Abstract
Disruption of iron homeostasis as a consequence of aging is thought to cause iron levels to increase, potentially promoting oxidative cellular damage. Therefore, understanding how this process evolves through the lifespan could offer insights into both the aging process and the development of aging-related neurodegenerative brain diseases. This work aimed to map, in vivo for the first time with an unbiased whole-brain approach, age-related iron changes using quantitative susceptibility mapping (QSM)—a new postprocessed MRI contrast mechanism. To this end, a full QSM standardization routine was devised and a cohort of N = 116 healthy adults (20–79 years of age) was studied. The whole-brain and ROI analyses confirmed that the propensity of brain cells to accumulate excessive iron as a function of aging largely depends on their exact anatomical location. Whereas only patchy signs of iron scavenging were observed in white matter, strong, bilateral, and confluent QSM–age associations were identified in several deep-brain nuclei—chiefly the striatum and midbrain—and across motor, premotor, posterior insular, superior prefrontal, and cerebellar cortices. The validity of QSM as a suitable in vivo imaging technique with which to monitor iron dysregulation in the human brain was demonstrated by confirming age-related increases in several subcortical nuclei that are known to accumulate iron with age. The study indicated that, in addition to these structures, there is a predilection for iron accumulation in the frontal lobes, which when combined with the subcortical findings, suggests that iron accumulation with age predominantly affects brain regions concerned with motor/output functions.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT This study used a whole-brain imaging approach known as quantitative susceptibility mapping (QSM) to provide a novel insight into iron accumulation in the brain across the adult lifespan. Validity of the method was demonstrated by showing concordance with ROI analysis and prior knowledge of iron accumulation in subcortical nuclei. We discovered that, beyond these regions, there is extensive involvement of the frontal lobes that has been missed by past ROI analyses. Broadly speaking, therefore, the motor system selectively accumulates iron with age. The results offer insights into the aging process, but also offer a new approach to studying the role of iron dysregulation in the evolution of age-related neurodegenerative diseases.
Introduction
Nonheme iron is an essential cofactor for neuronal oxidative metabolism and in the biosynthesis of DNA, myelin and neurotransmitters (Ward et al., 2014). With aging, iron dysregulation occurs (Daugherty et al., 2015), resulting in a chain of events that, although not fully elucidated, appear to be largely deleterious: for example, insidious damage to the blood–brain barrier (Farrall and Wardlaw, 2009) and excessive free radical generation inducing oxidative stress (Poon et al., 2004) are mechanisms thought to be associated with age- and disease-related iron accumulation. In addition, it is also thought that microglial activation stimulates iron uptake from damaged cells and downregulates export to exacerbate iron imbalance (Rathore et al., 2012), thus compromising microglial neuroprotection (Streit et al., 2008) and increasing neurotoxicity (Block et al., 2007).
Previous studies using either postmortem material or in vivo iron-sensitive MRI techniques have shown that iron concentration is unevenly distributed in the brain, with areas such as the basal ganglia showing the highest levels (Morris et al., 1992; Haacke et al., 2005). It has also been demonstrated that cerebral iron levels increase with age (Hallgren and Sourander, 1958; Bartzokis et al., 2007), which, in turn, has been linked to cognitive impairment (Penke et al., 2012; Daugherty et al., 2015; Ghadery et al., 2015) and motor system degeneration (Spatz, 1922). Therefore, with age being the strongest risk factor for many neurodegenerative diseases, it has been proposed that iron overload might be a marker or even predictor of neurodegenerative pathology (Ward et al., 2014). This has been partially corroborated by a number of postmortem and in vivo human studies that have identified abnormal iron concentrations in neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer's disease (AD) (Cornett et al., 1998; Bartzokis et al., 2000) and Parkinson's disease (PD) (Sofic et al., 1991; Bartzokis et al., 1999). Although such findings do not yet clarify whether pathological iron accumulation is a cause or consequence in the neurodegenerative cascade, they reinforce the notion that monitoring the spatial distribution and temporal dynamics of iron deposition in the aging brain may inform understanding of successful aging and neurodegenerative disease (Koeppen, 1995).
