Abstract
Neurons respond to specific features of sensory stimuli. In the visual system, for example, some neurons respond to motion of small but not large objects, whereas other neurons prefer motion of the entire visual field. Separate neurons respond equally to local and global motion but selectively to additional features of visual stimuli. How and where does response selectivity emerge? Here, we show that wide-field (WF) cells in retino-recipient layers of the mouse superior colliculus (SC) respond selectively to small moving objects. Moreover, we identify two mechanisms that contribute to this selectivity. First, we show that input restricted to a small portion of the broad dendritic arbor of WF cells is sufficient to trigger dendritic spikes that reliably propagate to the soma/axon. In vivo whole-cell recordings reveal that nearly every action potential evoked by visual stimuli has characteristics of spikes initiated in dendrites. Second, inhibitory input from a different class of SC neuron, horizontal cells, constrains the range of stimuli to which WF cells respond. Horizontal cells respond preferentially to the sudden appearance or rapid movement of large stimuli. Optogenetic reduction of their activity reduces movement selectivity and broadens size tuning in WF cells by increasing the relative strength of responses to stimuli that appear suddenly or cover a large region of space. Therefore, strongly propagating dendritic spikes enable small stimuli to drive spike output in WF cells and local inhibition helps restrict responses to stimuli that are both small and moving.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT How do neurons respond selectively to some sensory stimuli but not others? In the visual system, a particularly relevant stimulus feature is object motion, which often reveals other animals. Here, we show how specific cells in the superior colliculus, one synapse downstream of the retina, respond selectively to object motion. These wide-field (WF) cells respond strongly to small objects that move slowly anywhere through a large region of space, but not to stationary objects or full-field motion. Action potential initiation in dendrites enables small stimuli to trigger visual responses and inhibitory input from cells that prefer large, suddenly appearing, or quickly moving stimuli restricts responses of WF cells to objects that are small and moving.
Introduction
Visual motion is an important sensory cue (Frost, 2010). Global motion of the entire visual field, along with vestibular signals and motor-related corollary discharge, provides information about self-movement and helps animals monitor and stabilize their trajectory through space. Local (object) motion facilitates detection and tracking of other animals and often triggers shifts in spatial attention and associated orienting movements.
Where and how does the visual system encode object motion? One possibility is that object motion is detected as early the retina, where a subset of retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) respond to differential motion of their receptive field center and surround (Olveczky et al., 2003; Zhang et al., 2012). However, these cells, like RGCs that respond to motion onset or reversal (Schwartz et al., 2007; Chen et al., 2013) or RGCs that are tuned to movement speed and direction (Oyster, 1968), also respond to the sudden appearance/disappearance of stationary stimuli and therefore do not specifically signal object motion.
One synapse downstream of the retina, wide-field (WF) cells in the superficial superior colliculus (sSC) are potentially well suited to selectively encode information about object motion rather than sudden changes in the environment or global motion. WF cells respond weakly (if at all) to large, suddenly appearing stimuli or full-field drifting gratings. Conversely, they respond comparatively strongly to small, slowly moving stimuli presented anywhere within a large region of space (Gale and Murphy, 2014).
A variety of mechanisms might contribute to response selectivity in WF cells. First, some response properties of WF cells could reflect properties of the input that they receive from retina or cortex (Gabriel et al., 2012; Hunter et al., 2013; Preuss et al., 2014). Second, active dendrites or other intrinsic properties could enhance selectivity of spike output relative to weakly selective input (Smith et al., 2013). Third, untuned inhibition could enhance selectivity by narrowing the range of tuned excitatory input that crosses the spike threshold (Isaacson and Scanziani, 2011). Finally, tuned inhibition or other mechanisms could generate selectivity de novo. For example, in the zebrafish optic tectum and mouse visual cortex, inhibitory interneurons that pool excitatory input over a wide region prefer large stimuli and contribute to opposing selectivity for small stimuli in cells that they target (Del Bene et al., 2010; Adesnik et al., 2012).
Here, we identify mechanisms underlying response selectivity in WF cells. First, by comparing responses to a variety of visual stimuli, we show that translational motion of small objects most strongly drives spike output in WF cells; full-field motion is an ineffective stimulus on its own and strongly suppresses responses to object motion. We then demonstrate that individual dendrites of WF cells can independently initiate strongly propagating dendritic spikes, and that nearly all spike output in response to visual stimuli is driven by such spikes. Last, we show that response selectivity in WF cells is at least partly explained by the inhibitory input that they receive from horizontal cells, the predominant (if not exclusive) class of GABAergic neurons in the sSC. These horizontal cells respond preferentially to many characteristics of visual stimuli to which WF cells respond poorly and hyperpolarizing them broadens size tuning and reduces movement selectivity in WF cells.
Materials and Methods
Mice.
