Abstract
Aging compromises the frontal, striatal, and medial temporal areas of the reward system, impeding accurate value representation and feedback processing critical for decision making. However, substantial variability characterizes age-related effects on the brain so that some older individuals evince clear neurocognitive declines whereas others are spared. Moreover, the functional correlates of normative individual differences in older-adult value-based decision making remain unclear. We performed a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in 173 human older adults during a lottery choice task in which costly to more desirable stakes were depicted using low to high expected values (EVs) of points. Across trials that varied in EVs, participants decided to accept or decline the offered stakes to maximize total accumulated points. We found that greater age was associated with less optimal decisions, accepting stakes when losses were likely and declining stakes when gains were likely, and was associated with increased frontal activity for costlier stakes. Critically, risk preferences varied substantially across older adults and neural sensitivity to EVs in the frontal, striatal, and medial temporal areas dissociated risk-aversive from risk-taking individuals. Specifically, risk-averters increased neural responses to increasing EVs as stakes became more desirable, whereas risk-takers increased neural responses with decreasing EV as stakes became more costly. Risk preference also modulated striatal responses during feedback with risk-takers showing more positive responses to gains compared with risk-averters. Our findings highlight the frontal, striatal, and medial temporal areas as key neural loci in which individual differences differentially affect value-based decision-making ability in older adults.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Frontal, striatal, and medial temporal functions implicated in value-based decision processing of rewards and costs undergo substantial age-related changes. However, age effects on brain function and cognition differ across individuals. How this normative variation relates to older-adult value-based decision making is unclear. We found that although the ability make optimal decisions declines with age, there is still much individual variability in how this deterioration occurs. Critically, whereas risk-averters showed increased neural activity to increasingly valuable stakes in frontal, striatal, and medial temporal areas, risk-takers instead increased activity as stakes became more costly. Such distinct functional decision-making processing in these brain regions across normative older adults may reflect individual differences in susceptibility to age-related brain changes associated with incipient cognitive impairment.
Introduction
Stimuli that predict reward evoke higher frontostriatal activity than stimuli that induce expectations of less reward or higher cost (Schultz et al., 1997; Breiter et al., 2001; Knutson et al., 2001; Preuschoff et al., 2006). Moreover, whereas expected outcomes minimally modulate frontostriatal activity relative to baseline, unexpected negative outcomes reduce activity (negative prediction error) in these regions, while unexpected positive outcomes enhance it (positive prediction error). The fidelity of such Bayesian-like choice value prediction and feedback-based adjustment in these frontal, striatal, and medial temporal areas (Koster et al., 2015) is crucial for rapid and accurate assessment of environmental stochastic dangers and opportunities. Compromises to these areas can bias individuals to inappropriately engage in risk-aversive or risk-taking behaviors leading to suboptimal outcomes.
With aging, structural, neurobiological, and functional changes affect the frontal, striatal, and medial temporal areas and reduce the selectivity of neural responses to different stimuli (Goh and Park, 2009; Eppinger et al., 2011; Goh, 2011). Thus, neural representations of stimulus reward and cost may be less veridical in older than in younger individuals, culminating in suboptimal decisions. Indeed, older adults showed less neural sensitivity than younger adults to different levels of anticipated monetary losses (Samanez-Larkin et al., 2007). Greater age was also associated with greater variability of nucleus accumbens (NAcc) activity, possibly reflecting noisier value signaling related to greater risk taking (Samanez-Larkin et al., 2010).
However, there are substantial individual differences in the effects of aging on the brain and cognitive processing. Even within cognitively normal older adults, some individuals show greater age-related declines in executive processing and related frontal processes that are predictive of later mild cognitive impairment or Alzheimer's disease (Goh et al., 2012, 2013; Beason-Held et al., 2013). In the Iowa Gambling Task, young adults show better reward learning than older adults as a group, but at least a subset of older adults show learning comparable to young adults (Denburg et al., 2005). Treatment with a dopamine agonist increased frontostriatal dopamine responsiveness that correlated with better reward learning in some older adults, whereas others were unaffected or even showed poorer learning (Chowdhury et al., 2013). Apart from cognitive and reward learning differences, individual variability in value representations during choice may also explain why some older adults engage in more risk-taking behaviors while others are more risk-averse. Nevertheless, the neural activities underlying individual differences in older-adult value representations remain unclear.
