Abstract
Fragile X mental retardation protein (FMRP) is thought to regulate neuronal plasticity by limiting dendritic protein synthesis, but direct demonstration of a requirement for FMRP control of local protein synthesis during behavioral plasticity is lacking. Here we tested whether FMRP knockdown in Xenopus optic tectum affects local protein synthesis in vivo and whether FMRP knockdown affects protein synthesis-dependent visual avoidance behavioral plasticity. We tagged newly synthesized proteins by incorporation of the noncanonical amino acid azidohomoalanine and visualized them with fluorescent noncanonical amino acid tagging (FUNCAT). Visual conditioning and FMRP knockdown produce similar increases in FUNCAT in tectal neuropil. Induction of visual conditioning-dependent behavioral plasticity occurs normally in FMRP knockdown animals, but plasticity degrades over 24 h. These results indicate that FMRP affects visual conditioning-induced local protein synthesis and is required to maintain the visual conditioning-induced behavioral plasticity.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Fragile X syndrome (FXS) is the most common form of inherited intellectual disability. Exaggerated dendritic protein synthesis resulting from loss of fragile X mental retardation protein (FMRP) is thought to underlie cognitive deficits in FXS, but no direct evidence has demonstrated that FMRP-regulated dendritic protein synthesis affects behavioral plasticity in intact animals. Xenopus tadpoles exhibit a visual avoidance behavior that improves with visual conditioning in a protein synthesis-dependent manner. We showed that FMRP knockdown and visual conditioning dramatically increase protein synthesis in neuronal processes. Furthermore, induction of visual conditioning-dependent behavioral plasticity occurs normally after FMRP knockdown, but performance rapidly deteriorated in the absence of FMRP. These studies show that FMRP negatively regulates local protein synthesis and is required to maintain visual conditioning-induced behavioral plasticity in vivo.
- dendritic protein synthesis
- plasticity
- retinotectal
- visual avoidance behavior
- visual conditioning
- Xenopus
Introduction
Fragile X mental retardation protein (FMRP) is a highly conserved mRNA binding protein expressed throughout the brain (Santoro et al., 2012). Loss of FMRP, as occurs in fragile X syndrome (FXS), is thought to lead to deficits in nervous system function underlying cognitive and behavioral deficits associated with FXS (Liu-Yesucevitz et al., 2011; Darnell and Klann, 2013). Studies in mouse models of FXS and in humans suggest that FMRP regulates sensory system function and plasticity (Kwon et al., 2001; Kogan et al., 2004; Harlow et al., 2010; Kéri and Benedek, 2011; Till et al., 2012; Gandhi et al., 2014). For instance, visual experience increases FMRP expression in visual cortical neurons (Gabel et al., 2004), and loss of FMRP impairs critical period plasticity in the visual, auditory, and somatosensory systems (Dölen et al., 2007; Harlow et al., 2010; Kim et al., 2013), possibly by affecting long-term potentiation (Yang et al., 2014). Loss of FMRP results in exaggerated protein synthesis (Laggerbauer et al., 2001; Li et al., 2001; Qin et al., 2005), which is thought to underlie cognitive deficits seen in FXS. For instance, Fmr1 knock-out mice exhibit increased protein synthesis-dependent mGluR-mediated hippocampal long-term depression (LTD), suggesting that the increased proteins synthesized in the absence of FMRP enhance this type of plasticity (Huber et al., 2000; Bear et al., 2004; Sidorov et al., 2013). Whether such a model generalizes to sensory system plasticity is not yet clear.
FMRP and its mRNA targets are packaged in ribonucleoprotein (RNP) granules, which are distributed in neuronal somata, dendrites, and axons and are associated with polyribosomes (Feng et al., 1997; Ling et al., 2004; Weiler et al., 2004; Wang et al., 2014; Faulkner et al., 2015), suggesting that FMRP may regulate neuronal plasticity by negatively regulating protein synthesis downstream of synaptic activity (Feng et al., 1997; Martin et al., 2000; Ling et al., 2004; Weiler et al., 2004; Antar et al., 2005; Sidorov et al., 2013). FMRP-dependent control of protein synthesis in neuronal processes could allow spatiotemporal control over availability of proteins required for plasticity (Liu-Yesucevitz et al., 2011; Darnell and Klann, 2013). Nevertheless, no direct evidence has distinguished FMRP-regulated somatic and local protein synthesis in vivo, and no study has demonstrated a specific requirement for FMRP-regulated local protein synthesis in behavioral plasticity in an intact animal.
Here we tested whether FMRP knockdown in Xenopus optic tectal neurons affects protein synthesis in neural processes in the tectal neuropil and whether FMRP knockdown affects protein synthesis-dependent visual avoidance behavioral plasticity. Direct investigation of the role of FMRP-regulated local protein synthesis in behavioral plasticity in intact systems has been challenging because it requires methods that can detect low-abundance newly synthesized proteins among the large pool of preexisting proteins and that can minimize contamination of locally synthesized proteins in neuronal processes with somatically synthesized proteins. To address these problems, we optimized the temporal and spatial resolution of fluorescent noncanonical amino acid tagging (FUNCAT; Dieterich et al., 2010; Hinz et al., 2012) in Xenopus optic tectum to visualize proteins that are newly synthesized within neural processes in a timeframe consistent with induction and expression of behavioral plasticity (Shen et al., 2014). Under these conditions, we show that protein synthesis in the tectal neuropil increases in vivo in response to visual conditioning and with FMRP knockdown. Furthermore, we show that induction of visual avoidance behavioral plasticity occurs normally with FMRP knockdown, but plasticity degrades over 24 h in the absence of FMRP. These results suggest that FMRP normally limits the synthesis of proteins that degrade behavioral plasticity.
