Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Brief Communications

Metabotropic Glutamate Receptors Induce a Form of LTP Controlled by Translation and Arc Signaling in the Hippocampus

Hui Wang, Alvaro O. Ardiles, Sunggu Yang, Trinh Tran, Rafael Posada-Duque, Gonzalo Valdivia, Min Baek, Yang-An Chuang, Adrian G. Palacios, Michela Gallagher, Paul Worley and Alfredo Kirkwood
Journal of Neuroscience 3 February 2016, 36 (5) 1723-1729; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0878-15.2016
Hui Wang
1Mind/Brain Institute, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland 21218,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Alvaro O. Ardiles
1Mind/Brain Institute, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland 21218,
2Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia de Valparaiso, Universidad de Valparaiso, Chile,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Sunggu Yang
1Mind/Brain Institute, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland 21218,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Trinh Tran
1Mind/Brain Institute, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland 21218,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Rafael Posada-Duque
1Mind/Brain Institute, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland 21218,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Gonzalo Valdivia
1Mind/Brain Institute, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland 21218,
2Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia de Valparaiso, Universidad de Valparaiso, Chile,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Min Baek
1Mind/Brain Institute, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland 21218,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Yang-An Chuang
3BCMB-Neuroscience Johns Hopkins Medical School, Baltimore, Maryland 21205,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Adrian G. Palacios
2Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia de Valparaiso, Universidad de Valparaiso, Chile,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Adrian G. Palacios
Michela Gallagher
3BCMB-Neuroscience Johns Hopkins Medical School, Baltimore, Maryland 21205,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Michela Gallagher
Paul Worley
4Department of Brain and Psychological Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland 21218, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Alfredo Kirkwood
1Mind/Brain Institute, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland 21218,
4Department of Brain and Psychological Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland 21218, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Alfredo Kirkwood
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Activity-dependent bidirectional modifications of excitatory synaptic strength are essential for learning and storage on new memories. Research on bidirectional synaptic plasticity has largely focused on long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD) mechanisms that rely on the activation of NMDA receptors. In principle, metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGluRs) are also suitable to convert synaptic activity into intracellular signals for synaptic modification. Indeed, dysfunction of a form of LTD that depends on Type I mGluRs (mGluR-LTD), but not NMDARs, has been implicated in learning deficits in aging and mouse models of several neurological conditions, including Fragile X syndrome and Alzheimer's disease. To determine whether mGluR activation can also induce LTP in the absence of NMDAR activation, we examined in hippocampal slices from rats and mice, an NMDAR-independent form of LTP previously characterized as dependent on voltage-gated Ca2+ channels. We found that this form of LTP requires activation of Type I mGluRs and, like mGluR-LTD but unlike NMDAR-dependent plasticity, depends crucially on protein synthesis controlled by fragile X mental retardation protein and on Arc signaling. Based on these observations, we propose the coexistence of two distinct activity-dependent systems of bidirectional synaptic plasticity: one that is based on the activity of NMDARs and the other one based on the activation of mGluRs.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Bidirectional changes of synaptic strength are crucial for the encoding of new memories. Currently, the only activity-dependent mechanism known to support such bidirectional changes are long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD) forms that relay on the activation of NMDA receptors. Metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGluRs) are, in principle, also suitable to trigger bidirectional synaptic modifications. However, only the mGluR-dependent form of LTD has been characterized. Here we report that an NMDAR-independent form of LTP, initially characterized as dependent on voltage-gated Ca2+ channels, also requires the activation of mGluRs. These finding suggest the coexistence of two distinct activity-dependent systems of bidirectional synaptic plasticity: one that is based on the activity of NMDARs and the other one based on the activation of mGluRs.

  • arc
  • bidirectional
  • metabotropic glutamate receptors
  • protein synthesis
  • synaptic plasticity
  • translation

Introduction

The capacity of synapses to undergo lasting increases or decreases in strength in response to activity patterns is thought to be essential for the processes of learning and memory formation. Currently, the most comprehensive (and most studied) models of bidirectional synaptic modification are activity-dependent forms of long-term potentiation (LTP) and long term-depression (LTD) that require the activation NMDA receptors to initiate insertion or removal of AMPA receptors from the synapse (Huganir and Nicoll, 2013). The focus on NMDAR-dependent LTP and LTD (NMDAR-LTP/D) results in part because their molecular mechanisms have been worked out to a great detail, particularly in the CA1 region of the hippocampus, and in part because of the wealth of evidence implicating these forms of plasticity in multiple learning processes (Morris, 2013). In this study, we examined the possibility that the metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGluRs) can also support bidirectional changes in synaptic strength, independently of NMDARs.