Several MRI techniques are sensitized to measure brain iron; however, in practice, these have not been widely used for technical or logistical reasons. For example, field-dependent transverse relaxation rate (R2) increase (FDRI) (Bartzokis et al., 1993), the gold standard in this context, requires multiple MRI field strength measurements. R2 and R2* (Ordidge et al., 1994) require multiecho scans that are prone to subject motion and other errors, although, with current technological advances, this is becoming less problematic. Finally, susceptibility-weighted imaging (SWI) (Haacke et al., 2004) yields nonlocal measures that are difficult to interpret in biological terms (Pfefferbaum et al., 2009). To circumvent such limitations, a new contrast referred to as quantitative susceptibility mapping (QSM) has been proposed (de Rochefort et al., 2008; Kressler et al., 2010). QSM can measure magnetic susceptibility from MRI scans using standard commercial scanners (Haacke et al., 2015; Wang and Liu, 2015). QSM results are concordant with postmortem iron measurements (Zheng et al., 2013; Sun et al., 2015) and other iron-sensitive MRI techniques (Bilgic et al., 2012; Poynton et al., 2015) and have demonstrated reproducible results (Lin et al., 2015). To date, studies of iron accumulation in healthy aging, whether postmortem or MRI based, have been mostly limited to ROI analysis. Although, as shown in a previous proof-of-concept QSM study in AD (Acosta-Cabronero et al., 2013) and with a quantitative MRI protocol in aging (Callaghan et al., 2014), whole-brain analysis is now feasible, thus offering the potential to uncover relationships that would be missed with limited ROI sampling. This study presents an unbiased whole-brain mapping of iron deposition across the adult lifespan as measured by the new iron-sensitive QSM contrast.
Materials and Methods
Study subjects.
Healthy volunteers (N = 116) ranging in age from 20 to 79 years (mean: 54 ± 19) and comprising 60 males and 56 age-matched females participated in the study. To further explore age dependencies, for some analyses, the cohort was split at a cutoff age of 55 years into young (n = 48, age: 33 ± 10; range, 20–53 years) and elderly (n = 68, age: 69 ± 5; range, 59–79) groups.
Subjects aged 50 years and older were screened to exclude neurological or major psychiatric illness; this included a brief neuropsychological assessment comprising mini-mental state examination, forward and backward digit span, copy and 30 min delayed recall of the Rey complex figure, phonemic and semantic category fluencies, and a 15-item Boston naming test. The complete cohort consisted of N = 121 healthy subjects; before analysis, n = 2 were excluded due to cognitive impairment (both for scoring worse than 1.5 σ below the age-adjusted control mean for recall of the Rey figure).
T2-weighted images and susceptibility maps (see next section) were also inspected visually to exclude brain abnormalities. Vascular pathology was assessed on T2-weighted MRI by applying the Fazekas scale (Fazekas et al., 1987) with deep or periventricular scores of ≥2 for either being an exclusion (n = 2 subjects were excluded on this criteria for periventricular hyperintensities). In addition, one more subject was excluded due to a severe pallidal calcification, which was visible both on QSM and T1-weighted images. Mild lesions (small calcium deposits), however, were cataloged as a normal occurrence in healthy aging. No subject had evidence of microhemorrhages. In addition, data on vascular risk factors were collected on the cohort, namely history of hypertension, heart attack, and diabetes, as well as height and weight. Eighteen percent of participants had been diagnosed with hypertension (30% of all subjects aged 60 years and over), 4% had a history of diabetes, 3% were obese (body-mass index >30.0), and one subject reported having a previous heart attack.
Imaging protocol.
MRI measurements were performed on a Siemens Verio 3 Tesla system with a standard Siemens 32-channel phased-array head coil for reception.
Susceptibility-weighted data were collected as complex MRI signals from each receiver channel. This is possible in Siemens systems enabling “save uncombined” under the “system” task card of a SWI sequence; in this study, a product (syngo VB19; Siemens Healthcare) 3D-flow compensated, spoiled-gradient-recalled echo acquisition (Haacke et al., 2009) with slab-selective excitation. The excitation flip angle was set to 17°; echo time 20 ms; receiver bandwidth 100 Hz per pixel; and repetition time 28 ms. Matrix size was 256 × 224 (right-left phase encoding direction); 80 straight-axial slices were acquired for whole-brain coverage plus oversampling of 16 additional slices to reduce aliasing and 0.4 mm interslice spacing to minimize cross talk interference; voxel resolution was 1 × 1×2 mm3; and GRAPPA (Griswold et al., 2002) was enabled with an acceleration factor of 2 and 24 reference lines, giving a scan time of 5:32 min.