In this study, we characterize the functional neural correlates of value processing underlying the influences of age and individual differences that dissociate risk-averters from risk-takers in a large sample of cognitively normal older adults from the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging (BLSA). Participants underwent a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) lottery choice task experiment in which they accepted or declined costly to more desirable stakes depicted by negative to positive expected values (EVs) over trials. We assessed acceptance rates of each participant's choices over these trials, which varied in value magnitude and probability, and indexed risk preference using the decision threshold (DT) EVs at which an individual switched from declining to accepting stakes (Dohmen et al., 2011). We expected greater age to correspond with suboptimal acceptance rates and altered functional responses to increasing EVs. In addition to age effects, we expected that neural representations of EVs in the frontal, striatal, and medial temporal areas of the reward system would vary across individuals such that risk-averters and risk-takers evince dissociated neural responses to stimulus values during choices that correlate with their decision behaviors. Specifically, risk-averters should show greater sensitivity to increasing EVs relative to more risk-neutral counterparts, whereas risk-takers, being more partial to experiencing gains under risk and less motivated by safe options, should show greater sensitivity to decreasing EVs. Finally, we evaluated how risk preferences during choice also modulate reward processing during feedback. Individuals with more extreme risk preferences should evince reduced responses to unexpected loss outcomes (“disappointment”) or enhanced responses to unexpected wins (“surprise”).
Materials and Methods
Participants.
Two hundred and twenty-eight participants underwent this cross-sectional fMRI lottery choice task experiment conducted from 2011 to 2013 as part of the ongoing neuroimaging substudy of the BLSA (Resnick et al., 2000). All participants were cognitively normal (no mild cognitive impairment or dementia) at the time of the fMRI experiment based on prior annual regular case conferences with consensus diagnosis using neurological assessments, and on clinical and neuropsychological data (Kawas et al., 2000; Driscoll et al., 2006). The local institutional review board approved the research protocol for this study, and written informed consent was obtained from all participants. Of these participants, 173 entered the final sample (Table 1) after these exclusions: the data of four participants were unavailable due to technical faults; eight participants had brain image artifacts; 10 participants had excessive head motion (>2 mm translation or >2° rotation); two participants experienced no losses in the lottery choice task throughout an entire run; one participant gave no responses to one condition; and 30 participants showed atypical decision behaviors, including accepting more low than high probability trials, or having DTs that were outliers based on Grubb's test, which call into question understanding of the task.
fMRI lottery choice task stimuli and procedure.
EV was manipulated in a fMRI lottery choice task experiment (Fig. 1). There were 72 lottery trials, each with a choice phase and a feedback phase. For the choice phase, the top text depicted the magnitude of points at stake (M), and the bottom texts depicted the percentage probability of winning (P) or losing (1 − P) the stakes. M and P comprised the EV across trials according to the equation EV = PM + (1 − P) (−M). Higher positive EVs denote more desirable stakes, for example when a trial has high M and high P (close to 1). By contrast, lower negative EVs denote more costly stakes, for example when a trial has high M but low P (close to 0). We applied six discrete combination choice conditions of ranges of P and M (HH: high P, high M; HL: high P, low M; MH: middle P, high M; ML: middle P, low M; LH: low P, high M; LL: low P, low M; 12 trials per condition; Table 2) to facilitate pseudorandom ordering of trial EVs as well as predetermined outcomes (see below). Specifically, trials were distributed in the event-related fMRI experiment such that ≤3 in a row had the same choice condition or predetermined outcome. Two lists of stimuli with different M and P but similar ranges of EVs were generated for counterbalancing across participants (91 saw list A; 82 saw list B) to minimize list-specific effects. All trials were unique so that no number combinations of P and M were repeated throughout the study.
For the feedback phase, the top text depicted the outcome points for that trial and the bottom number showed the current accumulated points. There were three feedback conditions: gain, loss, or null (decline to respond or null response). Outcomes were stochastically predetermined based on the given choice probabilities with the restriction that no choice condition predetermined outcomes could have only winning or only losing trials (Table 2). Note that this resulted in choice conditions in which the actual number of win trials did not correspond to the range of probabilities stated in the stimuli. Nevertheless, the discretization of trials into six conditions was internal to our analysis and participants were only instructed to make decisions based on trial-wise information.