Materials and Methods
Animals and transfection.
Stage 46–48 albino Xenopus laevis tadpoles of either sex were bred in house or purchased (Nasco or Xenopus Express) and used for all experiments. The neuronal-specific β-tubulin (NβT)–GFP transgenic tadpoles, which express GFP from the tubulin β class IIb (tubb2b) promoter, were purchased from the National Xenopus Resource at the Marine Biological Laboratory. All animal protocols were approved by the Institutional Animal Use and Care Committee of the Scripps Research Institute. Animals were anesthetized by 0.02% MS-222 before injections or electroporation or were terminally anesthetized in 0.2% MS-222. Plasmid constructs (1–2 μg/μl), antisense morpholino oligonucleotides against fmr1a (FMOs; 0.1mm; GeneTools) and scrambled control morpholinos (CMOs; 0.1 mm; GeneTools), and cell transfection with electroporation have been described previously (Faulkner et al., 2015).
FUNCAT.
l-Azidohomoalaine (AHA) at 500 mm, pH 7.4 (Click Chemistry Tools) colored with ∼0.01% fast green was pressure injected into the midbrain ventricle of anesthetized tadpoles. We also mixed AHA with anisomycin (25 μm; Sigma), cycloheximide (50 μm; Sigma), or colchicine (Acros Organics) as stated. Brains were dissected and fixed with 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in PBS (pH 7.4), 0.5, 2, 3, or 24 h later, as specified. Samples were processed for click chemistry according to the following protocol, modified from previous studies (Dieterich et al., 2010; Hinz et al., 2012). Permeabilized whole-mount brains or vibratome sections were transferred to Eppendorf tubes or six-well plates with reaction mixture composed of 100 μm Tris [(1-benzyl-1H-1,2,3-triazol-4-yl)methyl]amine (TBTA, Sigma) dissolved in 4:1 tBuOH/DMSO (Sigma), 100 μm CuSO4 (Sigma), 1.25 μm Alexa Fluor 488 alkyne (Invitrogen), and 250 μm Tris(2-carboxyethyl)phosphine (TCEP, Sigma). The reaction proceeded overnight at room temperature. Samples were washed several times and then either cleared and mounted in 50% glycerol/6 m urea and imaged with an Olympus FluoView500 confocal microscope with a 20× [0.8 numerical aperture (NA)], 40× (1.0 NA), or 60× (1.4 NA) oil-immersion lens or processed for immunohistochemistry as described below.
Immunohistochemistry and quantification.
Tadpoles were anesthetized in 0.02% MS-222 solution and fixed with 4% PFA in PBS, pH 7.4, overnight at 4°C for immunolabeling with anti-Sox2 antibodies or anti-NβT antibodies. To immunolabel with anti-vimentin antibodies, animals were fixed with 100% methanol overnight at −20°C or 2% PFA for 1 h at room temperature, followed by 100% methanol overnight at −20°C. To immunolabel with anti-GABA antibodies, we fixed animals with 4% PFA and 1% glutaraldehyde overnight at 4°C. Brains were dissected and either processed as whole brains or sectioned at 40 μm on a vibratome. Samples were permeabilized and blocked in 4% BSA and 1% normal goat serum in 0.5% Triton X-100 for 1 h at room temperature and then incubated in 1:200 mouse anti-β-tubulin I+II (T8535; Sigma; Moody et al., 1996), 1:200 rabbit anti-Sox2 [2748S (recognizes Xenopus Sox2 and Sox3); Cell Signaling Technology; Gaete et al., 2012)], 1:1000 rabbit anti-GABA (A2052; Sigma; Li and Cline, 2010), or 1:50 mouse anti-vimentin (14h-7; Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank; Dent et al., 1989) overnight at 4°C, followed by 2 h in 1:200 anti-mouse Alexa Fluor 647 or anti-rabbit Alexa Fluor 647 (Life Technologies) at room temperature. Samples were cleared and mounted in 50% glycerol/6 m urea and imaged with an Olympus FluoView500 confocal microscope with a 20× (0.8 NA) or 60× (1.4 NA) oil-immersion lens or on a Nikon A1 confocal microscope with a 60× (1.4 NA) oil-immersion lens.
Fluorescence intensity of AHA labeling in the cell body layer or in the neuropil was quantified in single optical sections from confocal z series through the brain using custom applications created in MATLAB 2009b (MathWorks, Psychophysics Toolbox extensions). We selected the most dorsal optical section through the optic tectum ventral to the anterior commissure from the confocal z stacks and oriented the brain image along the rostrocaudal axis. Only samples in which the ventricle is closed at the dorsal midline were included in the analysis. We manually drew a dorsoventral line 40–60 pixels long (24.8–37.2 μm) along the midline, extending caudally from the anterior commissure, and randomly plotted 10 mediolateral lines perpendicular to the midline. We calculated the average fluorescence intensity along the 10 mediolateral lines from the midline through the cell body layer to the neuropil in both the left and right tectal lobes. The ventricular cell body layer is enriched for neural progenitor cells (NPCs), identified by Sox2 immunolabeling, and neurons, identified by NβT labeling, are lateral to the NPCs. Samples with >10% difference in total fluorescence intensity between the left and right hemispheres were excluded from the analysis. Fluorescence intensity in the cell body layer was measured in the 50 pixels (31 μm) to the left and right of the midline, and the neuropil was measured between 80 and 100 pixels (49.6–68.2 μm) to the left and right of the midline.