It is well established that stimulation of Type I mGluRs (mGluR1 and mGluR5) alone can induce LTD expressed as endocytosis of synaptic AMPA receptors (Lüscher and Huber, 2010). Mechanistically, this mGluR-dependent form of LTD (mGluR-LTD) and NMDAR-LTD are widely different. They can operate on different subunits of the AMPA receptor (Casimiro et al., 2011); and while the induction of mGluR-LTD requires intact protein synthesis and it is under the control of Arc/arg3.1 signaling, the induction of NMDAR-LTD does not (Huber et al., 2000; Park et al., 2008; Waung et al., 2008). Importantly, the dysregulation of mGluR-LTD has clear functional consequences as it has been implicated in learning deficits associated with aging (Lee et al., 2005; Boric et al., 2008; Yang et al., 2013), as well as in neuropathological conditions, including fragile X mental retardation (Huber et al., 2002; Bear et al., 2004) and Alzheimer's disease (Hsieh et al., 2006).

In contrast to the well-characterized mGluR-LTD, the case for mGluR-dependent LTP has been more elusive. Although Type I mGluRs modulate the induction of NMDAR-LTP in multiple synapses (Abraham, 2008), LTP that exclusively requires mGluRs but not NMDARs has only been reported in the subiculum (Fidzinski et al., 2008), where it is typically “masked” by NMDAR-LTD. This paucity of information prompted us to determine whether activation mGluRs can induce not only LTD but also LTP in the absence of NMDAR. An attractive candidate model to examine was a form of LTP that depends on L-type voltage-gated calcium channels (VGCC-LTP) (Grover and Teyler, 1990). VGCC-LTP is typically induced under NMDAR blockade with tetanic stimulation of higher frequency and longer duration than the one used for NMDAR-LTP (Grover and Teyler, 1990). By analogy with mGluR-LTD induction, which also requires stronger stimulation than NMDAR-LTD, we reasoned that the higher induction threshold in VGCC-LTP might reflect the need to build up glutamate at the periphery of the synapse to recruit the largely perisynaptic mGluRs. Our examination of this form of LTP indicated that is indeed dependent on Type I mGluRs and, like mGluR-LTD but unlike NMDAR-LTP, it also requires protein synthesis and Arc signaling.

Materials and Methods

Animals.

Animals used were 4-month and 20-month-old male Long–Evans rats or 2-month-old male Arc/Arg3.1 KO and Fmr1 KO mice. Both mouse lines were kept in a BL6 background. Hippocampal slices (400 μm) were prepared as described previously (Yang et al., 2013) in ice-cold dissection buffer containing the following: 212.7 mm sucrose, 2.6 mm KCl, 1.23 mm NaH2PO4, 26 mm NaHCO3, 10 mm dextrose, 3 mm MgCl2, and 1 mm CaCl2 bubbled with a mixture of 5% CO2 and 95% O2. For recordings in aged rats, we used transcardial perfusion before isolating the brain (Yang et al., 2013). All the procedures were approved by the Johns Hopkins University animal care committee. The slices were recovered for 1 h at room temperature in ACSF as follows: 124 mm NaCl, 5 mm KCl, 1.25 mm NaH2PO4, 26 mm NaHCO3, 10 mm dextrose, 1.5 mm MgCl2, and 2.5 mm CaCl2 bubbled with a mixture of 5% CO2 and 95% O2.

Synaptic responses.