All procedures were approved by the Janelia Research Campus Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. CB57BL/6J mice of either sex were 6–18 weeks old at the time of all in vitro and in vivo electrophysiological recordings. For some experiments, we used the following transgenic mice: Ntsr1-GN209-Cre (Gerfen et al., 2013) crossed to Ai32 (Madisen et al., 2012), vGAT-ChR2 (Zhao et al., 2011), or Gad2-Cre (Taniguchi et al., 2011).
In vitro electrophysiology, imaging, uncaging, and optogenetics.
Four-hundred-micrometer-thick parasagittal brain slices were cut with a vibratome (Leica) in chilled cutting solution containing the following (in mm): 60 sucrose, 83 NaCl, 25 NaHCO3, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 2.5 KCl, 0.5 CaCl2, 6 MgCl2, 20 d-glucose, 3 Na pyruvate, and 1 ascorbic acid. Slices were transferred to warm (34°C) cutting solution, which was then allowed to cool to room temperature. Approximately 60 min after cutting, slices were transferred to artificial CSF (ACSF) containing the following (in mm): 125 NaCl, 25 NaHCO3, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 2.5 KCl, 1.3 CaCl2, 1 MgCl2, 20 d-glucose, 3 Na pyruvate, and 1 ascorbic acid for recording (at 32°C) or further storage (at room temperature).
Whole-cell current-clamp recordings were made with glass pipettes filled with the following (in mm): 134 K gluconate, 6 KCl, 4 NaCl, 10 HEPES, 2 MgATP, 0.4 NaGTP, 10 tris phosphocreatine, 0.05 Na Alexa Fluor 594 hydrazide, and in some experiments 2 QX-314. Electrode resistance was 3–8 MΩ. Membrane voltage was amplified 50×, low-pass filtered (4 kHz cutoff) with a Multiclamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices), and digitized at 50 kHz with an ITC-18 data acquisition interface (Heka).
For Ca2+ imaging experiments, 0.1 mm Oregon green BAPTA-1 (OGB1) was included in the pipette internal solution. An arbitrarily shaped line crossing one or more dendritic segments was scanned with 920 nm laser light via high-speed galvometers (Prairie Ultima). The line-scan period was 1.1–4.3 ms.
During two-photon glutamate uncaging experiments, 8.33 mm MNI-glutamate in ACSF was pressure ejected from a glass pipette positioned at the surface of the slice above the uncaging location. Laser pulses (720 nm) of 0.2 ms duration were delivered at each of 13–25 sites on the distal dendrite of a WF cell with 0.2 ms between each pulse/site. No response was observed to laser pulses in the absence of MNI-glutamate.
To express channelrhodopsin-2 (ChR2) in RGCs, we injected 1 μl of AAV-2.1-syn-ChR2-GFP into each eye and brain slices were prepared 4–5 weeks later. ChR2 was activated with 1 ms LED flashes (470 nm peak emission) delivered through a 63× objective. Synaptic responses were abolished after bath application of the Na+ channel blocker TTX (0.5 μm) or a combination of the AMPA and NMDA receptor antagonists NBQX (10 μm) and AP5 (50 μm), respectively.
To express ChR2 or ArchT in horizontal cells, we injected 20 nL of AAV-2.1-syn-ChR2–2a-GFP or AAV-2.1-syn-ArchT-GFP into each of two sites bilaterally in the SC of Gad-Cre mice. Coordinates (in millimeters: anterior from lambda, lateral from midline, and depth) were 0.3, 0.3, 1.0 and 0.1, 0.8, 1.0. Recordings were performed 4–5 weeks after virus injection.
In vivo electrophysiology, juxtacellular labeling, and optogenetics.
Mice were anesthetized via intraperitoneal injection of urethane (1.5 g/kg). Body temperature was maintained with a warm blanket under the animal. A craniotomy was made over the right SC and a plastic head holder was attached to the skull. For extracellular recordings, a glass electrode filled with 0.9% NaCl and 1–1.5% neurobiotin was lowered into the right SC. Current pulses were used to monitor changes in pipette resistance. When a cell was encountered, pipette pressure (20–30 mbar) was released and a loose seal (<20 MΩ) formed. Extracellular voltage signals were filtered (0.1–4 kHz) and sampled at 20 kHz. Whole-cell recordings were obtained in vivo using the same internal solution described for in vitro experiments. Series resistance was 30–100 MΩ and action potentials always overshot 0 mV. For some recordings, an acid-etched optical fiber (9 μm core) was positioned in the recording pipette ∼200–300 μm from the tip, which enabled us to deliver light that activated ChR2 or ArchT.