To test whether AHA-labeled proteins are enriched in cell types or cellular compartments labeled by the different markers, we created masks of regions of interest (ROIs) based on immunolabeling with Sox2, GABA, and NβT in the cell body layer or neuropil. The average AHA labeling intensities per unit area within or outside of the masks were normalized to average AHA intensity within ROIs without masking.
For quantification of NβT-immunolabeled neuronal processes in Figure 3, we used the 2D Sholl analysis plugin in Fiji (Ferreira et al., 2014). We determined the numbers of intersections of NβT-immunolabeled neuronal processes with rings of increasing radius extending from the neuropil in the left top corner through the cell body layer in the right bottom corner in the z projections of images of NβT immunolabeling in the optic tectum 0.5 and 2 h after PBS or colchicine injection. Mean intersections (see Fig. 3e) are the average of intersections at all rings.
To quantify the effect of FMRP knockdown on translation, we electroporated tadpole midbrains with CMOs or FMOs and, 2 d later, injected the midbrain ventricle with AHA, as described above. Animals were terminally anesthetized and fixed 1 or 3 h later and processed for click chemistry, as described above. A single experiment included animals from a single clutch of embryos processed together, blind to treatment. Fluorescence intensity in the NPC layer was measured in the 20 pixels (12.4 μm) to the left and right of the midline, and the neuronal cell layer measurement was made from the 30 pixels (18.6 μm) just lateral to the NPC layer. The average fluorescence intensities of animals with 3 h AHA labeling were then normalized to the average intensities of animals with 1 h AHA labeling from the same experiment. The AHA intensity ratio 3 h/1 h of CMO- or FMO-treated animals from each independent experiment was paired for statistic analysis.
For quantification of AHA-labeled proteins in neuronal processes within the optic tectal neuropil, we developed and performed customized image analysis macros in NIH ImageJ. We used NβT immunolabeling as a mask to identify neuronal processes and measured AHA fluorescence intensity only in the NβT mask within a fixed ROI of 1762.66 μm2 in the tectal neuropil (see Fig. 5), propagated through the z series of the tectal neuropil. The final fluorescence intensity value for each animal was an average from image stacks of two or more representative 40-μm-thick sections. The AHA fluorescence intensity of the experimental group was normalized to the control group within each independent experiment and then pooled for statistic analysis.
All samples were prepared, imaged, and analyzed in parallel, blind to treatment, using the same image acquisition and analysis settings. Figures were compiled using Adobe Illustrator (Adobe Systems).
In vivo time-lapse imaging and analysis of NβT–GFP transgenic tadpoles.
NβT–GFP transgenic tadpoles were imaged on a custom-built two-photon microscope with a 20× water-immersion lens (0.95 NA). To reduce interference from pigment cells in the skin, we raised tadpoles in 0.001% phenylthiourea (Sigma) starting 2–3 d after fertilization until stage 47/48 when they were used for experiments. We collected time-lapse images of optic tecta before, 0.5 h, and 2 h after injecting the midbrain ventricle with ∼10 nl of PBS or 5 or 10 mg/ml colchicine. All images were collected using the same image acquisition settings.
To compare the change in cell morphology of NβT–GFP-expressing neurons in Figure 3b, we measured the complexity of the GFP-labeled neuronal processes using Fuji 3D Sholl analysis. We counted the numbers of intersections of NβT–GFP neuronal processes with rings of increasing radius at 100, 150, 200, 250, and 300 μm extending from the neuropil in the left top corner through the cell body layer in the right bottom corner in the z projections of images of NβT–GFP. The mean intersections are the average of intersections at all radii (see Fig. 3a). The values for each time point for each animal were normalized to the value before PBS or colchicine injection. The normalized value for each time point from individual animals was paired for statistic analysis.
In vivo time-lapse imaging and analysis of cytoplasmic polyadenylation element binding protein–CFP RNPs and mitochondria.
We determined the effect of colchicine on the distribution of cytoplasmic polyadenylation element binding protein (CPEB)-CFP-labeled RNP granules (see Fig. 4) as follows: Optic tecta were sparsely transfected with cytosolic yellow fluorescent protein (YFP) and CPEB-CFP and, after 3 d, were imaged on a PerkinElmer Ultraview Vox spinning disk confocal microscope with a 25× water-immersion lens (1.1 NA) at stated times after ventricular injection with colchicine or PBS. To image YFP and CFP, we sequentially excited YFP and CFP with 488 or 405 nm laser lines, respectively, and imaged with UltraVIEW emission filters “527(W55)” or “485(W60)” accordingly. To quantify CPEB–CFP RNP granules, we used the 3D object counter plugin in NIH ImageJ (Bolte and Cordelieres, 2006). We set the threshold intensity to 150 and counted objects larger than 0.25 μm3 within YFP+ areas. Then we assigned each object to an individual cell in the original images.