Over two functional runs in the scanner (each 360 s duration), choice and feedback stimuli were presented, interleaved by fixation crosses with jittered intertrial intervals. Twenty second fixation intervals preceded and ended each run to facilitate baseline signal estimation. Participants were instructed to accumulate as many points as possible by deciding during the choice phase of each trial whether to accept or decline the given stakes and indicate their decision with assigned button presses. Choice stimuli remained on screen for a full 4 s even after responses. For accepted trials, feedback was either a gain or a loss, based on the predetermined outcomes. For declined trials, a null feedback was given. Trials in which participants took >4 s to respond also resulted in null feedback plus a textual reminder to respond. Participants were trained with a practice version of the task before scanning to ensure that they understood their task goal and were familiar with the range of probabilities and points magnitudes used. Stimuli were presented as black text on white background using E-Prime software (Psychology Software Tools) via back-projection onto a screen in the scanner room, which participants viewed through a mirror mounted on the head coil. All participants were able to read the stimuli either without aid or with corrected vision.
Brain imaging protocol.
Brain imaging data were acquired in a 3T Philips Achieva system (Philips Healthcare) with an eight-channel head coil. For each participant, 180 functional scans were acquired in each run using an echo-planar imaging sequence with 37 axial slices parallel to the anterior–posterior commissural plane [voxel size, 3.00 × 3.00 × 3.00 mm; in-plane matrix size, 80 × 72; repetition time (TR) = 2 s; echo time (TE) = 30 ms; flip-angle, 75°]. A high-resolution T2 image coplanar to the functional scans was also acquired for coregistration, with 50 slices (voxel size, 0.94 × 0.96 × 3.00 mm; in-plane matrix size, 256 × 218; TR = 3 s; TE = 100 ms; flip-angle, 90°). Finally, a high-resolution T1 magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo image was acquired for normalization to standard space with 170 sagittal slices (voxel size, 1.20 × 1.00 × 1.00 mm; in-plane matrix size, 256 × 240 mm; TR = 6.5 ms; TE = 3.1 ms; flip angle, 8°).
Lottery choice task behavioral analysis.
Behavioral analyses were conducted using R version 3.0.2 (www.r-project.org). Since the safe outcome in all trials would always decline with no gain or loss (zero EV), individual indices of risk preference were estimated as the DT that denotes the certainty equivalent (Dohmen et al., 2011) of zero EV as follows. For each participant, logistic regression was first applied on the binary behavioral responses (accept, 1; decline, 0; null response trials excluded) as the dependent variable over the range of negative to positive EVs as the independent variable across trials using the model log(Response) = β0 + β1EV + ε. The DT was then computed as −(β0/β1), which is the estimated EV when the participant's likelihood of accepting is 0.5. In our experiment, a risk-neutral agent would have a DT (or certainty equivalent) of 0, which is exactly the EV when the probability of winning or losing is 0.5. Higher to lower DTs then indicate certainty equivalents greater or lower than EV = 0 when probability of winning or losing was 0.5, reflecting risk-averse to risk-taking preferences, respectively. To visualize continuous associations between risk preference and behavioral and brain responses, modeled data in respective analyses were plotted over four incremental levels of DT. These levels were set as the mean DTs of the lower [D̅T̅ (SD) = −6.6 (3.7); N = 60] and upper [D̅T̅ (SD) = −0.7 (0.7); N = 61] halves of participants with DT ≤ 0 for very risk-taking (VRT) and moderately risk-taking (MRT) levels, respectively, and the lower [D̅T̅ (SD) = 0.3 (0.3); N = 26] and upper [D̅T̅ (SD) = 2.8 (2.4); N = 26] halves of participants with DT > 0 for moderately risk-averse (MRA) and very risk-averse (VRA) levels, respectively.
Mean acceptance rates and response times for each of the six discrete conditions were obtained for each participant. However, to formally validate the effects of EV, age, and individual risk preferences (indexed by DT) in modulating lottery choice task behavioral performance, group-level mixed-model analyses were computed for the trial-wise acceptance rates and response time data. Specifically, for acceptance rates over the range of EVs used, a logistic mixed model was implemented with binary responses from all trials of all participants as the dependent variable and trial-wise EV, age, DT, and the interactions of EV with age and EV with DT as fixed effects of interest. Trial-wise variances (computed as P[M − EV]2 + [1 − P] [(−M) minus EV]2) may also modulate uncertainty about trial-wise EVs in relation to risk processing (Preuschoff et al., 2006) and were included as a fixed-effect covariate. Other fixed-effect covariates were current accumulated scores and sex, and participant was a random effect. We then specifically tested the significances of regression coefficients for the effects of EV, age, DT, and the specified interactions based on this logistic mixed model. A similar analysis was applied to the response time data for all trials except that a linear mixed model was used and EV was modeled to the quadratic term, based on inspection of trends in the data.
fMRI data preprocessing.