Visual stimuli were back-projected on a screen at 120 frames/s with a modified digital light-processing projector and a SugarCube LED illuminator (Edmund Optics). The screen was 12.1 cm from the mouse's left eye and subtended 15–100° laterally from directly in front of the mouse and 10° below and 40° above the mouse's eye. Spatial receptive fields were mapped by presenting upward and downward moving circular spots at varying azimuth and leftward and rightward moving spots at varying elevation; subsequently, stationary stimuli were presented at the subjectively chosen center of the spatial receptive field and moving stimuli crossed this location (Gale and Murphy, 2014). Background luminance was constant for the duration of each experiment. For one set of experiments, background was 0.2 cd/m2 and spot stimuli were 48 cd/m2; extracellular recordings with these particular light levels include WF and horizontal from our previous study (Gale and Murphy, 2014). In separate experiments, background luminance was 24 cd/m2 and spots were 0.2 cd/m2; 16 and/or 48 cd/m2 spots were also presented in a subset of these experiments. For random checkerboard stimuli, dark and light squares were 0.2 and 48 cd/m2, respectively, and the luminance between trials was 24 cd/m2. The random checkerboard was stationary for 0.5 s before and after stimulus movement. The size (pixels) and speed (pixels/second) of circular spots and checkerboard squares were adjusted as a function of their position to maintain constant angular size and speed.
To express ArchT in horizontal cells, we injected 10 nL of AAV-2.1-ArchT-GFP into each of two sites in the right SC of Gad-Cre mice (same coordinates as in vitro experiments). Recordings were performed 4–5 weeks after virus injection. An optical fiber was positioned inside the recording electrode ∼300 μm from the tip. The 594 nm laser power was 2–5 mW at the tip of the optical fiber.
For morphological identification of SC cell types, neurobiotin was electroporated into the cell after recording visual responses. Positive current pulses were applied at 2 Hz with a 50% duty cycle and increasing amplitude (0.5–8 nA) until the recorded cell began to fire action potentials entrained to the current steps. Mice were perfused and parasagittal sections (50 μm) containing the right SC were cut with a vibratome. Neurobiotin cell fills were visualized with Alexa Fluor 488- or Alexa Fluor 594-conjugated streptavidin and imaged with a Zeiss confocal microscope.
Cell type identification.
WF and other SC cell types were identified in vitro via their distinct electrophysiological properties as described previously (Gale and Murphy, 2014). Cells directly relevant to this study were combined with a larger dataset of 886 sSC neurons. Eleven electrophysiological parameters were calculated for each cell and hierarchical clustering into four groups was performed using Euclidean distance in 11-dimensional space and Ward's linkage criterion.
Similarly, WF and horizontal cells were identified in vivo via clustering in eight-parameter morphological space as described previously (46 WF and 37 horizontal cells; Gale and Murphy, 2014) or by responses to blue light emitted from an optical fiber in the recording electrode in transgenic mice with cell-type-specific expression of ChR2 (Ntsr1-GN209-Cre × Ai32 mice for 68 WF cells and VGAT-ChR2 mice for 41 horizontal cells; see Results and Fig. 1).
Data analysis.
Comparing the response of a cell to stationary or quickly moving stimuli versus slowly moving stimuli is nontrivial because the stimulus is changing for a longer duration in the latter case and therefore typically elicits a greater total number of spikes. Because responses to the onset and/or offset of stationary stimuli or to quickly moving stimuli are brief, we chose to define each cell's response as the peak of the trial-averaged spike density function for each stimulus. Spike density functions were calculated by convolving spike times for each trial with a Gaussian filter (SD, 20 ms) and then averaging across trials for each stimulus type. Using a longer window duration to calculate response magnitude increases the movement selectivity of WF cells and decreases their preferred speed (Gale and Murphy, 2014).
Best/preferred size or speed was defined as the spot diameter or speed that elicited the strongest response. Size or speed tuning width was the integral of the size or speed tuning curve divided by its peak. A surround suppression index was calculated as the difference between the maximum response and the response to the largest spot (32° diameter) divided by the maximum response. Movement selectivity index was the difference between the maximum response to a moving stimulus (optimal size and speed) and the maximum response to a stationary stimulus divided by the sum of these responses.
Elliptical 95% confidence regions in spike baseline/onset potential space (see Fig. 6B,E) were estimated using the principal components for all spikes from each cell in this space. The two eigenvalues accounting for the most variance determined the length of the major and minor ellipse axes (a normal distribution of the same variance was used to translate these values to a 95% confidence level) and the corresponding eigenvectors determined the orientation of the axes. The calculated confidence regions contained 92–100% of spikes for each cell.
To compare size tuning width, surround suppression, speed tuning width, and movement selectivity for laser on and laser off conditions (for ArchT and control experiments shown in see Fig. 8C), we performed two-sided Wilcoxon signed-rank tests. Statistical significance was considered p < 0.05/4 for the four comparisons for each population (ArchT and control).