To determine the effect of colchicine on the distribution of mitochondria (see Fig. 3d–g), stage 47/48 tadpoles received ventricular injections of 50 μm MitoTracker DeepRed FM (Invitrogen) diluted in PBS from 1 mm stock solution in DMSO mixed with PBS or colchicine. Time-lapse confocal z series through the optic tectum were collected at the stated times on a PerkinElmer Ultraview Vox spinning disk confocal microscope with a 25× water-immersion lens (1.1 NA). The fluorescence intensity of MitoTracker labeling was measured in an ROI of the cell body layers and an ROI of the neuropil along the mediolateral extent of the tectum in three individual optical sections per brain at each time point. The ROIs were 150 × 100 pixels (43.86 × 29.24 μm) positioned at the ventricular and pial edges of the tectal lobe (see Fig. 3b). Each fluorescence intensity value is the average from three sections per tectal lobe. The average fluorescence intensities from the 0.5 and 2 h time points were normalized to the intensities at the 0 h time point for each animal. The normalized MitoTracker labeling intensity from each time point from individual animals was paired for statistic analysis.
Visual conditioning and visual avoidance assay.
Visual conditioning was provided to animals as described previously (Shen et al., 2014). Animals were exposed to moving bars (1 cm width; 0.3 Hz; luminance, 25 cd/m2) continuously for 1.5 h. The visual avoidance assay was conducted as reported previously (Dong et al., 2009; Shen et al., 2014). Briefly, four to six tadpoles were placed in a 8 × 3 cm tank filled with ∼25 ml of Steinberg's rearing solution. The bottom of the chamber was mounted with a backprojection screen. Visual stimuli were projected on the screen using a microprojector (MPro110; 3M). Videos of tadpoles illuminated by infrared LEDs were recorded with a Hamamatsu ORCA-ER digital camera. Visual stimuli were generated and presented by MATLAB 2009b. Randomly positioned moving spots of 0.4 cm diameter were presented in pseudorandom order for 60 s. Visual avoidance behavior was scored as a change in swim trajectory or speed and plotted as an avoidance index (AI), the ratio of avoidance responses to the first 10 encounters with an approaching visual stimulus. Animals in which >50% of turning events were independent of an encounter with visual stimuli were not included for additional analysis. Outliers, defined as those with AI greater that two SDs from the mean, were excluded from the analysis.
Statistical tests.
All data are presented as mean ± SEM based on at least three independent experiments unless otherwise noted. Data are considered significantly different when p values are <0.05. The nonparametric Mann–Whitney U or Wilcoxon's signed-rank tests were used to compare between two independent groups or matched pairs as stated. The Steel–Dwass test was used for nonparametric multiple comparisons between all pairs. The Wilcoxon's test was used for nonparametric multiple comparisons for each pair. Experiments and analysis were performed blind to the experimental conditions. JMP 11 statistics software (SAS Institute) was used for all statistics analysis.
Results
FUNCAT detects rapid changes in translation in intact animals
FUNCAT allows visualization of newly synthesized proteins after incorporation of a noncanonical amino acid, such as AHA, a methionine analog, in place of endogenous methionine. Click chemistry is then used to tag a bio-orthogonal group with a fluorescent reporter. Previous studies applied FUNCAT to intact zebrafish and showed that exposing animals to AHA in rearing solution over a period of several days labeled proteins throughout the animal without toxicity or behavioral impairment (Hinz et al., 2012). We have shown that AHA injection into the brain ventricle allows rapid labeling of newly synthesized proteins over the time course of hours using biotin-based bio-orthogonal noncanonical amino acid tagging (BONCAT) on dot blots (Shen et al., 2014). Here we tested whether ventricular injection of AHA improves the temporal resolution of FUNCAT in Xenopus optic tectum in vivo. AHA was injected into the midbrain ventricle, and brains were dissected and processed for click chemistry to tag AHA with fluorophore for visualization of AHA-labeled proteins. Figure 1a shows an example of AHA labeling detected 3 h after ventricular injection next to a sketch of the tadpole midbrain with the tectal cell body layer and neuropil shaded in green and gray, respectively. AHA labeling was detected 2 h after injection of AHA compared with minimal background labeling after PBS injection (Fig. 1b,c). Coinjection of AHA with the translational inhibitors anisomycin (25 μm) or cycloheximide (50 μm) significantly decreased AHA labeling seen over 2 h (Fig. 1b,c). When animals were treated with 2 h bath application of 15 mm pentylenetetrazol (PTZ), a GABA receptor antagonist that increases brain activity, AHA labeling was significantly greater in both the tectal cell body layer and neuropil, indicating a similar proportional increase in activity-induced protein synthesis in the cell bodies and processes (Fig. 1d,e). We compared the intensity of AHA labeling in the neuropil to the cell body layer and found that the neuropil has 18.8 ± 0.35% of AHA labeling intensity in the cell body layer under control conditions and 18.1 ± 0.98% after PTZ treatment, indicating that PTZ increased protein synthesis to a comparable extent in the cell body layer and neuropil. These data show that FUNCAT has sufficient sensitivity to detect changes in protein synthesis in the cell body layer and neuropil within a 2 h window when animals are exposed to drugs that either block translation or indirectly increase protein synthesis downstream of increased neuronal activity.
Next, we tested whether shorter AHA labeling times could be used to visualize newly synthesized proteins with FUNCAT and how long AHA-labeled proteins can be detected to identify the temporal resolution of FUNCAT in vivo. We detected AHA-labeled proteins as soon as 0.5 h after AHA injection into the midbrain ventricle, and AHA labeling increased over 3 h after injection in both the cell body layer and neuropil. When the animals were fixed 24 h after AHA injection, AHA-labeled proteins could still be detected (Fig. 1f,g). These data indicate that newly synthesized proteins can be detected in live animals as soon as 0.5 h after AHA injection and that they persist for at least 1 d.