T1-weighted 3D-MPRAGE anatomical images (Mugler and Brookeman, 1990) were also collected to resolve independently the underlying brain anatomy. For consistency with the SWI scan and to aid coregistration, slices were prescribed in straight sagittal orientation. The following acquisition parameters were used: inversion time was set to 1100 ms, flip angle was 7°, time to echo was 4.37 ms, receiver bandwidth was 140 Hz/pixel, echo spacing was 11.1 ms, and repetition time was 2500 ms. The 3D matrix dimensions were 256 × 256 × 192 (0.5 mm interslice gap), 7/8 partial Fourier, and 1 × 1×1 mm3 voxel size. GRAPPA was also enabled with acceleration factor of 2 and 24 reference lines.
T2-weighted turbo spin echo images were also acquired in the same scanning session for all subjects. To ease visual inspection, the field of view was aligned at acquisition to the anterior commissure–posterior commissure line. The prescribed scan parameters were as follows: flip angle 150°, echo time 96 ms, receiver bandwidth 220 Hz/pixel, turbo factor 18, 13 echo trains per slice with echo spacing set to 9.64 ms, and repetition time of 8160 ms. The in-plane matrix was 320 × 320 (resolution: 0.7 × 0.7 mm2), with 45 axial slices enabling whole-brain coverage (thickness: 3 mm; gap: 0.9 mm). The GRAPPA factor was set to 2 with 51 reference lines.
A thin pillow was placed on the base of the coil surrounding the sides and the back of the head to minimize motion and increase intersubject reproducibility in positioning. It should also be noted that these are relatively fast scans, which, in addition to the use of the pillow, helped to minimize motion-related artifacts.
QSM reconstruction.
Multichannel complex data were combined for optimal phase reconstruction using a robust adaptive algorithm (Walsh et al., 2000) extended for automatic reference channel selection (i.e., that with the greatest fifth percentile of signal magnitude within a brain mask). Brain masks were calculated using the BET2 algorithm (Smith, 2002) in FSL version 5.0 (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk) with fractional threshold set to 0.1. Combined phase images were then unwrapped with a continuous Laplacian approach (Schofield and Zhu, 2003); however, please note that it has been found recently that such a method may induce some errors in the vicinity of vasculature (Haacke et al., 2015). After unwrapping, the local field was revealed through global extraction of background effects; this was estimated with the SHARP filtering method (Schweser et al., 2011) using a sphere radius of 5 mm. Finally, to condition the susceptibility restoration problem, an iterative, morphology-enabled, nonlinear dipole inversion (MEDIN) solver with dynamic noise-weighting reduction (MERIT) was used (Liu et al., 2013). MEDIN assumes that magnetic susceptibility sources are piecewise-constant compartments regularised by the ℓ1 norm of a total variation operation over the morphology-constrained solution, which “smooths” an otherwise data-consistent solution. The consistency term is regularized to suppress streaking artifacts and noise amplification arising from the undetermined terms of the magnetic dipole kernel, but it is also vulnerable to phase noise nonuniformities; therefore, it was weighted by an estimate of the inverse of the phase noise variance, that is, the signal magnitude-to-noise ratio (SNR) (Conturo and Smith, 1990). SNR maps were calculated as the magnitude adjusted for noise bias (Gudbjartsson and Patz, 1995) divided by the SD of the real component of the complex signal across eight 14 × 14 × 14 mm3 background regions located at the corners of the field of view (excluding the outermost voxels in all directions). The QSM inversion algorithm was set to recalculate a new set of initial conditions every 500 conjugate gradient iterations and was terminated when the normalized residual ratio fell below 0.01. There is a trade-off element between data consistency and smoothing that can be controlled via a free regularization parameter, the Lagrange multiplier. The optimum multiplier was set to match the norm of the consistency residual with an estimate of the measurement noise (Morozov, 1966). The application of the discrepancy (noise-matching) principle yielded largely concordant optimal parameters on five randomly selected datasets (in λ = 500 steps), the median of which was used throughout this study (λ = 1000).
