect of ETP69 treatment on discrimination index in young animals. Mice treated with 10 mg/kg of ETP69 (i.p.) showed no improvement in discrimination index [time spent at novel object location − time spent at familiar object location]/[total time exploring both objects × 100] compared with age-matched controls. Data are mean ± SEM; n = 6/group.
ETP69 administration improves performance in the USL task in aged animals
To evaluate whether H3K9me3 is implicated in measures of spatial learning and to evaluate molecular endpoints, we conducted a second series of experiments, with the USL task being the behavioral endpoint. The USL task is a simple behavioral paradigm in which mice are allowed to freely explore a four-compartment environment for a specified period of time. Reduction in overall activity and exploration of the test arena over this time is considered a measure of learning and short-term memory. If this occurs at similar rates in the two groups, it indicates no significant differences in short-term learning and the locomotor pattern of animals during this task is highly correlated with synaptic changes in the hippocampus (Cox et al., 2014).
During the test, well-handled mice were placed in the four-compartment test box and their movements were monitored for 30 min on day 1 and then again 24 h later on day 2. We found that animals treated with ETP69 (10 mg/kg, i.p.) showed a significant decrease in habituation/exploration (as measured by distance traveled) 24 h after treatment but not in the first 30 min on day 1 (Fig. 5b). One-way ANOVA revealed significant group differences (F(3,19) = 4.05, p < 0.05), and post hoc testing confirmed the difference between performance on day 1 and day 2 in ETP69-treated animals (p < 0.05) but not in the age-matched control group (Fig. 5c). The 30 min of exploration on day 1 is sufficient to induce some long-term memory of the test arena in the mice and can be measured on day 2 of testing. We found a difference in activity levels on day 2 and interpret this as improved retention and recall in ETP69-treated animals, even when learning remains the same between groups.
a, The unsupervised learning test arena is a large open field divided by walls into 4 chambers, all accessible by small entrances in each dividing wall, and a black box into which mice can escape. b, Aged mice treated with ETP69 (10 mg/kg, i.p.) showed a significant decrease in habituation/exploration (as measured by distance traveled) on day 2 compared with day 1. Aged control mice spent equal time(s) exploring the test arena on both day 1 and day 2. Habituation was measured over 30 min, and data shown are in 5 min bins. *p < 0.05, significant difference between habituation/exploration on day 1 and day 2 at the specified time points (Tukey's t test). n = 5 or 6/group. c, Graph represents the total distance traveled over 30 min on day 1 and day 2 following administration of ETP69 (10 mg/kg, i.p) or vehicle. *p < 0.05, significant decrease in total distance traveled in 30 min between day 1 and day 2 (Tukey's t test). Data are mean ± SEM; n = 5 or 6/group.
ETP69 administration improves performance in the contextual fear conditioning task in aged animals
To confirm our findings from the OLM and USL tasks, we used the contextual fear conditioning to test the effect of H3K9me3 downregulation on hippocampal-dependent learning and memory. We found that aged animals (n = 7/group) that had been treated with ETP69 showed improved performance on the task compared with aged controls (p < 0.01; Fig. 6). Overall, these behavioral data suggest that ETP69 administration improves hippocampal-dependent learning and memory over a battery of tests.
Aged mice treated with ETP69 (10 mg/kg, i.p.) showed improved performance when contextual fear conditioning was assessed. *p = 0.01 (Bonferroni t test). Data are mean ± SEM; n = 7 per group.
ETP69 administration decreases levels of H3K9me3 in the hippocampus of aged mice
We first compared H3K9me3 levels in the hippocampus and cerebellum of young versus old mice and found significantly higher level of H3K9me3 in aged mice in the hippocampus (Fig. 7a; p < 0.05) but not in the cerebellum (Fig. 7b). The OLM and USL are both hippocampal-specific, and we found a difference in H3K9me3 levels in the hippocampus of young versus old mice. Accordingly, we next tested the total level of H3K9me3 in the hippocampus following drug treatment in hippocampal tissue samples from the cohort of animals described above in the OLM task. Our results showed a significant effect of the drug in histone extracts from the hippocampus of the drug-treated animals. Statistical analysis showed decreases in H3K9me3 levels in aged animals treated with ETP69 (p < 0.05, Fig. 7c) but not in young animals (Fig. 7d).
Trimethylation levels of H3K9 in (a) hippocampal extracts young versus aged mice and (b) cerebellum extracts of young versus aged mice. n = 3/group. *p < 0.05 (Student's unpaired t test). Trimethylation levels of H3K9 in the hippocampus of (c) aged mice and (d) young mice, which received 10 mg/kg of ETP69 (i.p.) or vehicle, 24 h after drug administration. n = 7/group. *p < 0.05 (Student's unpaired t test).