We combined FUNCAT with immunohistochemistry for cell-type-specific markers to determine whether AHA labeling differed in different cell types. We observed higher-intensity AHA labeling in the ventricular layer cells that are labeled by Sox2 antibodies, a marker of NPCs, indicating more active protein translation in NPCs than in neurons. We quantified the intensity of AHA labeling in Sox2+ cells compared with Sox2− cells using a mask based on the Sox2 immunolabeling, as described in Materials and Methods. This analysis indicated that AHA labeling is significantly enriched in Sox2+ cells (Fig. 2a; Table 1). Note that the Sox2 mask only measures AHA in the NPC nuclei, whereas AHA labeling is both nuclear and cytosolic, so the AHA enrichment in NPCs is an underestimate. AHA labeling is comparable in GABA-immunopositive and GABA-immunonegative neurons and processes in both the cell body layer and the neuropil (Fig. 2b; Table 1). AHA-labeled proteins were detected not only in the cytosol, where translation occurs, but also in the nuclei, consistent with translocation of newly synthesized proteins from the cytosol to the nuclei (Fig. 2a,b). We also observed AHA labeling along the processes of NPCs and neurons, marked by antibodies against vimentin and NβT (Moody et al., 1996), respectively. The vimentin labeled radial glial NPC processes were too fine to quantify AHA labeling reliably (Fig. 2c). By analyzing the intensity of AHA labeling per unit area in a mask of NβT+ labeling compared with NβT− labeling, we found that AHA labeling is significantly enriched in NβT+ processes in the cell body layer compared with the NβT− area (Fig. 2d; Table 1). It is unclear whether the AHA-labeled proteins localized in processes were synthesized locally or were synthesized in the somata and then transported to the processes.
Visualization of locally synthesized protein in neural processes in vivo
The goal of our study is to test whether FMRP regulates local protein synthesis in neuronal processes in vivo and whether FMRP-regulated protein synthesis is required for behavioral plasticity. Specific visualization of locally synthesized proteins within neural processes has been a challenge in intact animals because it has been difficult to segregate locally synthesized proteins from proteins that are synthesized in the cell body and transported to the processes. One way to tackle this problem is to block the cellular machinery required to transport newly synthesized proteins from the somata to the processes. Previous studies used colchicine, which inhibits microtubule polymerization, to block microtubule-dependent transport from the somata to the processes (Fischer and Schmatolla, 1972; Shan et al., 2003; Antar et al., 2005). For our purposes, we wanted to disrupt microtubules to interfere with protein transport from the somata but preserve the presence of RNP granules in neural processes, thereby maintaining the cellular machinery required for protein synthesis in processes. In preliminary experiments, we imaged transgenic Xenopus tadpoles expressing GFP driven by the neuron-specific β-tubulin class IIb (NβT or tubb2b) promoter in all neurons before and after ventricular injection of ∼10 nl of 5 or 10 mg/ml colchicine or PBS. These experiments suggested that treatment with 5 mg/ml colchicine might affect microtubule stability (data not shown). Furthermore, ventricular injections of dilute fast green dye show that small molecules are rapidly cleared from the brain ventricle, suggesting that ventricular colchicine may have a transient effect on microtubule structure. To address this directly, we tested whether microtubule structure could be disrupted by ventricular injection of colchicine, using immunolabeling with antibodies against NβT (Moody et al., 1996). Animals received ventricular injections of PBS or 5 mg/ml colchicine and were fixed 0.5 or 2 h later. As also seen in Figure 2, NβT immunolabeling identifies bundles of microtubules in neuronal processes extending from cell bodies into the neuropil in PBS-injected control animals (Fig. 3a,b). Microtubule bundles were disorganized 0.5 h after colchicine treatment, and this effect was reversed after 2 h (Fig. 3a,b), likely because the colchicine was cleared from the ventricle. Sholl analysis indicated that the integrity of NβT-labeled processes extending from the cell body layer to the neuropil decreased 0.5 h after colchicine and recovered to control values at 2 h (Fig. 3c).
Next, we examined whether microtubule-dependent transport from neuronal cell bodies into processes was affected by ventricular colchicine injection by tracking the distribution of mitochondria in the tectum (Plucińska et al., 2012). We labeled mitochondria by coinjecting MitoTracker with either PBS or 5 mg/ml colchicine into the midbrain ventricle and collected in vivo time-lapse confocal images of the optic tecta immediately after (0 h) and 0.5 and 2 h after injection. Figure 3d shows the location of the imaged portion of the tectum. Images in Figure 3, e and f, are inverted such that fluorescently labeled mitochondria appear black. We measured the fluorescence intensity across the tectum from the pial layer through the neuropil to the cell body layer, abutting the ventricle (Fig. 3e, bottom). MitoTracker quickly labels mitochondria in the cell body layer, and analysis of in vivo time-lapse images from control tecta indicates that the density of mitochondria in the cell body layer and neuropil increases over 2 h after PBS injection (Fig. 3f,g). In contrast, in vivo images collected 0, 0.5, and 2 h after 5 mg/ml colchicine treatment indicate that the density of mitochondria increases in the cell body layer but not in the neuropil (Fig. 3f,g). MitoTracker labeling intensity in the cell body layer and neuropil was comparable immediately after PBS and colchicine treatment [cell body layer: PBS, 4005 ± 607 arbitrary units (AU); colchicine, 4920 ± 369 AU; neuropil: PBS, 1452 ± 141 AU; colchicine, 1360 ± 101 AU; n = 9 brains for PBS and colchicine]. After 2 h, MitoTracker labeling intensity in the neuropil of PBS-treated animals was significantly greater than in colchicine-treated animals (Fig. 3f,g). Labeling in the cell body layer was comparable in PBS- and colchicine-treated animals, indicating that MitoTracker uptake into the cells was not affected (Fig. 3f,g). These results indicate that microtubule-dependent transport from neural cell bodies into processes is decreased by ventricular colchicine injection.