Raw QSMs are usually normalized (by direct subtraction) to a reference value to correct for the elimination of direct current offsets on dipole kernel operations. To date, however, the relative magnitude of such adjustment is unknown and most approaches for selecting a suitable reference region are unchallenged. In this study, we contrasted results from raw (non-normalized) QSM data with those from data that were normalized using a semiautomated method for reference selection. The reference region was manually traced on the distribution of the first percentile of a relative variance map (relative variance <0.005) from which a fairly confluent posterior white matter region (far from any gray matter boundary) emerged.
QSM spatial standardization.
Radio-frequency bias corrected (N4-ITK) (Tustison et al., 2010) MPRAGE images were standardized (to a studywise space) by way of a parallel routine for template calculation based on the diffeomorphic Greedy-SyN transformation model (Avants et al., 2008) in ANTs version 2.1 (http://stnava.github.io/ANTs). Simultaneous coregistrations were driven by cross-correlation minimizations performed over three resolutions, with a maximum of 90 iterations at the coarsest level, 30 at the next coarsest, and 90 at full resolution; template update step size was set to 0.1 mm. Four rigid-then-affine iterations were followed by six full runs of the above nonlinear multiresolution routine to ensure stable convergence. Subsequently, bias-corrected SNR maps for each susceptibility-weighted dataset were affine coregistered using ANTs to their corresponding MPRAGE volume. QSM spatial standardization was achieved through the warp composition of the above transformations and third-order b-spline interpolation.
Figure 1 summarizes the processing pipeline proposed for QSM standardization and analysis.
Coregistration performance.
The following analyses strongly rely on the performance of the coregistration routine because warping inaccuracies due to anatomical heterogeneity, geometric distortions, brain atrophy, and so on could bias measurements that are extracted directly from template space. Such errors, however, can be well characterized by the spatial distribution of high, relative QSM variances. The nature of QSM is well suited to measuring registration errors and other imperfections using such a procedure because susceptibility gradients are strong—sign reversal in some cases at the interface between iron-rich structures and white matter or CSF and between deoxygenated blood and adjacent parenchymal tissue—leading to spurious measurement dispersion when misregistration occurs.
Whole-brain QSM analyses.
Although, to date, a number of studies have adopted an exponential model to describe the relationship between iron deposition and age (Hallgren and Sourander, 1958; Li et al., 2014), this appears only suitable for describing behaviors across the entire lifespan. In this study, however, we were not concerned with the fast iron increase during early human development, so we simplified the model to a linear approximation. Age dependencies were thus estimated with a whole-brain linear regression using Randomize version 2.9 (Winkler et al., 2014) and threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE) (Smith and Nichols, 2009) in FSL. Cluster-based positive relationships between QSM and age were inferred from a Monte Carlo simulation of a random subset of 20,000 permutations at a familywise error (FWE)-corrected threshold level of 0.005 with default TFCE settings (“−T” flag in Randomize). Before performing statistics, standardized QSMs were smoothed by convolution with a 3D Gaussian kernel (SD: 3 mm). After analysis, the QSM template and study results were warped to MNI152 space (Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Canada) using b-spline and trilinear interpolation, respectively. Note the studywise template to MNI152 transformation was calculated with a rigid, affine, plus deformable b-spline coregistration routine in ANTs (“antsRegistrationSyN”).
Sex dependencies were also tested in the whole brain by way of a two-group comparison using an analogous permutation-based framework. Furthermore, given the relatively high incidence of hypertension in the elderly, we also conducted an experiment to determine whether hypertension could play a role in explaining age-related QSM abnormalities. We identified 15 subjects (age: 70 ± 5 years old, 11 male and 4 female) with a previous diagnosis of hypertension but otherwise no history of diabetes or heart attack and compared them against an equally sized, age- and sex-matched control group with no previous history of hypertension, diabetes, or heart attack. To further contextualize the size of QSM effects due to hypertension in relation to those driven by the normal course of aging, a group of 15 healthy young subjects (age: 27 ± 4 years) was finally contrasted with the nonhypertensive, nondiabetic elderly group.