Synaptic responses were evoked with 0.2 ms pulses (10–80 μA) delivered through concentric bipolar stimulating electrodes (FHC) or theta glass micropipettes filled with ACSF, recorded extracellularly in CA1 or CA3 stratum radiatum, and quantified as the initial slope of the field potential. Baseline responses were recorded at 0.033 Hz using a stimulation intensity that evoked a half-maximal response, defined as the maximal response without a population spike (pop-spike). Slices were discarded when the pop-spike appeared in the initial rising phase (an indication of hyperexcitability), when paired-pulse facilitation at a 50 ms interval was less than ∼10% (i.e., response 2/response 1 < 1.1), or when the baseline was not stable (>5% drift). High-frequency tetanus consisted of four 200 Hz epochs (0.5 s) delivered at 0.2 Hz in the presence of an NMDA receptor antagonist (100 μm d,l-APV). Under our experimental conditions, this protocol induces robust LTP in Long–Evans rats in both CA1 (Boric et al., 2008) and CA3 synapses (Yang et al., 2013). Theta burst stimulation (TBS) consisted of 4 or 6 theta epochs delivered at 0.1 Hz. Each epoch, in turn, consisted of 10 trains of 4 pulses (at 100 Hz) delivered at 5 Hz. LTP magnitude was calculated as the average (normalized to baseline) of the responses recorded 50–60 min after conditioning stimulation, corresponding to the early phase of LTP (to distinguish it from late LTP). Drugs included 2-Methyl-6-(phenylethynyl)pyridine (MPEP), LY-367385, (2S)-α-Ethylglutamic acid (EGLU), anisomycin, (2R)-amino-5-phosphonovaleric acid (APV), and nifedipine from Tocris Bioscience; all other chemicals were from Sigma or Fisher Scientific.

Immunoblot analysis.

Western blot was performed using a previously published protocol (Zhang et al., 2015). Slices were solubilized in cold (4°C) PBS buffer containing 1% Triton, mm EDTA, 5 mm EGTA, and inhibitors of proteases (Roche, 11873580001) and phosphatases (Roche, 4906837001). After sonication, the lysates were gently mixed for 30 min and centrifuged at 13,200 × g for 15 min at 4°C. The supernatant was collected and mixed with Laemmli buffer for further immunoblotting.

Samples (3.75 μg) were loaded onto 4%–12% precast gels (Novex, WG1403A) in a scrambled order-to minimize uneven blotting and ran for 2.5 h at 120 V. Blots were transferred overnight to PVDF membranes at 40 V and blocked with 3% BSA in TBST buffer for 1 h. After proper washing, the membranes were incubated with HRP-linked secondary antibodies for 1 h at room temperature. The antibodies used include the following: anti-Arc monoclonal antibody (Dr. Paul Worley, 1:1000), HRP-linked anti-mouse IgG antibody (GE Life Science, NA931V, 1:1500), HRP-linked anti-rabbit IgG antibody (GE Life Science, NA934V), and peroxidase-conjugated anti-β-actin antibody (Sigma, 1: 3000). The developed films were imaged and quantified (at 45 kDa for Arc, and 42 kDa for β-actin) with ImageJ tools (Zhang et al., 2015). All protein bands were normalized to β-actin in the same samples.

Statistical significance.

Statistical significance was evaluated using Prism GraphPad software. The specific tests used were as follows: the two-tailed Mann–Whitney rank test for two sample comparisons and the Kruskal–Wallis test followed by a Dunn's correction for multiple comparisons. Data are mean ± SEM.

Results

The central aim of the study was to determine whether mGluRs could induce LTP in the CA3→CA1 synapses in the absence of NMDAR activation. To that end, we tested whether the NMDAR-independent form of LTP initially characterized as VGCC-LTP requires the activation of mGluRs. Therefore, unless otherwise noted, in these studies we recorded CA3→CA1 synaptic responses as fEPSP in hippocampal slices from adult (2–4 month) Long–Evans rats, and LTP was induced with a high-frequency tetanus in the presence of 100 μm APV to block NMDA receptors (see Materials and Methods).