To examine whether the cellular machinery required for protein synthesis is retained in processes after colchicine treatment, we labeled RNP granules and visualized cells by coexpressing CPEB–CFP fusion protein and cytosolic YFP (Bestman and Cline, 2009). We did not label RNP granules with FMRP fusion proteins because overexpressing FMRP causes apoptosis in NPCs and decreases dendritic arbor elaboration in neurons (Faulkner et al., 2015). Three days later, we injected PBS or 5 mg/ml colchicine into the midbrain ventricle and collected in vivo confocal images of labeled optic tectal cells before and 0.5 and 2 h after colchicine or PBS injection (Fig. 4a). We identified CPEB–CFP puncta in YFP-expressing cells based on orthogonal views of x, y, z confocal images (Fig. 4b). All of the cells with CPEB–CFP-labeled RNP granules at the first time point retained them in both colchicine- and PBS-injected animals (Table 2). We next tested whether ventricular injection of colchicine affects AHA labeling and found that the intensity of AHA labeling in the tectal cell body layer and neuropil was comparable in animals 0.5 h after ventricular injection with PBS or colchicine (Fig. 4c,d), indicating that colchicine treatment does not interfere with protein synthesis. Together, these data indicate that ventricular injection of 5 mg/ml colchicine transiently disrupts microtubule structure and microtubule-dependent transport from the somata into neural processes without eliminating RNP granules in neural processes or decreasing AHA labeling. These data also suggest that these conditions allow local protein synthesis but minimize transport of somatically synthesized proteins into neural processes.
Visual conditioning increases local protein synthesis in the tectal neuropil
Using the conditions established above, we tested whether a visual conditioning protocol, known to induce protein synthesis-dependent behavioral plasticity in vivo (Shen et al., 2014), changes protein synthesis in the tectal neuropil, using the protocol in Figure 5a. The visual stimulus used for conditioning consisted of 1-cm-wide white bars on a dark background moving at 0.3 Hz in each of the four cardinal directions in pseudorandom order (Fig. 5b). We injected 5 mg/ml colchicine into the tectal ventricle 0.5 h before exposing animals to visual conditioning or ambient light (control) and then injected AHA, mixed with colchicine, immediately before visual conditioning to label proteins that were synthesized in response to visual conditioning. AHA was mixed with colchicine to avoid diluting colchicine with the AHA injection. Based on our observation that colchicine disrupted microtubules for up to 2 h after injection, animals were exposed to visual conditioning for a total of 1.5 h, starting 0.5 h after the first colchicine injection. This time window was intended to limit the contribution of somatic protein synthesis to the AHA fluorescence signal in the neuropil. To quantify newly synthesized proteins in neuronal processes in the tectal neuropil, we created a mask of the ROI based on NβT immunolabeling (Fig. 5c). We then determined the AHA fluorescence intensity in the NβT+ ROI to capture only the AHA labeling within neuronal processes. Animals exposed to visual conditioning had 23 ± 8% greater AHA labeling in NβT+ neuronal processes compared with animals exposed to ambient light (Fig. 5d). In contrast, AHA labeling intensity measured in the same region of neuropil without the NβT mask was comparable in animals exposed to ambient light or conditioning (Fig. 5d). Our data indicate that visual conditioning increases protein synthesis in neuronal processes in the tectal neuropil.
FMRP knockdown increases protein synthesis in NPCs and neurons
We showed that visual conditioning increases protein synthesis in the tectal neuropil (Fig. 5), but the underlying mechanisms remain unclear. We are interested in testing whether the increase in local protein synthesis in response to visual conditioning is regulated by FMRP. First, we tested whether FMRP knockdown increases protein synthesis in the optic tectum by comparing the difference in AHA labeling over 1 and 3 h of AHA exposure. We knocked down FMRP by electroporating tectal cells with antisense FMOs, which reduce FMRP expression in Xenopus tectum (Faulkner et al., 2015). Control animals were electroporated with CMOs. After 2 d to allow knockdown, AHA was injected into the midbrain ventricle of tadpoles, they were fixed after 1 or 3 h, and their brains were processed for AHA labeling. We measured the fluorescence intensity of AHA labeling in the ventricular layer, which is populated by NPCs, in the neuronal cell body layer and in the neuropil in paired groups of animals that were treated with CMOs or FMOs from six clutches of tadpoles (Fig. 6a,b). The average fluorescence intensities of animals after 3 h AHA labeling were normalized to the average intensities of animals after 1 h AHA labeling from the same clutch. FMRP knockdown increased AHA labeling in NPCs, neuronal cell bodies, and neuropil compared with animals treated with CMOs (Fig. 6c; Table 3). These data indicate that FMRP knockdown increases newly synthesized proteins in somata and processes detected over 2 h exposure to AHA.