Results
Identification of WF cells
The sSC contains at least four cell types with distinct morphological and physiological properties. In previous work and for much of this study, WF and other cells were electroporated with neurobiotin after characterization of visual responses and distinguished according to quantitative morphological properties (Gale and Murphy, 2014). For some experiments in this study, we used an alternative method to target WF cells directly. Ntsr1-GN209-Cre mice, in which Cre-recombinase is expressed in WF but not other sSC cell types, were crossed to Ai32 mice to enable Cre-dependent ChR2 expression (Madisen et al., 2012; Gerfen et al., 2013; Gale and Murphy, 2014).
To confirm that WF cells express ChR2 selectively in Ntsr1-GN209-Cre × Ai32 mice, we first recorded responses of WF and other SC cell types to ChR2 activation in brain slices. WF cells exhibited sustained spiking for the full duration of stimulation with blue light (n = 6; Fig. 1A1). All other cells spiked once at the onset of light or did not spike at all (n = 3 narrow-field, n = 5 stellate, and n = 4 horizontal cells; Fig. 1A2); these responses, but not those of WF cells, were abolished by coapplication of the iontotropic glutamate receptor antagonists NBQX and AP5. Synaptic responses were not observed in non-WF cells when ChR2 was expressed in WF cells via virus injection in the sSC, so they likely originate from axons of Cre-expressing cells outside of the sSC (Gale and Murphy, 2014).
Similar to our observations in vitro, WF cells recorded in vivo (and identified post hoc by their morphology) showed sustained firing for the duration of stimulation with blue light (n = 7), whereas other cells spiked once at the onset of stimulation or did not respond (n = 3 narrow-field, n = 3 stellate, and n = 2 horizontal cells; Fig. 1B). Therefore, sustained spiking in response to blue light enabled immediate and reliable identification of WF cells.
WF cells respond most strongly to slowly moving objects
We previously characterized the selectivity with which WF cells responded to stationary or moving bright spots of varying sizes on a dark background (Gale and Murphy, 2014). WF cells responded preferentially to a small subset of the stimuli presented— in particular, when the stimulus was small and moving slowly. Such stimuli mimic a limited subset of the stimuli that a mouse might encounter in the wild; for example, slow movement of a small, nearby object or faster movement of a larger, distant object.
The appearance and radial expansion of a dark spot may represent a more behaviorally relevant stimulus: the approach of a predator. How do WF cells respond to such stimuli? We presented expanding dark or light spots on a gray background that simulate an object approaching at constant speed; the expansion rate is a nonlinear function of the object size/speed ratio (Fig. 2A1). Like some cells that respond selectively to approaching objects in insects and pigeons (Sun and Frost, 1998; Gabbiani et al., 1999), the time of the peak response in WF cells was linearly related to the size/speed ratio of the approaching object corresponding to a particular angular size. Responses to dark objects were stronger than those to light objects of equal absolute contrast or lower contrast dark objects. However, responses to expanding spots were similar in strength or much weaker than responses to spots of optimal size and speed moving across the screen (Fig. 2A2). Therefore, approaching objects drive spike output in WF cells equally or less effectively as translational object motion.
Perhaps the most salient aspect of both expanding and translating stimuli is the contrast at the moving border between the stimulus and background. The selectivity with which neurons respond to visual stimuli can vary as a function of stimulus contrast (Levitt and Lund, 1997; Kapadia et al., 1999; Sceniak et al., 1999, 2002; Pack et al., 2005; Priebe et al., 2006). To investigate how size tuning and movement selectivity WF cells changes as a function of background luminance and object contrast, we compared responses to light spots on a dark background (Gale and Murphy, 2014) with responses to light and dark spots of varying contrast on a lighter (gray) background.
As for expanding stimuli, dark spots elicited stronger responses than light spots of equal absolute contrast. The preferred spot size for both dark and light spots on a gray background was larger than observed for light spots on a darker background (Fig. 2B). Regardless of the background luminance and stimulus contrast, however, the degree of surround suppression generated by the largest stimulus (32° diameter) was the same (Fig. 2B2, inset). Speed tuning and movement selectivity were also unchanged (Fig. 2C,D) and similar results were obtained with lower contrast dark spots that elicited weaker responses. Therefore, the fundamental features of WF cell visual responses, including surround suppression and preferential responses to slowly moving stimuli, are preserved across a range of stimulus and background luminance levels.
The results presented thus far demonstrate that WF cells respond best to small, moving stimuli. However, some WF cells respond at least half-maximally to stimuli much larger than their preferred size. It was thus unclear whether WF cells might encode information about full-field motion in addition to object motion. To compare responses to object and background motion directly, we presented random checkerboard patterns consisting of 1° light and dark squares that maintained the same luminance throughout the duration of the stimulus (Fig. 3A). A full-field “background” pattern was stationary or moved coherently in a particular direction. On some trials, we concurrently presented a smaller, independently moving “patch” of random checkerboard indistinguishable from the background. The patch is invisible except when moving differentially from background and appears to humans as a square that “pops out” (Frost et al., 1988).