VGCC-LTP induction requires mGluR stimulation and protein synthesis

We first tested the effects of blocking the activation of mGluRs on VCCG-LTP using a combination of mGluR antagonists known to block mGluR-LTD. The combination included 10 μm MPEP (against mGluR5) and LY-367385 at a high dose (100 μm) that blocks all Type I mGluRs (Volk et al., 2006). As shown in Figure 1A, LTP measured at 60 min after tetanus was substantial (132 ± 4%, n = 10 slices) in control conditions (only APV) but negligible in the presence of all antagonists (APV plus MPEP and LY-12436: LTP = 108 ± 1%, n = 10 slices). These differences were significant (Mann–Whitney: p = 0.0011), indicating that this form of LTP, initially characterized as VCCG-LTP, is indeed an mGluR-dependent form of LTP (mGluR-LTP). Hence, from here on, we refer to it as mGluR-LTP. Next, we asked whether, as in the case of mGluR-LTD (Volk et al., 2006), it is necessary to coapply both antagonists to block mGluR-LTP. We found that mGluR-LTP was not blocked by either MPEP (control: 126 ± 8%, n = 11; MPEP: 123 ± 7, n = 15, p = 0.99) or LY-367385 (control: 133 ± 8, n = 14; LY-12436: 125 ± 5, n = 20; p = 0.500) applied alone (not shown), suggesting that Type I receptors can substitute each other in supporting LTP. On the other hand, the Type II antagonist EGU (10 μm) did not affect mGluR-LTP (control: 128 ± 7, n = 12; EGLU: 134 ± 5, n = 10; p = 0.500). Together, the results strongly suggest that mGluR-LTP depends specifically on the activation of Type I mGluRs.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Induction of non-NMDAR-LTP in hippocampal pathways depends on mGluR and protein synthesis. A, A 200 Hz tetanus delivered in the presence of 100 μm APV induces robust LTP of the CA3→CA1 field EPSPs (open circles), which is blocked by the addition of antagonists to Type I mGluRs (100 μm LY-367385 and 10 μm MPEP; filled circles). B, LTP induced by the 200 Hz tetanus in APV is blocked by the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin (10 μm). Open circles represent control DMSO. Filled circles represent anisomycin. A, B, Superimposed traces represent example experiments. Each trace is the average of 10 responses recorded before (thin traces) and 60 min after the tetanus (thick traces). Calibration: 1 mV, 10 ms. C, D, In slices from aged rats (18–20 months), the 200 Hz LTP is also blocked by mGluR antagonists in both CA3→CA1 synapses (C) and in CA3→CA3 synapses (D). The number of experiments is indicated in parentheses.

A defining feature of mGluR-LTD that distinguishes it from NMDAR-LTD is its strict requirement on normal protein translation (Huber et al., 2000). We examined, therefore, whether this is also the case with mGluR-LTP. We found that incubation with protein synthesis inhibitor anisomicyn (10 μm starting at least 30 min prior collecting baseline and throughout the experiment) severely prevented the induction of mGluR-LTP (Fig. 1B; anisomycin: 108 ± 7%, n = 7; DMSO control: 129 ± 6%, n = 7; p = 0.0011). Together, the results indicate that the induction of mGluR-LTP, like mGluR-LTD, depends on both stimulation of Type I mGluRs and intact protein synthesis.

An important motivation for the present study was the observation that, during aging, the integrity of mGluR-dependent plasticity is critical for maintaining youthful learning performance (Lee et al., 2005; Boric et al., 2008; Yang et al., 2013). Importantly, learning performance in aged individuals correlates positively with the magnitude of LTP induced with VGCC-LTP protocols in both the CA3→CA1 and the CA3→CA3 hippocampal pathways. It was of interest, therefore, to confirm the mGluR dependence of this type of LTP in late aging. We found that, in slices from old rats (20–24 months), the Type I mGluR antagonist combination (10 μm MEP and 100 μm LY 367385) prevents LTP induced with VGCC-LTP protocols in both the CA3→CA1 pathway (Fig 1C: control 124 ± 5%, n = 8; antagonists: 103 ± 1%, n = 9; p = 0.0011) and in the CA3→CA3 pathway (Fig 1D; control, 122 ± 4%, n = 16; antagonist, 104 ± 1%, n = 21; p < 0.001), indicating that the strict mGluR dependence of this type of LTP is maintained during aging.