FMRP knockdown and visual conditioning increase local protein synthesis in the tectal neuropil
Next, we tested whether FMRP knockdown regulates protein synthesis in the tectal neuropil and whether the visual conditioning-dependent increase in newly synthesized proteins in the tectal neuropil is regulated by FMRP, using the protocol in Figure 7a. Animals were electroporated with either CMOs or FMOs. Two days later, we injected the midbrain ventricle with 5 mg/ml colchicine 0.5 h before visual conditioning and then AHA with colchicine immediately before visual conditioning, as described above. To determine whether visual conditioning affects protein synthesis in the tectal neuropil in the FMRP knockdown animals, we compared the AHA labeling in animals exposed to visual conditioning or ambient light (control) for 2 h (Fig. 7a). Visual conditioning significantly increased AHA labeling in the tectal neuropil of animals treated with CMOs. FMRP knockdown alone increased AHA labeling in the tectal neuropil compared with CMOs (Fig. 7b), but we did not see an additional increase in AHA labeling when we exposed FMRP knockdown tadpoles to visual conditioning (Fig. 7b). These results indicate that both FMRP knockdown and visual conditioning increase protein synthesis in the neuropil. Together with previous work describing protein synthesis-dependent visual system plasticity (Shen et al., 2014), the data suggest that FMRP knockdown might affect visual system plasticity.
FMRP knockdown blocks maintenance of visual conditioning-induced behavioral plasticity
To determine the functional consequences of FMRP knockdown on protein synthesis-dependent visual system plasticity, we examined visual avoidance behavioral plasticity in FMRP knockdown animals. Tadpoles exhibit visual avoidance behavior in response to an approaching object (Dong et al., 2009; McKeown et al., 2013), and their rate of successful avoidance responses increases after animals receive conditioning visual stimulation (Shen et al., 2014). The behavior is quantified as an AI, the ratio of avoidance responses to encounters with an approaching visual stimulus (Shen et al., 2014).
Baseline avoidance behavior was tested 2 d after animals were electroporated with either CMOs or FMOs, and then animals were subjected to 1.5 h visual conditioning followed by avoidance behavior tests 2, 2.5, and 24 h after the onset of visual conditioning (Fig. 8a). We found no significant difference in baseline avoidance behavior before animals were exposed to visual conditioning (AI: CMOs, 0.36 ± 0.02; FMOs, 0.39 ± 0.03). Control and FMRP knockdown animals showed comparable plasticity of the visual avoidance behavior immediately after visual conditioning (AI: CMOs, 0.49 ± 0.03; FMOs, 0.54 ± 0.03). CMO-treated animals maintained the plasticity for up to 24 h after visual conditioning (Fig. 8b,c), but the visual conditioning-induced behavioral plasticity was not maintained at the 24 h time point in FMRP knockdown animals (Fig. 8d,e). These data indicate that behavioral plasticity can be induced in FMRP knockdown animals, but the plasticity degrades in the absence of FMRP.
Discussion
Considerable evidence has shown the importance of newly synthesized mRNA and proteins in long-term synaptic plasticity and memory formation (Agranoff and Klinger, 1964; Agranoff et al., 1965; Lamprecht and LeDoux, 2004; Cajigas et al., 2010; West and Greenberg, 2011). Nevertheless, the underlying mechanisms of activity-induced protein synthesis-dependent synaptic plasticity are unclear. In this study, we optimized the spatial and temporal resolution of FUNCAT to allow systematic analysis of the role of FMRP in the regulation of local protein synthesis and experience-dependent behavioral plasticity in intact Xenopus tadpoles. We demonstrated that protein synthesis in the tectal neuropil increases in response to visual conditioning in the developing visual system and that both visual conditioning and FMRP regulate protein synthesis in tectal neuronal processes. Finally, using an assay of visual avoidance behavioral plasticity, we showed that FMRP knockdown does not interfere with induction of behavioral plasticity, but it blocks maintenance of plasticity. Together, we conclude that FMRP is important for visual conditioning-induced local protein synthesis and is required to maintain visual conditioning-dependent behavior plasticity.
Changes in local protein synthesis has been reported in response to different treatments, for example, BDNF and (RS)-3,5-dihydroxyphenylglycine (DHPG) in vitro (Huber et al., 2000; Aakalu et al., 2001; Dieterich et al., 2010), but, as a result of limited tools available for in vivo studies, few studies have investigated whether sensory input, such as visual experience, affects local protein synthesis in intact animals. Recently, several methods have been developed to visualize protein synthesis (Schmidt et al., 2009; Dieterich et al., 2010; Ngo and Tirrell, 2011; Goodman et al., 2012; Hinz et al., 2012; Elliott et al., 2014). We improved the temporal resolution for detection of newly synthesized proteins in X. laevis in vivo to correspond to the time course of induction of protein synthesis-dependent visual avoidance plasticity. By treating animals with translational inhibitors or increasing translation with PTZ or FMRP knockdown, we showed that FUNCAT has sufficient sensitivity and dynamic range to detect increases and decreases in translation. Combining FUNCAT with immunolabeling different cell types enabled us to increase the cellular resolution of FUNCAT and demonstrate that NPCs are more active in protein synthesis than neurons.