In addition, we also evaluated the global levels of H3K9me2 in aged mice, which has already been linked to learning and memory deficits (Balemans et al., 2013), but did not observe any significant effects of ETP69 treatment in aged mice (p = 0.95, unpaired t test, data not shown).
H3K9me3 inhibition increases spine density and GluR1 receptor surface expression in hippocampal synaptosomes
It has been reported that novel memory formation can be associated with a transient increase in spine density in the hippocampus (Restivo et al., 2009). In particular, improved performance in the OLM (Babayan et al., 2012) and USL (Cox et al., 2014) tasks is associated with structural and/or functional changes at excitatory glutamatergic synapses Thus, we examined whether pharmacological inhibition of the histone methyltransferase (SUV39H1) was sufficient to induce an increase in dendritic spine formation in the hippocampus.
Specifically, using Golgi-stained sections, we counted and classified spines on CA1 pyramidal neurons. Dendrite segments from neurons in CA1 of the hippocampus were imaged by confocal microscopy, and a blinded investigator counted and classified dendritic spines according to their morphology. Spines were manually counted and classified as thin, mushroom, or stubby, according to previously described criteria (Harris et al., 1992).
For each animal, at least 5 pyramidal neurons and 200 μm of total dendrite length and 250–500 spines were analyzed (n = 6 animals per group). All of the neurons were chosen from the dorsal part of the CA1 hippocampal region (according to its essential role in spatial memory) and were completely stained along basal and apical dendrites (Fig. 8b). Segregation analysis based on anatomically observed differences was done to differentiate basal from apical dendrites in the same neuron. On average, ETP69-treated mice had 45% more total number of spines per length of apical dendrite (microns) compared with controls Analysis of apical spine density by morphology revealed a significantly higher density of thin (p < 0.001, Student's t test) and stubby spines (p < 0.05, Student's t test), in ETP69-treated mice compared with control mice (Fig. 8a). There was no difference in the numbers of mushroom spines/length of dendrite measured (Fig. 8a). In contrast, spines on basal dendrites showed no differences between groups. t test comparisons between drug- and vehicle-treated groups showed the following p values for thin spines is 0.82, for stubby spines is 0.83, and for mushroom spines is 0.95 (data not graphed).
a, Effect of ETP69 treatment on spine counts (thin, mushroom, and stubby) in the CA1 region of the hippocampus *p < 0.05, significantly different compared with age-matched controls (Student's unpaired t test). **p < 0.01, significantly different compared with age-matched controls (Student's unpaired t test). b, Representative images showing effect of ETP69 treatment on spines in the CA1 of the hippocampus. Top, Hippocampal neurons from control animals. Bottom, Spines observed in ETP69-treated animals.
Next, we studied the influence of ETP69 on synapses using flow synaptometry (Prieto et al., 2015), an innovative approach to molecularly characterize isolated synaptosomes (presynaptic terminals attached to postsynaptic dendritic spines) (Wilhelm et al., 2014). Specifically, we quantified the levels of PSD95 (scaffolding protein) and GluR1 (subunit of AMPA receptors), as the activity-dependent incorporation of AMPA receptor at PSD-enriched regions promotes spine growth and increases synaptic transmission efficiency (Zhang et al., 2015a). After mice treatment, hippocampal synaptosome fractions were immunostained and analyzed by flow synaptometry, which first identifies synaptosomes by size using calibrated beads, as previously described (Prieto et al., 2015). We have previously shown that the subset of particles between 0.5 and 3.0 μm is highly enriched in synaptosomes, as indicated by the high levels of synaptophysin and PSD95 in most (>70%) size-gated particles (Prieto et al., 2015). Consistent with the ETP69-induced increase in spine numbers (Fig. 8a), our analysis on size-gated synaptosomes revealed that ETP69 increases the proportion of particles with detectable levels of PSD95 (p < 0.05; Fig. 9c), thus reflecting an increase in the number of synapses. No changes were observed in the PSD95 levels per particle, as determined by the mean fluorescence of intensity. Next, to test whether ETP69 increases the relative levels of spines expressing surface GluR1, we quantified the proportion of size-gated synaptosomes coexpressing GluR1 at surface and PSD95 intracellularly by double labeling combining extracelullar (no permeabilization) and intracellular immunodetection. We found an increase in the percentage of GluR1-PSD95 double-positive (GluR1+PSD95+) events in ETP69-treated mice, compared with vehicle-treated controls (p = 0.05; Fig. 9e).