Regional QSM assessment in deep-brain structures.
The primary objective of the present study was to map the whole-brain landscape of age-related QSM alterations; therefore, a regional assessment in this context was not to validate in a fully independent manner such analysis, but to quantify and illustrate the nature of the alterations captured by the proposed whole-brain method. In addition, because ROI extractions in template space circumvents Gaussian smoothing, reproducing the whole-brain results with such an approach would confirm the validity of smoothing whole-brain QSM data to minimize the impact of warping errors. For ROI analysis, unsmoothed median QSM values were extracted bilaterally from the following structures: caudate nucleus, putamen, globus pallidus, hippocampus, amygdala, thalamus, red nucleus, substantia nigra, and dentate nucleus. The former six ROIs were inferred automatically from the studywise anatomical template using the FIRST algorithm (Patenaude et al., 2011) in FSL and the latter three were traced manually on the QSM template. To reduce edge effects due to registration errors and other standardization imperfections, all ROIs were eroded in 3D by convolution with a 1 mm radius spherical kernel. Subsequently, mask overlays were visually inspected on each template-warped QSM and edited accordingly to ensure that all masks excluded spurious voxels. Test–retest reliability of manual QSM extractions was evaluated using single-measure, intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) and associated 95% confident intervals inferred from ROIs traced by two trained raters (M.J.B. and J.A.-C.). ICCs for nigral, red nucleus, and dentate nucleus measurements were 0.94 ± 0.02, 0.99 ± 0.01, and 0.98 ± 0.01, respectively; therefore, the template-based manual ROI approach was deemed to be highly reliable. Interhemispheric differences were probed using Wilcoxon rank-sum tests, which revealed that left/right measurements were essentially identical (p > 0.4). For simplicity, then, median ROI values were averaged across hemispheres and Pearson age correlation and old-versus-young Wilcoxon rank-sum statistics were computed. In addition, old-versus-young F tests for equal variances were performed and linear least-square fits were calculated for regions where the Pearson correlation test returned p < 0.0056 (i.e., Bonferroni-corrected p < 0.05 for 9 ROIs). All tests were performed two-tailed.
Computational framework and processing times.
All data postprocessing was performed in a desktop computer with two 3.1 GHz octa-core hyperthreading CPUs, 64 GB DDR3 RAM, and a hardware array of four solid-state drives operated by Ubuntu Linux 12.04. Except where stated otherwise, processing tasks (including QSM reconstruction and regional data analysis) were prototyped and executed in the Matlab (R2012a) environment (The Mathworks). The average processing time for single-thread QSM reconstructions was 122 min; however, on this computing system, up to 32 pipelines could be executed simultaneously. In such a parallel environment, the template creation routine was completed in 52 h.
Results
Single QSM examination
Individual QSMs (see example in Fig. 2) revealed very heterogeneous magnetostatic patterns. It is noteworthy that, even for the youngest subjects (20 years old) in this study, the basal ganglia system showed clear signs of metal load, as did the cortex (with the motor strip as a prominent feature) and other deep-brain nuclei. In addition, the venous system was also clearly identifiable. Small, scattered signal inhomogeneities (possibly related to calcifications and appearing mostly in the basal ganglia of some older subjects) were also visible, although, in the absence of cognitive impairment, these were considered nonpathological occurrences (e.g., see small deposits in the putamen and pallidum in Fig. 2, cuts z = −4 mm to z = 5 mm).
QSM data dispersion
Registration errors at structural boundaries or, more generally, those stemming from heterogeneous behaviors due to, for example, atrophy, venous idiosyncrasies, or imaging artifacts, leave a mark in the form of abnormally high QSM variance. This was set empirically in Figure 3 to 10% (or greater) relative to the global mean; that is, an order of magnitude greater than that measured in the dentate nucleus ROI (see regional results below). As can be seen from Figure 3, subcortical masks largely avoided such regions, except for the amygdala and hippocampus, both of which were possibly affected by the large number of blood vessels in their vicinity, as well as the thalamic subnuclear complex, which is locally heterogeneous. The cortex is perhaps more problematic due to its complex topology and close proximity to generators of background field offset, particularly at the pial surface (where the outer cortex meets cerebrospinal-fluid) and near cranial cavities (i.e., in the inferior frontal and anterior temporal areas). However, importantly, across large portions of the cortical ribbon including the majority of deep sulci, the proposed QSM methodology appeared to be sufficiently robust for whole-brain analysis.