Analyses of functional brain data were conducted using SPM8 (Statistical Parametric Mapping, Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, UK). For each participant, functional images were preprocessed with motion realignment and slice-time correction. T2 anatomical images were coregistered to the functional images, then T1 images to the coregistered T2 images. T2 images for five participants were unavailable so that coregistration of functional to T1 images were applied directly for these cases. Functional images were then normalized to a study-specific template generated from participants' coregistered T1 images using diffeomorphic anatomical registration through exponentiated linear algebra (Ashburner, 2007). Normalized functional images were then affine-transformed to the standard Montreal Neurological Institute template space and smoothed with a 3D 8 mm Gaussian kernel.
fMRI analysis of neural responses during choice and feedback.
We applied a standard first-level analysis on the preprocessed functional images to estimate mean neural responses for each of the six discrete conditions for each participant. This involved a general linear model that included delta function onset regressors (duration, 0 s for this event-related design) for each of the six choice conditions (HH, HL, MH, ML, LH, LL) and each of the three feedback conditions (gain, loss, null), which were then convolved with the hemodynamic response function (HRF). Covariates in this model included one regressor for trial-wise variances and one for current accumulated scores, which were generated by convolving the HRF with a choice onset regressor modulated by variances and current scores (with no orthogonalization), respectively. Six movement parameters were also included as head-motion covariates, resulting in a total of 17 regressors in this model.
For formal testing of age and individual differences in neural processing in the lottery choice task, we sought to identify brain regions that linearly changed neural responses with trial-wise EV as well as regions in which risk preferences modulated these linear response changes. We reasoned that linear responses reflect neural activity that increased (or decreased) with increasing value, at least in terms of rank order. Nonlinear responses that may reflect transitivity violations or distortions are not presently evaluated in this study. Note that we excluded participants with behavioral performances that had these counter-indications (see above). Estimates of each participant's neural sensitivity to EV during the choice phase were obtained by submitting the preprocessed functional images to a model-based first-level analysis. This model included one regressor for choice onsets, one for trial-wise EV (during choice), and one for each of the three feedback conditions (gain, loss, null). There was also one covariate regressor for trial-wise variances, one for current accumulated scores, and six head-movement parameters resulting in a total of 13 regressors in this model. Regressors for choice onset and feedback were delta function onsets convolved with the HRF only. Trial-wise EV, variance, and current score regressors were based on the HRF-convolved choice-onset regressor modulated by the respective trial-wise variables (with no orthogonalization). This analysis resulted in whole-brain voxelwise estimates of each participant's neural sensitivity to EV, controlling for the other variables, with higher positive voxel coefficients indexing greater sensitivity to increasingly desirable EVs and more negative voxel coefficients indexing greater sensitivity to increasingly costly EVs. These individual whole-brain EV estimates were then passed into a second-level group analysis using age and risk preference (DT) as independent variables, with sex as a covariate. The intercept of this second-level analysis yields brain areas in which trial-wise EV modulated neural activity at the group level after accounting for age and individual differences in risk preference. The effect of age in the model reveals brain areas in which neural sensitivity to trial-wise EV variation differed from younger to older adults (ranging from 43 to 91 years), accounting for individual differences in risk preference and the group mean response to EV. Finally, the effect of DT reflects brain areas in which individual risk preference modulated neural EV sensitivity after accounting for age and the group mean response to EV. Tests for significant contrasts were based on a voxel threshold of p < 0.001 (uncorrected) with cluster extent correction of >20 voxels, except for the striatal and medial temporal regions. For these smaller structures, we created a subcortical volume mask that included the caudate, putamen, globus pallidus, thalamus, parahippocampal, hippocampal, and amygdala regions (based on the Automatic Anatomical Labeling atlas; Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002). Within this mask, we adopted a small volume correction approach and evaluated voxels surpassing p < 0.005 (uncorrected) with cluster extent correction of >15. Both criteria are within recommended standards to relate cognitive variables to brain functional measures (Lieberman and Cunningham, 2009) and also comparable to those used in previous studies examining value-based decision processing in these regions (Samanez-Larkin et al., 2007). Both of these significance criteria also fulfilled a whole-brain α of p < 0.05 using Monte Carlo simulation with 10,000 iterations (Slotnick et al., 2003, 2011).