a, Representative density plot showing size-gated synaptosomes. b, Representative histogram showing an increase in the PSD95-positive subpopulation of synaptosomes following ETP69 treatment. c, Percentage of PSD95-positive events increases following ETP69 treatment in synaptosomes obtained from aged mice. *p < 0.05. d, Representative density plots showing surface GluR1 and PSD95 expression in size-gated synaptosomes. e, GluR1 and PSD95 double-positive events (top right quadrant) increase following ETP69 treatment in synaptosomes obtained from aged mice (p = 0.05, aged-treated vs aged control mice). f, Representative density plots showing pAkt and PSD95 detection in size-gated synaptosomes. pAkt and PSD95 double-positive events (top right quadrant) increase following ETP69 treatment. g, Percentage of pAkt and PSD95 double-positive events increases following ETP69 treatment in synaptosomes obtained from aged mice (*p < 0.05 aged-treated vs aged control mice).
Synaptic plasticity depends on intracellular signaling and trafficking. In particular, the activation of the serine/threonine kinase Akt is an essential step for the activity-dependent transport of PSD95 to dendrites after NMDA receptor activation (Yoshii and Constantine-Paton, 2010). Therefore, we next analyzed the effects of ETP69 on Akt activation (phosphorylation at Serine-473, pAkt) at the synapse by flow synaptometry. According to the increased proportion of PSD95+GluR1+ events after ETP69 treatment, SUV39H1 inhibition by ETP69 also increased the proportion of pAkt+PSD95+ events, relative to samples from vehicle-treated mice (p < 0.05; Fig. 9g). Overall, these results suggest that H3K9me3 inhibition acts, at least in part, by positively modulating molecular processes that promote spine generation and/or stability.
ETP69 increases H3K9me3 levels at BDNF promoter and BDNF protein levels in hippocampus of aged mice
It stands to reason that, whether treatment with SUV39H1 inhibitor stimulates spine formation in the hippocampus, it should also produce gene-specific changes in distinct signaling cascades that can serve learning and memory. In the adult brain, one such molecule, which plays a major role on synapse formation and plasticity, is BDNF (Park and Poo, 2013). Thus, we tested the effect of H3K9me3 downregulation on BDNF protein levels, which is critical for consolidation of hippocampal-dependent learning and memory (Intlekofer et al., 2013). The bdnf gene is composed of several noncoding exons, each one regulated by its own promoter and responding to different stimuli (Aid et al., 2007). We therefore evaluated whether ETP69 treatment decreases trimethylation of H3K9 at bdnf Promoters I, IV, and VI. A significant effect of treatment was found for H3K9me3 at bdnf I (p = 0.05; Fig. 10b), but not bdnf IV or VI. Therefore, exon I appears to be the main contributor to the differential regulation of pan-BDNF levels observed following acute ETP69 treatment. Importantly, ETP69-induced epigenetic changes at BDNF promoter were associated with an increase of BDNF protein levels in hippocampus. We found significantly elevated levels of BDNF in the hippocampus of ETP69-treated mice relative to controls (F(2,16) = 8.09, p < 0.01; Fig. 10a).
a, BDNF protein levels in mice treated with 10 mg/kg of ETP69 (i.p.) either 30 min before or right after the acquisition phase in the OLM task are increased in the hippocampus compared with aged controls. **p < 0.01, aged-treated compared with aged control animals (Dunnett's t test). n = 6/group. b, H3K9me3 levels at Promoter I of BDNF was significantly lower in ETP69-treated animals compared with controls. p = 0.056 (Mann–Whitney t test). n = 7 for control; n = 9 for ETP69-treated animals.
Discussion
In this study, we tested the potential of acute in vivo SUV39H1 inhibition and the consequent H3K9me3 downregulation to attenuate learning and memory deficits in aging. We further evaluated potential mechanisms that may contribute to cognitive benefits observed following H3K9me3 manipulation. Our findings show that performance of aged animals was improved in the object location memory task and the unsupervised learning task following SUV39H1 inhibition. This corresponded with an increase in dendritic spine density of pyramidal neurons and an increase in active synapses expressing GluR1-containing AMPA receptors in the hippocampus, along with increased levels of pAkt in synaptosomal extracts obtained from the hippocampi of these animals. Levels of BDNF were also upregulated in the hippocampus of animals, which had been administered the SUV39H1 inhibitor. These results, therefore, provide support for the amelioration of cognitive deficits and suggest that SUV39H1 inhibition and likely the resulting H3K9me3 downregulation trigger a cascade of events involving BDNF, spine remodeling, and growth.