WF cells responded vigorously to moving patches when the random checkerboard background was stationary; the patch size eliciting the largest response was typically between 4° and 16° (Fig. 3B). Therefore, object motion elicits responses in WF cells even when object texture and mean luminance do not differ from background. In comparison, responses to background (full-field) motion alone were rare and weak (Fig. 3C,D). Further, background motion strongly suppressed responses to motion of an optimally sized patch. The suppressive effect of background motion was weaker when patch motion was most distinct; that is, when there was a large difference in object/background speed and when the patch and background moved in opposite directions (Fig. 3C,D). These results suggest that WF cells most likely encode information about object motion rather than movement of the entire visual field (such as optic flow).
Together, our results show that WF cells respond most strongly to slowly moving objects. The remaining sections below explore the mechanisms underlying the visual response properties of WF cells. How do cells with broad dendritic arbors respond preferentially to stimuli covering a relatively small region of space? What mechanisms enhance responses to small, slowly moving stimuli and/or suppress responses to stimuli that appear suddenly, move quickly, or cover a large region of space?
Distal dendritic branches of WF cells control spike output independently via strongly propagating dendritic spikes
WF cells respond best to small, moving stimuli anywhere within a large region of visual space. The anatomical basis for their large spatial receptive field is a broad dendritic arbor sampling retinotopically organized input. The length and thin caliber of these dendrites, however, could significantly constrain the amount/pattern of input necessary to trigger action potentials. In the absence of active amplification, postsynaptic potentials triggered by a small visual stimulus in a distal dendritic branch might be too severely attenuated to evoke an action potential at the axon (which originates near the soma). Previous studies suggest that sodium spikes initiated in the dendrites of WF cells can propagate strongly to the soma (Endo et al., 2008). Here, we build on this result by showing that: (1) restricted activation of any small, distal region of the dendritic arbor is sufficient to trigger somatic spikes; (2) separate branches of the dendritic arbor act independently to control spike output; and (3) spike output in response to visual stimuli is almost entirely driven by spikes initiated in dendrites.
Previous studies of dendritic spikes in WF cells relied on electrical stimulation of afferent axons or pressure ejection of glutamate over a large region (Endo et al., 2008). To confirm and extend these results, we used in vitro two-photon glutamate uncaging to investigate whether input restricted to a small, distal dendritic region can drive spike output in WF cells. Uncaging MNI-glutamate at 13–25 spots near a distal dendritic branch (375–475 μm from the soma; n = 9 locations, 5 cells) triggered either a small, subthreshold depolarization (0.9–1.9 mV) or one or more full-amplitude action potentials recorded at the soma (Fig. 4A1).
Action potentials evoked by glutamate uncaging differed markedly from those evoked by somatic current injection, emerging rapidly from the baseline potential without any noticeable proceeding depolarization (Fig. 4A2). This was true regardless of whether the baseline potential was near the resting potential or hyperpolarized >15 mV via somatic current injection. To quantify this observation, we calculated the potential at which the rate of change in membrane potential (dV/dt) crossed a threshold of 50 mV/ms (spike onset potential) and the average dV/dt just preceding (0.5–1 ms) spike onset (prespike slope). Action potentials evoked by glutamate uncaging always had a onset potential within a few millivolts of the baseline potential and a prespike slope near zero (Fig. 4A3,A4). Similar results were observed for every distal uncaging site, including multiple distinct locations (up to three) on the same cell. These results suggest that synaptic input localized to any distal branch is sufficient to initiate strongly propagating dendritic spikes.
Similar spikes were observed in WF cells in response to endogenous glutamate released from the axons of RGCs expressing ChR2 (Fig. 4B). Action potentials elicited in other SC cell types by the same stimulus, by comparison, exhibited depolarized onset potentials (similar to spikes driven by somatic current injection) and much faster prespike slopes (Fig. 4C). Therefore, unlike WF cells, spike output in other SC cell types appears to be driven by axosomatic integration of subthreshold EPSPs.
The strong coupling of dendritic and axosomatic spikes in WF cells prevented direct observation of dendritic spike potentials at the soma independently of a corresponding axosomatic spike; we did not observe smaller amplitude spikelets even when the soma was hyperpolarized >15 mV. To separate dendritic and axosomatic spikes in WF cells, we added the sodium channel blocker QX-314 (2 mm) to the pipette internal solution. In this condition, somatic current injection evoked a single spike overshooting 0 mV, but not sustained spiking (WF cells normally sustain spiking faster than 100 spikes/s) or smaller amplitude spikelets. Conversely, synaptic input provided by ChR2-expressing RGC axons evoked 10–20 mV spikelets or smaller subthreshold EPSPs (Fig. 4D). As with dendritic spikes recorded with normal internal solution, spikelets could still be evoked when the soma was hyperpolarized >15 mV from rest. On some occasions, spikelets were sufficiently depolarizing to initiate an axosomatic spike. These results confirm that WF cell dendrites initiate strongly propagating spikes that, under normal conditions, reliably evoke axonal spike output.