Regions-of-interest (ROIs) for further analyses were defined as contiguously significant voxels within spheres of 10 mm radius around peak contrast voxels. To interrogate ROI EV-related responses during the choice phase in greater detail, the response estimates for choice onset and trial-wise EV from each participant's model-based first-level analysis were extracted. These individual choice and EV response estimates were then grouped based on the four levels of risk preference. For each risk preference level, the mean choice and mean EV response estimates across individuals in that group were then used to derive the modeled linear neural responses over the range of EVs in the task. Estimated responses to the six choice conditions from the standard first-level analysis were also extracted from ROIs to aid visual comparison of measured to modeled results.
Feedback response estimates from the first-level model-based analysis were further used to perform a conjunction analysis for voxels that simultaneously showed significant positive gain response and significant negative loss response relative to baseline fixation with a threshold of p < 0.001 (cluster size, >20). This conjunction was applied to identify brain areas involved in canonical prediction errors during feedback, which we defined as regions that generated both positive signals for rewards and negative signals for punishments. Gain and loss responses were then extracted from ROIs identified based on the conjunction analysis. These were then passed into linear models with DT, its quadratic term, and age as independent variables, and sex as a covariate. We then examined statistical significance of the regression coefficients associated with DT in the models to evaluate how risk preference modulated responses to gain and loss outcomes during feedback.
Results
Individual differences in lottery choice task behavioral performance
Across participants, the distribution of DTs was right-skewed with mean <0, indicating a general risk-taking behavioral preference in our sample of older adults [D̅T̅ (SD) = −2.07 (4.21), t(172) = −6.47, p < 0.001; Fig. 2]. Applying a linear regression model with DT as the dependent variable and age (mean-centered) as the independent variable with sex as a covariate, yielded no further effect of greater age in this older-adult sample in modulating the overall risk-taking preference. Critically, there were substantial individual differences in risk preferences reflected by the range of negative to positive DTs (minimum, −16.4; median, −0.80; maximum, 10.9] with 52 of 173 individuals showing DT > 0.
Consistent with reduced neural sensitivity to different values, greater age was associated with more suboptimal decision behavior characterized by higher acceptance rates at more costly negative EVs and lower acceptance rates at more desirable positive EVs (Fig. 3A,D). Moreover, acceptance rates increased as EVs increased and also as risk taking increased (VRA < MRA < MRT < VRT), particularly for more costly negative EVs (Fig. 3B,E). A logistic mixed model at the group level formally validated how age and risk preference modulated acceptance rates over the range of EVs presented in the lottery choice task (see Materials and Methods). Log odds of acceptance rates increased with increasing EV (EV: Z = 39.7, p < 0.001), decreased with greater age (age: Z = −2.62, p < 0.01), and decreased with higher risk-aversion (DT: Z = −14.0, p < 0.001). Importantly, the effect of EV on acceptance rates was also modulated by age (EV*age: Z = −5.90, p < 0.001) and risk preference (EV*DT: Z = 14.6, p < 0.001). These interactions were due to decreasing influence of EVs on participant decisions as age or risk taking increased.
Response time model analysis showed a significant convex quadratic overall effect of EVs such that response time was estimated to be slowest at EV = −12.6 on average, becoming faster with more extreme EVs (EV2: t(11860) = −15.0, p < 0.001; Fig. 3C,F). Although there was no significant direct effect of risk preference, the convex quadratic effect of EVs on response time was reduced with increasing risk taking (EV2*DT: t(11860) = −4.05, p < 0.001; EV*DT: t(11850) = 7.93, p < 0.001) and approached a more linear trend with slower response times as EVs decreased (EV: t(11860) = −13.0, p < 0.001). Greater age was associated with an overall slowing of response times (age: t(177) = 2.86, p < 0.01), which did not interact with EV or risk preference.
We considered that individual risk preferences might reflect differences in cognitive ability as well. To examine this, we further applied a linear model with DT (up to the quadratic term) as a variable explaining Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores with age and sex as covariates. Significant negative effects of DT (t(132) = −2.67, p < 0.01) and DT2 (t(132) = −2.15, p < 0.05) indicated a convex quadratic relationship between risk preference and MMSE performance (Fig. 2). Specifically, MMSE score was estimated to be maximum when DT = −7.4 and was lower with more extreme risk preferences, particularly for risk-averters.