To the best of our knowledge, there have been no studies that have evaluated the effect of H3K9me3 inhibition on learning and/or memory function. Here, we present the first evidence that decreased H3K9me3 in the hippocampus improves spatial memory in aged mice but not in young mice. This is an important finding in that histone deacetylase inhibition, which is a well-characterized epigenetic mechanism underlying synaptic plasticity and memory, is known to improve performance in even young animals (Stefanko et al., 2009). In our experiments in both pretraining and post-training paradigms of the OLM task, ETP69-treated aged (but not young) animals performed equally well during testing, 24 h after the acquisition trial. This led us to believe that acute treatment with ETP69 was inducing a hippocampus-specific change in H3K9me3 levels in aged animals. Our data from the USL task confirmed that ETP69-treated animals were showing improved retention and recall at 24 h but not at 30 min after drug treatment, when learning remained the same between groups. Repressors and cofactors recruited by H3K9me3 include histone deacetylases and heterochromatin protein-1α. Indeed, heterochromatin protein-1α associates directly with SUV39H1 and leads to a self-sustaining repressive cycle (Lund and van Lohuizen, 2004) and may require >30 min to show any behavioral manifestations. Thus, our findings are consistent with the hypothesis that changes at the molecular (and hence behavioral levels) are not rapidly engaged but need time to evolve the mechanism supporting improved retention and recall.
Transient increases in spine density have been associated with improved learning and memory, particularly in the hippocampus of aged mice (deToledo-Morrell et al., 1988; von Bohlen und Halbach et al., 2006), but the effect of H3K9me3 manipulation on synaptic function and spines is currently unexplored. Here we report that ETP69 increased both thin and stubby spine count in the CA1 region of ETP69-treated animals. Thin spines are flexible and critical for formation of new synapses, which makes them well suited for facilitating acute improvements in cognition observed with ETP69 treatment. Our data suggest that the drug engages mechanisms that support spine plasticity and growth. We also found increases in levels of both PSD+ synaptosomes and the PSD95+ synaptosomes expressing surface GluR1 in synaptic terminals Previously, it has been demonstrated that remodeling spines requires incorporation of AMPA receptor at PSD-enriched regions (Park et al., 2004). It is also known that synaptic connections can be strengthened by addition of AMPA receptor to synapses (Lee and Kirkwood, 2011; Huganir and Nicoll, 2013). Thus, our data support the hypothesis that H3K9me3 mediates changes to synaptic signaling in the hippocampus leading to improved cognitive function in aging.
BDNF is a key molecule serving synaptic plasticity and neuronal activity. We have previously shown that histone deacetylase inhibition in the aged brain improves spatial memory in a BDNF-dependent manner (Intlekofer et al., 2013). Because histone deacetylase is one of the cofactors recruited by H3K9me3, we tested the role of SUV39H1 inhibition on BDNF protein levels in the aged brain. Our data show that H3K9me3 modulates learning and memory, and it is likely that this effect is mediated by a BDNF-dependent mechanism. Our results suggest that it is primarily Promoter I that drives BDNF upregulation following ETP69 treatment in aged mice. It appears likely that H3K9me3 inhibition selectively participates in regulation of BDNF Promoter I, but the downstream result of this selectivity remains unknown. We would also like to point out that systemic drug treatment (as in this study) will reduce H3K9me3-mediated transcriptional repression across multiple genes, not only BDNF but likely transcripts that support or suppress plasticity. Further studies are warranted to evaluate these regulatory mechanisms.
In conclusion, this study provides a first look at the cascade of possibilities regulated either directly or indirectly by H3K9me3 downregulation. It is possible that activation of certain molecular pathway following removal of the H3K9me3 repressive mark activates hippocampal-memory pathway(s), which in turn may generate additional transcription and translation required for the maintenance of improved memory function in aged animals. Finally, this study provides support to the hypothesis that, by manipulating the enzyme that regulates histone methylation, it is possible to alter the chromatin state of subjects and restore memory function in the aging brain.
Footnotes
This work was supported in part by National Institute of Aging Grants AG012694-16, AG000538, and AG034667. A.P.D. was supported by the German Academic Exchange Service. B.M.L. was supported by National Cancer Institute 1F32CA180741 postdoctoral fellowship.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Shikha Snigdha, University of California–Irvine, Irvine, CA 92697. ssnigdha{at}uci.edu