Even if axosomatic integration of spatially distributed synaptic input is not essential for generating spike output in WF cells, it does not necessarily follow that each dendritic branch operates independently of others to influence spike output; spikes generated in one dendrite could propagate to other dendrites and influence subsequent spike initiation or propagation (Colbert et al., 1997; Golding and Spruston, 1998). To determine the degree to which action potentials back propagate in WF cells, we filled cells with the fluorescent Ca2+ indicator OGB1 and imaged dendritic Ca2+ signals associated with action potentials elicited by brief pulses of somatic current injection. Peak change in fluorescence (dF/F) differed little as we imaged increasingly distant locations from the soma (n = 11 cells, 2–9 locations per cell 25–400 μm from the soma), suggesting that action potentials back propagate throughout the entire dendritic arbor (Fig. 5A). Nonetheless, the effect of back propagating action potentials (bAPs) on dendritic spike generation was brief. The interspike interval between bAPs and synaptically evoked spikes, which reflects both the interval between bAPs and ChR2 stimulation (“pulse interval”) and the synaptic response latency, was ∼7 ms for the shortest pulse interval that reliably evoked dendritic spikes (2 ms pulse interval plus 5 ms response latency; Fig. 5B). The abbreviated influence of bAPs on dendritic spike initiation ensures that dendritic spiking in each dendritic branch occurs largely independently of spiking in other branches.
Previous bouts of synaptic input might also influence the ability of WF cell dendrites to generate spikes. In WF cells of the avian optic tectum, for example, strong and prolonged (lasting seconds) synaptic depression was proposed to underlie phasic signal transduction at inputs to an individual dendritic branch and therefore selective responses to stimuli that move across several branches (Luksch et al., 2004). However, in mouse WF cells, we did not observe this powerful synaptic depression. In our experiments, a broad swath of ChR2-expressing retinal axons was simultaneously activated by light pulses delivered through the microscope objective. Dendritic spikes were reliably evoked by sequential light pulses separated by as little as 20 ms in all WF cells (Fig. 5C).
Visual responses in WF cells are nearly exclusively driven by dendritic spikes
To what degree does dendritic action potential generation underlie visual responses of WF cells in vivo? To answer this question, we analyzed spikes evoked by visual stimuli during whole-cell recordings of WF and, for comparison, other sSC cell types in vivo. In WF cells (n = 7), baseline subthreshold membrane fluctuations were small (SD, 0.5–0.8 mV) and spikes evoked by visual stimuli emerged from an otherwise little changed or slightly hyperpolarized potential (Fig. 6A). Phasic inhibitory potentials were observed in response to the onset and/or offset of a stationary spot, particularly when the spot was large. Similar to spikes elicited by glutamate uncaging on distal dendrites of WF cells in vitro (Fig. 4B), spike onset potentials were close to the baseline potential (Fig. 6B) and prespike slopes were near zero (Fig. 6C). Therefore, nearly all visually driven spikes in WF cells appear to initiate in dendrites.
In comparison, in non-WF cell types recorded in the sSC (n = 10 horizontal and n = 5 stellate cells), subthreshold membrane potential fluctuations before visual stimuli were larger (SD, 1.2–3.9 mV) and during visual stimuli action potentials emerged on top of large, slow depolarizing responses (Fig. 6D). Similar to synaptic responses of these cell types in vitro (Fig. 4D), spike onset potentials were several millivolts above the baseline potential before visual stimulus presentation (Fig. 6E) and prespike slopes were positive (Fig. 6F).
Inhibitory SC neurons contribute to surround suppression and movement selectivity in WF cells
Dendritic spike generation in WF cells facilitates responses to small objects located anywhere within a large region of visual/dendritic space, but this leaves unanswered why responses to larger stimuli are relatively weak. Similarly, sequential activation of individual dendrites does not explain movement selectivity in mouse WF cells (Luksch et al., 2004). Indeed, moving stimuli elicit more (total) spikes than stationary stimuli in both WF cells and other SC cell types. WF cells are unique in exhibiting movement selectivity at very short time scale as well (briefer than required for slowly moving stimuli to activate several branches; Gale and Murphy, 2014).