Brain regions showing common EV sensitivity during choice evaluation
A number of regions in the frontal, striatal, and medial temporal areas were sensitive to EV variation at the group level regardless of age and individual differences in risk preference (Fig. 4A; Table 3). Specifically, we found increasing blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal as trials became more desirable with increasing EVs (positive EV sensitivity) in the right inferior frontal gyrus (pars orbitalis), left caudate, and a region spanning the ventral anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex, as expected. In addition, we observed increasing BOLD signal as trials became more costly with decreasing EV (negative EV sensitivity) in the bilateral putamen, thalamus, right middle frontal gyrus, left inferior frontal gyrus (pars opercularis), and hippocampus.
Brain regions where age and individual differences in risk preference modulated EV sensitivity during choice evaluation
Greater age was associated with greater negative EV sensitivity (increasing BOLD signal as trials became more costly) in the bilateral middle frontal, left superior medial frontal, and middle cingulate regions (Fig. 4B; Table 4). Critically, there was a distinct set of regions within the frontal, striatal, and medial temporal areas in which individual differences in risk preference significantly dissociated neural sensitivity to EVs (Fig. 5; Table 5). These regions included the anterior cingulate region (overlapping but posterior to that observed for the group response to EV above), medial superior frontal gyrus, bilateral parahippocampal gyri (entorhinal region), right orbitofrontal gyrus and caudate, and left thalamus and putamen. In all these areas, greater risk-aversion was positively correlated with greater neural EV sensitivity with no regions showing negative associations.
Functional responses in ROIs defined from the above frontal, medial temporal, and striatal regions were plotted as a function of risk preferences to visualize the effect of DT in modulating neural activity across the range of EVs used in the lottery choice task in these brain regions (Fig. 5). For frontal ROIs, we found an overall pattern of positive EV sensitivity at the VRA, MRA, and MRT levels and negative EV sensitivity at the VRT level (VRA, MRA, MRT > 0; VRT < 0; Fig. 5A). Further, whereas the effects of risk preference in medial frontal ROIs were more prominent during positive EVs, effects of risk preference were more prominent during negative EVs in the right orbitofrontal ROI. In parahippocampal ROIs, the VRA and MRA levels showed positive EV sensitivity, with minimal bias at the MRT level, and negative EV sensitivity at the VRT level (VRA, MRA > 0; MRT ≈ 0; VRT < 0; Fig. 5B). Finally, in striatal ROIs, only the VRA level showed positive EV sensitivity, with minimal biases at the MRA and MRT levels, and negative sensitivity at the VRT level (VRA > 0; MRA, MRT ≈ 0; VRT < 0; Fig. 5C).
Overall, these findings demonstrate that during choice evaluation, additional specific frontal, striatal, and medial temporal areas are involved in modulating neural responses across individuals over and above the common EV neural responses in these brain structures. Importantly, relative to their more risk-neutral counterparts, risk-aversive individuals showed accentuated sensitivity to EV in these additional brain areas, whereas risk-takers showed greater responses as decisions became more costly.
Risk preferences modulate striatal prediction error during feedback
Four ROIs fulfilled the conjunction criteria for canonical prediction error during feedback specified as positive responses to gains and negative responses to losses relative to fixation baseline. These included bilateral NAcc and the middle and posterior cingulate gyri. We found no significant effects of age on feedback responses in these four ROIs. Nevertheless, of these ROIs, risk preference significantly modulated feedback responses only in the bilateral NAcc (Fig. 6). Specifically, gain–loss contrasts were minimal for participants with DTs ∼ 0, but increased toward more extreme DTs [(gain–loss)2: left, t(168) = 3.47, p < 0.001; right, t(168) = 2.80, p < 0.01]. This effect of risk preference was driven mainly by increases in positive responses to gains in more risk-taking participants with lower DTs (gain2: left, t(168) = 2.30, p < 0.05; right, t(168) = 2.07, p < 0.05) but minimal effects in negative responses to losses.