Whole-brain analyses
The whole-brain regression (Fig. 4) revealed that iron deposition due to aging is spatially selective. Strong positive correlations, markedly bilateral and confluent, were identified in several cortical and subcortical regions. In deep gray matter, this included the striatum (putamen and caudate nucleus), mesencephalic structures (substantia nigra and red nucleus), diencephalic structures (subthalamic nucleus, mammillary bodies, parts of medial, and posterior thalamic subnuclei groups), and the cerebellar dentate nucleus. QSM covaried with age in the sensory-motor cortex; this was extensive and confluent in superior premotor cortex, prefrontal, insular areas, and dorsomedial frontal cortices down to the cingulate sulcus and cerebellar cortex; and in supramarginal and posterior parahippocampal gray matter and was minor in the posterior cingulate and precuneus cortical regions. White matter QSM relationships, conversely, were overall weaker. These observations were also true for reference-normalized data (results not shown).
The whole-brain QSM analysis in aged hypertensive subjects yielded no significant differences relative to age- and sex-matched controls at pFWE < 0.05, whereas the healthy young versus healthy elderly comparison shown in Figure 5 returned strong QSM alterations in a distribution that closely mirrored the full cohort age regression results. In addition, male-versus-female whole-brain comparisons were also negative (on both contrasts: QSMmale > QSMfemale and QSMmale < QSMfemale at pFWE < 0.05).
Deep-brain regional extraction
As expected, the ROI results confirmed large regional differences in absolute terms for QSM values ranging from substantia nigra and cerebellar dentate nucleus (highest) to hippocampus and amygdala (lowest) (Fig. 6, Table 1).
QSM in five large nuclei—putamen, red nucleus, caudate nucleus, substantia nigra, and dentate nucleus (listed by decreasing correlation strength)—covaried with age (Fig. 6, Table 1). The greatest slopes were found (sorted by steepness) in the red nucleus, dentate nucleus, putamen, and substantia nigra. In addition, in the globus pallidus, putamen, and less markedly in the caudate and dentate nuclei, the variance of the QSM distribution increased as a function of age (Fig. 6, Table 1). QSM in the hippocampus, amygdala, and thalamus, in contrast, remained largely age independent (Fig. 6, Table 1).
No major sex-related effects were identified—only QSM in the thalamus returned a nonsignificant statistical trend in the male/female group comparison (Z = −2.4, p = 0.02), with females presenting lower overall QSM values.
Analogous results to those shown in Figure 6 and Table 1 were obtained for normalized QSM data (see Pearson correlation coefficients and linear fit parameters at the bottom of Table 1).
Discussion
This study presents a novel MRI method to monitor brain iron levels, revealing extensive and largely confluent age-correlated clusters spreading across the cortex (sensory-motor, dorsal frontal lobes, and posterior insular), cerebellum (including cortex and dentate nucleus), as well as several other deep-brain nuclei including putamen, caudate nucleus, medial, posterior, and subthalamic nuclear groups, mammillary bodies, and mesencephalic structures (Fig. 4). In contrast, QSM in cerebral white matter, in a large portion of nonfrontal cortex, and in the hippocampus, amygdala, and other thalamic subnuclei was comparably age independent.
The regional results were tightly concordant with the whole-brain analysis in that median QSM values across striatal, midbrain, and cerebellar nuclei were strongly age correlated (Table 1). Such results were also concordant with past ROI, in vivo MRI (Daugherty and Raz, 2013; Li et al., 2014; Persson et al., 2015; Poynton et al., 2015), and postmortem iron analyses (Hallgren and Sourander, 1958; Ramos et al., 2014) in confirming that the putamen exhibits the tightest age relationship. They also agree with the aforementioned studies and those reviewed previously (Haacke et al., 2005) in that, independent of age, the basal ganglia, red nucleus, and dentate nucleus contain more iron than thalamus, hippocampus, amygdala, cortex, and white matter. It was noted, however, that QSM behaviors in the latter structures were more heterogeneous (Fig. 3), which may explain the conflicting results in, for example, the hippocampus, which was reported to have age-related R2* correlation in one study (Rodrigue et al., 2013; Daugherty et al., 2015).