mGluR-LTP and NMDAR-LTP are not independent in the CA3→CA3 pathway

Previous studies established that the nifedipine-sensitive and APV-sensitive components of LTP induced by the 200 Hz protocols (mGluR-LTP and NMDAR-LTP) are largely additive in both the CA3→CA1 and the CA3→CA3 pathways (Grover and Teyler, 1990; Cavuş and Teyler, 1996; Boric et al., 2008; Yang et al., 2013). However, other types of conditioning revealed nonadditive interactions between these two LTP forms in the CA3→CA1 pathway, where LTP induced with prolonged TBS is partially blocked by nifedipine and completely blocked by APV (Morgan and Teyler, 2001). Therefore, we examined LTP induction with multiple TBS epochs in the CA3→CA3 pathway. As shown in Figure 2A, and confirming previous observations (Boric et al., 2008; Yang et al., 2013), in the CA3→CA3 pathway the standard conditioning of 4 epochs of TBS induced robust LTP that is fully blocked by the NMDAR antagonist APV (control: 118 ± 5%, n = 8; APV: 101 ± 4%, n = 8; p = 0.008). Notably, when we increased the number of TBS epochs to 6, the magnitude of the resulting LTP did not change in the presence of APV (control: 116 ± 5%, n = 21; APV: 115 ± 4%, n = 10; Fig 2B), but the time course did. The presence of APV slowed down the initial development (the first 20 min) of 6TBS-LTP, which became similar to mGluR-LTP. In a similar fashion, the L-type Ca2+ channel blocker nifedipine did not affect the magnitude of 6TBS-induced LTP (nifedipene: 113 ± 1%, n = 8) but changed its time course, which became faster, like NMDAR-LTP. Although none of the drugs applied in isolation affected the final magnitude of 6TBS-LTP, they did block it when coapplied (APV and nifedipine: 101 ± 2%, n = 8). The Kruskal–Wallis test confirmed the significance of the differences between the 4 groups (H[4, 46] = 12.56; p = 0.057), and Dunn's post hoc test confirmed that only the APV and nifedipine group was different from control. Thus, in the CA3→CA3 pathway, 6TBS can induce either mGluR-LTP (when NMDARs are blocked) or NMDAR-LTP (when L-type channels are blocked), and in a nonadditive manner: in control conditions (absence of drugs), the magnitude of 6TBS-LTP was not the sum of mGluR-LTP and NMDAR-LTP; rather, the magnitude of the three forms of LTP was similar. On the other hand, we confirmed the nonadditive interactions of the two types of LTP in the CA3→CA1 pathway where 6TBS-LTP induced a large LTP that was fully blocked by APV and partially blocked by nifedepine (control: 155 ± 6%, n = 29; APV: 98 ± 2%, n = 12; nifedepine: 123 ± 8%, n = 17; H[3, 58] = 30.76; p < 0.0001; Fig. 2C). The observation that the two types of LTP are not additive in both the CA3→CA1 and the CA3→CA3 pathways suggests that they interact at some level.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

TBS induces mGluR-LTP in CA3→CA3 synapses. A, Four TBS epochs (open circles) induce a form of LTP that is completely blocked by 100 μm APV (filled circles). B, LTP induced with 6 TBS epochs (open circles) is not blocked by either 100 μm APV (gray circles) or 10 μm nifedepine (gray triangles), yet it is completely blocked by the coapplication of 100 μm APV and 10 μm nifedepine (black circles). C, In CA3→CA1 synapses, LTP induced with 6 TBS (open circles) is partially blocked by 10 μm nifedepine (black triangles) and completely blocked by 100 μm APV (black circles). The number of experiments is indicated in parentheses.

Role of fragile X mental retardation protein (FMRP) and Arc in the induction of mGluR-LTP