Discussion
Our results show that with greater age, value-based decisions become suboptimal with increased frontal responses to costlier choices. Given this, we examined individual differences in value-based decision processing to better understand how risk preferences are associated with functional differences in frontal, striatal, and medial temporal areas. All participants were exposed to the same ranges of desirable to costly EVs, clearly delineated by explicit probabilities of winning (or losing) and magnitudes. Within this context, although participants increased acceptance rates as EVs increased, this sample of middle age and older participants evinced a preference for risk taking as a group. Crucially, while at least a half of the sample still accepted risks when losses were likely, at least a third demonstrated a reluctance to accept risks when gains were likely. This variability in risk preferences was associated with individual differences in frontal, striatal, and medial temporal responses to EV during choice evaluation distinct from general group-level EV-related responses. Specifically, neural activity in risk-averters increased with increasingly desirable EVs, with accentuated sensitivity compared with their neutral counterparts. By contrast, risk-takers increased neural activity with increasingly costly EVs, consistent with a propensity for experiencing gains under risk. Finally, risk preferences modulated prediction error responses during feedback, with risk-takers showing more striatal activation to gains, possibly signaling greater surprise.
Our results are consistent with an increase in neural activity to increasing stimulus value in the frontostriatal areas (Schultz et al., 1997; Breiter et al., 2001; Knutson et al., 2001; Fiorillo et al., 2003; Preuschoff et al., 2006; Tobler et al., 2007; Tom et al., 2007). Such correspondence between neural activity and assigned stimulus values in these regions during choice evaluation reflects a neural mechanism for the comparative representation of anticipated rewards or costs, particularly for the likelihood of occurrence of the reward or cost (Fiorillo et al., 2003). Our findings extend this notion by showing that there are also important individual differences in the correspondence between neural activity in these regions and stimulus values that are associated with differences in relative value comparisons. We also show that increasing potential cost induced increasing neural activity (negative EV sensitivity) in the putamen, thalamus, lateral frontal, and hippocampal areas. This finding contrasts with a previous study that did not detect any negative correlations between brain responses and increasingly negative stakes (Tom et al., 2007), although this may be due to asymmetry in the range of gains and losses, a relatively small sample size, and the lack of outcomes in that study (Canessa et al., 2013; Sokol-Hessner et al., 2013). Our observations of negative EV sensitivity in these regions are consistent with their involvement in emotional regulation, which reduces loss aversion and may therefore increase risk-taking behavior (Knoch et al., 2006; Baumgartner et al., 2011; Sokol-Hessner et al., 2013). Note, risk preference effects on behavior were more pronounced during negative EVs, which might be affected by individual differences in loss aversion. Moreover, increased dorsolateral prefrontal activity is observed in individuals who have a propensity for more costly decisions (Baumgartner et al., 2011). Together, these findings may reflect the engagement of neural resources in some individuals to regulate experienced inconsistency between subjective goals and the options at hand. Our study and other supportive findings (Tobler et al., 2009) buttress this notion in that risk-takers but not risk-averters displayed increasing frontal, striatal, and hippocampal neural activity as decisions were associated with greater costs. Future studies directly evaluating loss aversion or other aspects of decision processing are required to determine the submechanisms operating in these regions that underlie the observed differences.
That older neural decision-processing differences were present during the choice phase suggests individually different value perceptions of stimuli during predictive processing. This is distinct from the effect of declines in feedback learning on the decisions of older adults (Denburg et al., 2005; Mata et al., 2011), as is typically tested with stochastic learning paradigms, such as the Iowa Gambling Task. Indeed, there were no age effects on neural responses during feedback in our task. As demonstrated in a study in younger adults with different decision strategies (Venkatraman et al., 2009), distortion of value information during the choice stage may carry into feedback so that prediction error is different (but not necessarily distorted) across older individuals with different risk preferences. Thus, our findings are consistent with individual differences in age-related effects on frontal, striatal, and medial temporal processing that influence older-adult value processing and affect feedback processing but are distinct from age effects on feedback learning.
Our findings point to a possible functional neural mechanism underlying individual differences in older adults displaying variability in risk-taking behaviors (Denburg et al., 2005) and, possibly, responses to drugs (Chowdhury et al., 2013) that affect neurotransmitter action, such as those that affect the action of dopamine (Wang et al., 1995; Volkow et al., 1998; Kaasinen et al., 2000; Erixon-Lindroth et al., 2005; Dreher et al., 2008). Dopamine efficacy is optimal at moderate levels of synaptic availability but dysregulated at both lower and higher levels (Goldman-Rakic et al., 2000). We speculate that the distribution of neural sensitivity to value in our study reflects a functional correlate of differences in dopamine levels at the lower end of the availability spectrum among older individuals. Specifically, older adults with moderate neural value sensitivity and decision behaviors in line with objective stimuli values may have optimal dopaminergic efficacy. By contrast, older adults with disproportionate neural value sensitivity or who show more responsiveness to costs than rewards may have dysregulated dopamine efficacy associated with noisier signaling (Eppinger et al., 2011). Only the latter, with irregular value sensitivity, should show more accurate value representations when dopamine antagonists or agonists are introduced that improve signal gain accordingly. Indeed, higher frontostriatal dopamine levels correlate with greater willingness to expend effort for gains (more risk-taking; Treadway et al., 2012) and higher frontal activity in young adults (Dreher et al., 2008). However, in older adults, higher midbrain dopaminergic synthesis correlates with lower frontal activity (Dreher et al., 2008), consistent with having lower baseline dopamine levels. Further studies involving direct manipulation of dopamine levels in older adults are necessary to validate this hypothesis.