Two observations led us to consider the possibility that size tuning and movement selectivity in WF cells is generated by suppressing responses to large and/or suddenly appearing stationary stimuli rather than enhancing responses to small and/or slowly moving stimuli. First, the visual stimulus features that most effectively elicited responses in sSC GABAergic neurons (Endo et al., 2003; Gale and Murphy, 2014; “horizontal cells”) were the opposite of those to which WF cells responded. Whereas WF cells responded most strongly to relatively small and slowly moving stimuli, horizontal cells responded best to the sudden appearance or brisk motion of larger stimuli (Fig. 7A–C). Also unlike WF cells, horizontal cells responded strongly to full-field, random checkerboard patterns, particularly when stimulus motion was fast (Fig. 7D). Second, horizontal cells inhibit WF cells directly. In SC slices from Gad2-Cre mice in which we injected virus coding for Cre-dependent ChR2 expression, brief flashes of blue light elicited spikes in horizontal cells and IPSPs in WF cells (Fig. 8A).
The opposing selectivity of visual responses of WF and horizontal cells suggests that horizontal-cell-mediated inhibition might constrain the stimuli to which WF cells respond. To test the influence of horizontal cell activity on size tuning and movement selectivity in WF cells directly, we recorded visual responses of WF cells while optogenetically inhibiting horizontal cells. Virus enabling Cre-dependent ArchT (Han et al., 2011) expression was injected into the sSC of Gad2-Cre mice (Fig. 8B,C). Cell-type specificity of ArchT expression was confirmed in brain slice experiments. ArchT activation hyperpolarized 13/14 horizontal cells (Fig. 8B) and had no effect on the membrane potential of other cell types (0/10 WF, 0/8 narrow-field, and 0/14 stellate cells).
For in vivo experiments, ArchT was activated via 594 nm light emitted from an etched optical fiber positioned inside the recording electrode ∼300 μm from the tip. Visual responses in WF cells were stronger on randomly interleaved trials in which ArchT was activated in horizontal cells for the duration of the visual stimulus (“laser on”) relative to control trials (“laser off”). The relative increase in response strength was particularly pronounced for stimuli that were large and/or stationary (Fig. 9A,B); accordingly, size tuning broadened and movement selectivity decreased (Fig. 9C).
In some cases, activating ArchT in horizontal cells unveiled WF cell responses to the onset or offset of stationary stimuli that did not exist under control conditions; nonetheless, in all cells, the component of the response that was strongest (“on” or “off”) did not change. Some cells recorded with a black background also exhibited an extended response to the offset of large, bright stimuli (Fig. 9A3); these responses were more common when horizontal cells were inhibited (4/8 cells), but also occurred in control conditions (1/8 cells). Delayed responses to the offset of large stimuli are also observed in other cell types in the sSC (unpublished observation) and may reflect disinhibition after strong activation of the receptive field surround.
Across the population of WF cells, activation of ArchT in horizontal cells substantially weakened surround suppression (mean 90% increase in size tuning width and 0.36 absolute decrease in surround suppression index; p < 0.01); in most cases, the stimulus size that elicited the strongest response also increased (5/8 cells for each background luminance). Increased responses to stationary stimuli relative to moving stimuli resulted in a 0.36 absolute decrease in movement selectivity index (p < 0.001). In comparison, activation of ArchT only moderately broadened speed tuning (40% increase in speed tuning width; p < 0.01) and, in control experiments in which there was no ArchT expression in the SC, laser stimulation had no effect on visual responses (Fig. 9C, far right). Therefore, tuned inhibition from horizontal cells suppresses responses of WF cells to large stimuli and to the sudden appearance/disappearance of stationary stimuli.
Discussion
Response selectivity is a fundamental property of sensory system neurons. Here, our results demonstrate how intrinsic properties and local synaptic input contribute to response selectivity of sSC neurons signaling object motion. Small, slowly moving objects trigger strongly propagating dendritic spikes in WF cells. Therefore, stimuli anywhere within the broad spatial receptive field of WF cells can reliably generate spike output in the absence of “summing” input from multiple dendrites at the soma. The range of stimuli that can trigger spikes is constrained by local inhibition from horizontal cells, which respond best to the sudden appearance or rapid motion of large stimuli. Other mechanisms (discussed below) also likely contribute to selectivity of visual responses in WF cells.
Strong propagation of dendritic spikes in WF cells enables spatially restricted synaptic input to reliably trigger axonal spike output. This highly localized synaptic integration is the primary, and perhaps exclusive, mechanism by which spike output is determined in response to physiological stimuli in WF cells. This mode of operation differs from neurons in which weakly propagating spikes contribute variable depolarization at the soma that can either trigger action potentials or summate with input from other dendrites (Golding and Spruston, 1998; Müller et al., 2012). Even relatively strongly propagating dendritic spikes in other neurons can fail to evoke a somatic spike when the soma is hyperpolarized by current injection or inhibitory synaptic input (Chen et al., 1997; Oesch et al., 2005; Sun et al., 2014; Trenholm et al., 2014). We did not observe spikelets suggestive of dendritic spike propagation failures in WF cells even when the soma was substantially hyperpolarized (we cannot, however, rule out propagation failures in the most distal dendritic branches; Sivyer and Williams, 2013). WF cells are most similar to globus pallidus neurons in which input targeting distal dendrites evokes all-or-none, strongly propagating dendritic spikes (Hanson et al., 2004).