The strength of a whole-brain approach, however, is that it can capture behaviors beyond those in predetermined ROIs. The most striking observation was that cortex rostral to the central sulcus is more prone to iron accumulation with age than caudal cortices, which is concordant with a previous ROI study using R2* (Rodrigue et al., 2011). This included confluent increases in motor, premotor, dorsal prefrontal, dorsomedial surface, and insula. It is intriguing that, although cortical QSM values are much lower in absolute terms than those in basal nuclei, age-related changes in striatum appear to be mirrored by changes in the frontal lobes; this is interesting given the well known connection of such regions via frontostriatal loops (Alexander et al., 1986). Broadly, most cortical and subcortical areas that accumulate iron are involved in output functions of the brain, motor and decision-making, whereas those involved in sensory processing, such as post-Rolandic cortices and much of the thalamus, do not show this phenomenon. It appears that the motor system, broadly defined, has a tendency to accumulate iron with age.
The exception to this observation was the globus pallidus, which although rich in iron in absolute terms (Fig. 2), did not show age-related QSM increases. In agreement with this result, very few imaging studies have reported stronger age effects in the pallidum compared with the putamen (Daugherty and Raz, 2013). It must be noted, however, that QSM conglomerates effects from all magnetic species, in particular those from comorbid calcification, which in magnetic susceptibility terms counteract paramagnetic moments, could mask the effect of iron accumulation. Although calcium should be considered as a potential confound, it seems highly unlikely that the absence of evidence for iron accumulation in the globus pallidus is spurious because autopsy investigations have also indicated that pallidal iron concentration reaches a plateau before age 50 (Hallgren and Sourander, 1958). Why the pallidum behaves in a different manner is not clear, but it is not hard to find biological differences that might offer clues. For instance, in contrast to its neighboring subcortical nuclei, the afferent input to the globus pallidus is predominantly inhibitory; another difference is its older phylogenetic history: paleostriatum versus neostriatum for caudate and putamen.
The strong correlation of putamenal QSM with age warrants a special mention because a number of studies have also identified metal overload in the AD putamen (Bartzokis et al., 2000; House et al., 2008; Acosta-Cabronero et al., 2013) over and above that seen in aged-matched controls. Incidentally, the striatum has been shown to be the earliest site of fibrillar β-amyloid accumulation in AD (Bateman et al., 2012). Together, these results suggest that the presence of AD pathology severely exacerbates what is otherwise a normal aging process of putamenal iron accumulation.
It has also been postulated that mesencephalic iron overload may be deleterious to normal neuronal activity (Zecca et al., 2004) and could play a pathogenic role in PD (Ostrerova-Golts et al., 2000). The present study supports the notion that midbrain nuclei may be vulnerable, in part through iron accumulation across the adult lifespan, putting them at risk of neurodegenerative disease (Breydo and Uversky, 2011). What makes basal ganglia cells particularly prone to scavenging iron during the course of aging and subsequently in disease is not well understood, although a number of theories, including blood–brain barrier failure or loss of intracellular homeostasis, have been proposed (Ward et al., 2014). Based on present and previous results, one obvious explanation could be that the basal ganglia's vulnerability to iron is simply because it comprises several of the most iron-rich structures in the first place.
This study also found that QSM became significantly more dispersed with age in several nuclei (Fig. 6, Table 1), suggesting that individuals might follow a wide range of age trajectories. This result is consistent with two recent QSM-ROI studies in large aging cohorts that also showed increased variance in the elderly for several subcortical structures (Li et al., 2014; Persson et al., 2015). Such variation might reflect individual differences in iron deposition (including hemosiderin burden) due to genetic (Bartzokis et al., 2010) and/or vulnerability to environmental factors (Hagemeier et al., 2015). Whether this variability could also offer a clue to successful versus nonsuccessful aging remains to be seen, but it certainly seems worth following up in longitudinal studies.