Many regions in which risk preferences modulated neural value processing in our study have been implicated as initiating loci for neuropathological progressions in various cognitive diseases. For example, medial temporal neurofibrillary tangles and medial frontal and striatal β-amyloid deposition occur in early stages of Alzheimer's disease (Braak and Braak, 1995, 1997). The loss of dopaminergic receptors (Wong et al., 1984; Wang et al., 1995; Volkow et al., 1998; Kaasinen et al., 2000; Erixon-Lindroth et al., 2005; Dreher et al., 2008) along with iron accumulation (which increases oxidative stress; Bartzokis et al., 1997; Pfefferbaum et al., 2010) in the striatal regions are also among the most reliably observed neurobiological changes associated with aging. Interestingly, in our study on cognitively normal older adults, individuals with more extreme risk preferences evinced poorer MMSE performance, with a tendency for those with a slight preference for risk to have the highest scores. Overall, the dissociated frontal, striatal, and medial temporal functional responses between risk-takers and risk-averters with opposite sensitivities to EV in our study may form a basis for a preclinical biomarker of age-related cognitive impairment.
Given the mixed findings on age effects on risk preferences that might stem from differences in decision contexts (Mata et al., 2011), key aspects of our design should be highlighted. First, we used numerical texts to explicitly depict value magnitudes and probabilities in each trial. Second, all information needed to make a decision was available in the stimuli, which were independent across trials, to minimize any involvement of learning and memory. Using any learning of past decision outcomes would be fallacious and a suboptimal strategy in the task. Third, we used mixed stakes that simultaneously included the possibility for wins and losses in every decision fairly across trials. Our design contrasts with studies that involved proxy representations of value, learning, or incorporation of outcome history, or separate losing and winning trials, or only wins and forfeitures. This last point in particular, was important for assessing individual risk preferences in older adults during costly but potentially tempting choices (mixed stakes), which have been associated with increased risk-taking with age (Rutledge et al., 2016). We note that our data are based on the BLSA, which consists of participants who choose to enroll in intensive physical and behavioral assessments and may reflect personalities associated with a risk-taking preference. Ongoing studies are investigating the extent to which these individual differences in frontal, striatal, and medial temporal functional activity are generalizable to other samples.
How a person makes decisions about value reveals much about the ability of his or her brain to regulate and integrate subjective goals with objective information. While we found that age generally compromises value-based decision behavior and neural processing, our study further links individual differences in older-adult risk preferences to differences in frontal, striatal, and medial temporal responses to value information during decision processing. These risk preference-related neural responses in older adults range from hypersensitivity to value in risk-averters to greater sensitivity to costly choices instead in risk-takers. Such patterns of neural responses are obscured in analyses focusing on common patterns of responses in groups of individuals, highlighting the importance of investigating individual differences to understand the neurobiology of decision making in older adults. Altered value representation in this neural system may indicate susceptibility to age-related changes in some older individuals resulting in biased risk preferences that compromise decision-making skills critical for optimal lifestyles in stochastic environments.
Footnotes
This work was supported in part by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute on Aging, National Institutes of Health; by the National Science Council, Taiwan, Grant #102-2410-H-002-004; and by the Ministry of Science and Technology, Taiwan, Grant #103-2410-H-002-082. We also thank Anne M. Farrell and Brian J. White for their conceptual input.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Joshua O. S. Goh, PhD, Graduate Institute of Brain and Mind Sciences, College of Medicine, National Taiwan University, No. 1 Ren-Ai Road, Section 1, RM 1554, Zhongzheng District, Taipei, Taiwan 10051. joshuagoh{at}ntu.edu.tw