The induction of mGluR-LTD and mGluR-LTP depends on intact protein synthesis and stimulation of Type I mGluRs. These common requirements prompted us to determine whether downstream molecules known to mediate and control mGluR-LTD also play a role in mGluR-LTP. These studies were done in transgenic mouse lines maintained in a BL6 background. Therefore, we confirmed that in BL6 the LTP induced by the 200 Hz protocol in the presence of APV is blocked by mGluR antagonists (MEP + LY: 108 ± 8%, n = 17; control: 135 ± 5, n = 12; p = 0.0059. Not shown), and by the L-type blocker nifedepine (nifedepine: 107 ± 5%, n = 14; control 133 ± 9, n = 19; p = 0.0139. Not shown). First, we examined FMRP, a regulator of dendritic protein synthesis, whose genetic deletion removes the vulnerability of mGluR-LTD to translation blockers and increases its magnitude (Huber et al., 2002; Nosyreva and Huber, 2006; Waung and Huber, 2009). We tested this possibility in the CA3→CA1 synapses of FMRP KO mice. We found that the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomicyn (anisomicyn: 10 μm), which blocks mGluR-LTP in wild-type mice (DMSO control: 123 ± 3%, n = 4 mice, 14 slices; anisomicyn: 100 ± 1%, n = 4,13; Fig. 3A), did not affect it in the FMRP KO mice (DMSO control: 129 ± 3%, n = 4,14; anisomicyn: 127 ± 2%, n = 4,14. Fig. 3B). These differences were significant (H[4, 55] = 31.77; p < 0.0001). The slight increase in the magnitude of mGluR-LTP in the FMRP KO did not reach the conventional significance criteria. Next, we examined the role of the immediate early gene Arc in the KO mice. Previous studies have implicated Arc in AMPAR endocytosis, and its genetic deletion completely blocks mGluR-LTD, but not NMDAR-LTD (Park et al., 2008; Waung et al., 2008). One reasonable expectation is that, to the extent that LTP and LTD are opposite processes, the deletion of Arc, which impairs mGluR-LTD, should enhance mGluR-LTP. Surprisingly while mGluR-LTP was robust and blocked by anisomicyn in the wild types (DMSO control: 126 ± 2%, n = 4,13; anisomicyn: 101 ± 1%, n = 4,14; Fig. 3C), mGluR-LTP was completely eliminated in the Arc KO, where the high-frequency tetanus (in APV) induced only a transient and modest post-tetanic potentiation similar to that observed under protein synthesis blockade (DMSO control: 101 ± 1%, n = 4,13; anisomicyn: 101 ± 1%, n = 4,14, p = 0.858; Fig. 3D). These differences were significant (H[4,54] = 31.77; p < 0.0001).

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Role of FMRP and Arc in the induction of mGluR-LTP in CA1→CA3 synapses. The induction mGluR-LTP with 200 Hz tetanus is blocked by the translation inhibitor anisomycin in control FMRP(+/+) mice (A) but not in FMRP(−/−) mice (B). C, D, Role of Arc. Only the transient potentiation normally observed in Arc(+/+) mice after blockade of protein synthesis (C) is induced in Arc(−/−) mice. Superimposed traces in each represent example experiments. Each trace represents the average of 10 responses recorded before (thin traces) and 60 min after the tetanus (thick traces). Calibration: 1 mV, 10 ms. The number of mice and experiments is indicated in parentheses.

Finally, we used immunocytochemistry to determine whether the 200 Hz tetanus in the presence of APV increases Arc protein in CA1. These experiments were performed in minislices with the CA3/DG subfields removed: half of the minislices were tetanized, whereas paired control minislices received only baseline stimulation. At 25–30 min later, the slices were collected and processed for immunobloting. As shown in Figure 4, Arc protein content was significantly larger in the tetanized slices (tetanized: 0.463 ± 0.028; control: 0.361 ± 0.024, n = 17 pairs, p = 0.0095). Together, these results indicate that mGluR-LTP and mGluR-LTD do share a substantial portion of the initial steps on their induction cascade, including Arc signaling.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Increase in Arc protein after 200 Hz tetanus. Western blot analysis of Arc expression in control (open circle) and tetanized (black circles) slices. Arc levels were relativized to β-actin through densitometry analysis and presented as relative units (RU). Individual data points are laterally displaced for illustrative purposes. Horizontal bars indicate mean ± SEM. *p < 0.01. Top, Example blots from control (C) and tetanized (T) minislices.

Discussion

The present examination of the induction of an LTP form previously described as VGCC-LTP revealed that it depends also on the stimulation of Type I mGluRs, on the integrity of Arc signaling, and on protein synthesis controlled by FMRP. Notably, these mechanistic requirements are also common to the induction of mGluR-LTD (Huber et al., 2002; Nosyreva and Huber, 2006; Park et al., 2008). Thus, in the absence on NMDAR activity, mGluR stimulation cannot only decrease synaptic strength (see Lüscher and Huber, 2010) but also increase it, and both processes involve Arc signaling. Based on these observations, we propose the coexistence of two distinct mechanisms of activity-dependent synaptic plasticity: one reads NMDAR activity and the other one reads Type I mGluR activity.