An additional risk factor that has been postulated as a potential contributor to abnormal brain iron accumulation is hypertension (Rodrigue et al., 2011). Therefore, we investigated whether a diagnosis of hypertension would return higher QSM levels than nonhypertensive age-matched controls, but found no support for this prediction. This does not, of course, mean that vascular risk factors may not have a modulatory role in iron accumulation that could emerge with a larger sample, but they are not the primary driver of such age-related changes. In addition, QSM levels in this study did not overall differ between males and females, only slightly in the thalamus. Previous MRI studies identified sex-related iron deposition differences in several brain structures (Bartzokis et al., 2007; Persson et al., 2015), although such subtle sex dependencies have not been reproduced in other in vivo MRI and postmortem analyses (Xu et al., 2008; Ramos et al., 2014).
Several technical aspects regarding the QSM method warrant mention. First, we note the caveat that QSM abnormalities might be attenuated (potentially to the point of being masked) by excessive background susceptibility effects, imperfect QSM inversion, registration errors, and other biases that might introduce excessive variability (Fig. 3). Second, it is worth noting that the outcome of raw and reference-normalized QSM did not differ, suggesting that the effect sizes in aging are much greater than the magnitude of the reference adjustment. Although further confirmation is needed, this is promising news for future studies because it suggests that normalization can be avoided, which is clearly desirable when exploring disease states in which one would wish to avoid making assumptions about areas being spared. The third caveat is that other paramagnetic metals such as copper, manganese, or aluminum could also, in theory, exert a QSM influence (Andrasi et al., 2005; Ramos et al., 2014), although this is quite unlikely in aging in view of their scarcity compared with brain iron (Krebs et al., 2014; Ramos et al., 2014). It was nonetheless reassuring that the concordance of QSM with past postmortem assays was high (Langkammer et al., 2012; Zheng et al., 2013), suggesting that nonheme iron deposition—likely ferric iron (Sun et al., 2015)—is the dominant source of QSM alterations due to aging. Demyelination could, however, play a more active role in modulating QSM alterations (Fukunaga et al., 2010; Stüber et al., 2014). It is interesting that susceptibility was increased both in the cortex and white matter, which is compatible with the idea that myelin loss might result in the attenuation of the lipid-driven diamagnetic component that competes with the effect of ferric iron. The fourth note is, although there was extensive frontal correlation with age, the ventral frontal cortex did not follow this pattern. This could mean the ventral surface does not accumulate iron, but failure to demonstrate age-related iron accumulation in this region may be technical. As with all MRI acquisitions that use gradient echoes, the scan used for QSM suffers from signal loss in areas close to air/bone interfaces; this is particularly true of the ventral frontal lobe, which lies directly above the nasal cavity (Fig. 3). As a final technical note, it is worth pointing out that the basal ganglia alteration observed in this study has consistently been reported using FDRI and R2* mapping (Daugherty and Raz, 2013; Callaghan et al., 2014), although, as mentioned in the Introduction, there are additional limitations with these methods. Whether FDRI, R2* and QSM are completely analogous in their responses to iron-related neurobiological processes (Li et al., 2014; Sun et al., 2015) or differentially sensitive (Deistung et al., 2013) requires further investigation.
To conclude, the present study has demonstrated that the proposed QSM methodology yields reliable measurements of iron deposition in aging. The regional findings were consistent with known patterns of iron load in deep-brain structures and their alterations with aging, whereas the whole-brain approach confirmed that there are additional extensive correlations with age in the frontal lobes. The spatial specificity and remarkable effect sizes further confirm that in vivo QSM is a useful new noninvasive tool with which to investigate cerebral iron accumulation.
Footnotes
We thank all of the participants in this research; Urte Schneider, Sabrina Rädler, and our local radiographers, Kerstin Möhring and Ilona Wiedenhöft, for help in subject recruitment, data collection, and dedicated efforts in scanning standardization and quality assurance; and Oliver Speck for helpful comments on parallel imaging reconstruction.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Julio Acosta-Cabronero., Deutsches Zentrum für Neurodegenerative Erkrankungen e.V. (DZNE) c/o Universitätsklinikum Magdeburg, Leipziger Straβe 44, Haus 64, 39120 Magdeburg, Deutschland. jac{at}cantab.net
This article is freely available online through the J Neurosci Author Open Choice option.