Inhibition of horizontal cells, which prefer large, quickly moving stimuli, had a strong effect on size tuning in WF cells, but only moderately broadened their speed tuning (Fig. 9B2,C2). One possible explanation is that the excitatory input that WF cells receive is broadly size tuned but relatively more narrowly tuned for (slow) speed. In this scenario, horizontal cell inactivation could strongly disinhibit WF cells during presentation of quickly moving stimuli but, in the absence of strong excitatory input, have little impact on their spike rate.
A potential mechanism underlying tuning for slow speeds in WF cells is that spikes initiated in one dendrite back-propagate to other dendrites in the line of object motion and diminish the probability of spike generation; slowly moving objects would evade this effect more than quickly moving objects. The brief refractory period that we measured, however, suggests that this is an unlikely explanation for speed tuning. The magnification factor of the mouse sSC retinotopic map is ∼60° or 80°/mm for elevation and azimuth, respectively (Cang et al., 2008). WF cells respond poorly to stimuli moving faster than 100°/s; at this speed, visual stimuli move just 0.7° through visual space (approximately the resolution of mouse vision) represented by <12 μm of the sSC retinotopic map during the relative refractory period of 7 ms. The dendritic arbor of WF cells, by comparison, extends nearly 1 mm across the retinotopic map. A more likely possibility is that the range of object speeds to which WF cells respond is limited by characteristics of their retinal input.
We also cannot rule out that WF cells to some degree “inherit” other characteristics of their response selectivity from retinal or cortical input. Indeed, the powerful role of local inhibition in generating response selectivity in WF cells contrasts with other visual system circuits in which selectivity is largely determined by biases in the excitatory input that each cell receives rather than tuned inhibitory input (Bock et al., 2011; Isaacson and Scanziani, 2011; Katzner et al., 2011; Ko et al., 2011). RGCs that are tuned to small stimuli and suppressed by global motion may influence the receptive field properties of WF cells; however, these cells differ from WF cells in their robust responses to the onset and offset of stationary stimuli and it is not known which sSC cell types they target (Olveczky et al., 2003; Zhang et al., 2012). WF cells, like all sSC cell types other than horizontal cells, are “speed tuned” in the sense that their preferred stimulus temporal frequency increases proportionally to increases in spatial frequency (Gale and Murphy, 2014). This property has not been reported in retina and may reflect cortical input (Priebe et al., 2006; Andermann et al., 2011). Cortical feedback might also regulate the magnitude and/or selectivity of responses in WF cells in a manner that depends on the relative impact of cortical synapses onto WF and horizontal cells (Zhao et al., 2014).
Our experiments do not rule out the possibility that changes in visual responses of WF cells during inhibition of horizontal cells were partially mediated by effects by indirect effects on other SC cell types and/or their anatomical targets. For example, at least some horizontal cells project to the lateral geniculate nucleus (Gale and Murphy, 2014; Bickford et al., 2015), which is a major source of excitatory input to visual cortical areas that target the sSC. More precisely targeted manipulation of horizontal-cell-mediated inhibition of WF cells is required to resolve this issue. Together, however, the opposing selectivity of visual responses in horizontal and WF cells and the effects of suppressing horizontal cell activity both point to an important role for horizontal cells in shaping response selectivity in WF cells.
WF cells project bilaterally to the lateral posterior nucleus of thalamus (LP), which is bidirectionally connected with visual cortex but receives little, if any, retinal input. In primates, inactivation of the pulvinar (LP homolog) strongly suppresses visual responses of cortical neurons (Purushothaman et al., 2012) and both LP or SC inactivation produce deficits in spatial attention (Lovejoy and Krauzlis, 2010; Zénon and Krauzlis, 2012; Zhou et al., 2016). In mice, LP neurons have large spatial receptive fields and may signal local divergences in self-generated global motion created by movement of external objects (Roth et al., 2016). By responding most strongly when the motion of object and background are the most distinct (Fig. 3), WF cells appear to be well suited to contribute to this computation. Object motion is a salient sensory cue that often signals the presence of other animals. Therefore, the detection of object motion by WF cells and their targets in LP may have a powerful influence on cortical circuits, spatial attention, and behavior.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the Howard Hughes Medical Institute. We thank Reza Behnam for sharing virus; Sarah Lindo for stereotaxic injections; John Macklin and Katie Bittner for help with optical fibers; Susan Michael for assisting with histology; and Abby Person, Corbett Bennett, Sonja Hofer, and Spencer Smith for helpful comments.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Samuel D. Gale, Allen Institute for Brain Science, 615 Westlake Avenue, Seattle, WA 98109. samg{at}alleninstitute.org.