Significant spatial and temporal resolution of FUNCAT within neural processes in vivo is critical to examine the functions of FMRP and visual conditioning in protein synthesis-dependent behavioral plasticity. Previous studies distinguished local protein synthesis in neuronal processes from somatic protein synthesis by growing cells in microfluidic cell culture devices or by sparsely culturing neurons to segregate the processes from somata (Dieterich et al., 2010; Swanger et al., 2013; Taylor et al., 2013) and physically cutting them from the somata (Huber et al., 2000; Aakalu et al., 2001) or microperfusing drugs locally to minimize the contribution from the somata (Dieterich et al., 2010). These methods required sparse or guided growth of neurons, which limit applications in vivo. Others have used photoconvertible fluorescent proteins to examine axonal protein synthesis (Leung and Holt, 2008). Here, we minimized the contribution of somatically synthesized proteins in neuronal processes by interfering with microtubule-based transport using colchicine, which has been shown to block somatic transport of newly synthesized proteins (Fischer and Schmatolla, 1972). Although colchicine can block transport of RNP granules (Shan et al., 2003; Antar et al., 2005), we identified conditions in which microtubule structure is transiently and reversibly disrupted without affecting RNP localization in neural processes or protein synthesis in intact animals. With brief colchicine treatments, FUNCAT can be used to visualize locally synthesized proteins in neural processes in vivo. The tectal neuropil includes tectal cell dendrites and local axons, afferent inputs, and radial NPC processes extending to the pia. All of these processes include RNPs (Leung and Holt, 2008; Bestman and Cline, 2009). We measured AHA-labeling intensity within an ROI defined by an NβT mask to capture signals only in neuronal processes in the tectal neuropil and exclude signal from NPCs. By combining these strategies with a visual conditioning protocol shown previously to induce protein synthesis-dependent behavioral plasticity, we documented a visual conditioning-dependent increase in neuronal local protein synthesis in intact animals.
FMRP is thought to regulate synaptic plasticity by suppressing local translation of its target mRNAs (Weiler et al., 2004; Darnell and Klann, 2013; Sidorov et al., 2013). Previous studies have shown that both FMRP knockdown and knock-out increase newly synthesized proteins by ∼20% (Qin et al., 2005; Dölen et al., 2007; Osterweil et al., 2010). We found 15–20% increase in newly synthesized protein in cell body layers of neurons and NPCs with FMRP knockdown and ∼60% increase in local protein synthesis in the neuropil, indicating that FMRP regulates protein synthesis in both somata and processes in neurons and NPCs. The increase in local translation we observe is larger than reported in the knock-out mouse, possibly because our knockdown is acute. Visual conditioning and FMRP knockdown produced similar increases in local protein synthesis, and exposing FMRP knockdown animals to visual conditioning did not further increase local protein synthesis. We examined visual conditioning-induced behavioral plasticity in FMRP knockdown animals to see whether FMRP knockdown animals can still respond to visual conditioning with induced behavioral plasticity. The ability to track changes in avoidance behavior over time enabled us to investigate the dynamics in behavior plasticity. We found that FMRP knockdown animals are able to induce behavioral plasticity in response to visual conditioning, but the plasticity degrades over the following 24 h. These data indicate that FMRP knockdown does not impair protein synthesis necessary for induction of visual condition induced plasticity (Shen et al., 2014), for instance by saturating local protein synthesis machinery. They further indicate that FMRP is required for maintenance of the visually induced behavioral plasticity. Our data support a model in which visual conditioning-induced protein synthesis is required for conditioning-induced behavior plasticity and suggest that FMRP normally limits the synthesis of a Degrader protein, so that plasticity is maintained in the presence of FMRP. In the absence of FMRP, visual conditioning induces behavioral plasticity, comparable with conditioned control animals, but synthesis of Degrader protein, which is no longer suppressed by FMRP, then prevents the maintenance of plasticity (Fig. 9). This idea is distinct from the model of how FMRP is thought to affect mGluR-mediated LTD, in which mGluR activity induces the synthesis of proteins required to maintain LTD, and FMRP limits their synthesis to cap the magnitude of LTD. In the absence of FMRP, synthesis of LTD proteins is not limited, and, consequently, the magnitude of mGluR-mediated LTD is greater than in controls (Huber et al., 2002; Bear et al., 2004). Application of this model to our system would result in greater magnitude behavioral plasticity than seen in CMO-treated animals. The common element of both models is that FMRP modulates the magnitude of long-lasting plasticity by limiting the synthesis of proteins that regulate the maintenance of plasticity (Fig. 9).
In summary, with improved spatiotemporal resolution of FUNCAT in intact animals, we showed that local protein synthesis increases in response to a behavioral plasticity-inducing visual conditioning protocol and demonstrate directly that FMRP knockdown increases protein synthesis in neuronal processes in vivo. Furthermore, using a visual avoidance assay, we demonstrate that FMRP is required to maintain long-term visuomotor behavioral plasticity. Our findings provide strong support for a role for FMRP in local protein synthesis-dependent behavioral plasticity and shed light on how behavioral plasticity mechanisms are regulated by FMRP through local protein synthesis.
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant EY011261, Salk National Eye Institute Core Grant P30 EY019005, and an endowment from the Hahn Family Foundation (H.T.C.). We thank members of the Cline laboratory for discussion and critical comments on this manuscript.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Hollis T. Cline, Dorris Neuroscience Center, Kellogg School of Science and Technology, Scripps Research Institute, 10550 North Torrey Pines Road, La Jolla, CA 92037. Cline{at}scripps.edu