Previous studies revealed clear differences in the induction cascades of NMDAR-LTP and mGluR-LTP. From the outset, NMDAR-LTP does not require mGluRs (Selig et al., 1995), whereas mGluR-LTP does not require NMDARs (Grover and Teyler, 1990). Downstream from there, the two forms of LTP depend on different sets of initial kinases (Cavuş and Teyler, 1996), although they appear to converge on the MAPK cascade (Kanterewicz et al., 2000). Consistent with the notion of different induction cascades, we found that mGluR-LTP requires intact protein synthesis and Arc signaling, which is not the case in NMDAR-LTP induction. These two processes are crucial for the consolidation of NMDAR-LTP into a long-lasting form of plasticity (Plath et al., 2006; Bramham, 2008) but are not required for its initial induction. In that regard, it would be of interest to determine whether protein synthesis-dependent mGluR-LTP is long lasting. Another remaining open question is how protein synthesis and Arc signaling are required for both mGluR-LTP and mGluR-LTD. Although the issue is puzzling, a common intermediate for LTP and LTD is not unusual. For example, there is evidence that NMDAR-LTP and NMDAR-LTD both depend on the activation of CaMKII (Stevens et al., 1994; Coultrap et al., 2014). Similarly, as mentioned, Arc signaling has been implicated in the induction of mGluR-LTD (Park et al., 2008) and in the maintenance of NMDAR-LTP (Plath et al., 2006). In sum, the differences in mechanism between mGluR-LTP and NMDAR-LTP, and the similarities with mGluR-LTD fit nicely with the notion that NMDARs and mGluRs can activate two distinct systems of synaptic plasticity. In this light, it would be interesting to consider whether mGluR-LTP is also involved in some of the processes where mGluR-LTD has been implicated, such as encoding novelty and learning deficits associated with the fragile X condition (for review, see Bear et al., 2004; Lüscher and Huber, 2010).

Activation of Type I mGluRs can result in either LTP or LTD through a common initial pathway that depends on protein synthesis, FMRP and Arc signaling. This raises the question of what specifies the polarity of mGluR-dependent synaptic plasticity. In the case of NMDAR-dependent plasticity, the prevailing (yet not universal) consensus is that the polarity of the changes is dictated by the magnitude of the initial intracellular Ca2+ signal. The recent demonstration that mGluR-LTD requires an intracellular Ca2+ increase (Kim et al., 2015) renders the Ca2+ magnitude hypothesis a plausible mechanism for mGluR-dependent plasticity also.

The observation that mGluR-LTP and NMDAR-LTP can interact (Fig. 2B) suggests that these two forms of LTP can coexist in the same synapse, or at least in close proximity, like NMDAR-LTD and mGluR-LTD (Oliet et al., 1997) coexist in CA1 synapses. The functional consequences of having two coexisting systems of plasticity remain to be explored. One clear difference between these two systems is that in vitro the induction of mGluR-dependent plasticity requires more presynaptic activity than the induction of NMDAR-dependent plasticity. Induction of mGluR-LTP requires tetanus of higher frequency and longer duration than NMDAR-LTP. Similarly, although both forms of LTD are induced with prolonged low-frequency stimulation, mGluR-LTD is typically induced with twice the stimulation pulses as NMDAR-LTD (Oliet et al., 1997; Huber et al., 2000). These observations open the consideration that these two plasticity systems operate at different levels of neural activity, with mGluR-dependent plasticity being recruited at higher levels of activity. For example, the changes in the correlation between presynaptic and postsynaptic activity are likely computed by the NMDAR-based system when the overall levels of activity are low to moderate. At a higher firing regimen, the NMDAR-based system might saturate and the mGluR-mediated plasticity would be better suited to subserve the function. This two dynamic ranges scenario resonates nicely with the observation that, in the aged hippocampus, which typically expresses high levels of firing rates and excitability, mGluR-dependent plasticity becomes crucial for learning performance (Lee et al., 2005; Boric et al., 2008; Ménard and Quirion, 2012a,b; Yang et al., 2013). In sum, the realization that Type I mGluRs can support bidirectional synaptic plasticity in similar fashion as previously demonstrated for NMDAR-dependent plasticity opens questions that are highly significant for our understanding of learning and memory formation.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by NIH/NIA Grants R01AG034606 to A.K. and PO1-AG09973 to M.G., and Instituto Cientifico Milenio Grant ICM-P09-022-F to A.G.P. We thank Hey-Young Lee for valuable discussions and comments.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Alfredo Kirkwood, Mind Brain Institute, Johns Hopkins University, 338 Krieger Hall, 3400 N. Charles Street, Baltimore, MD 21218. kirkwood{at}jhu.edu

References

  1. ↵
    1. Abraham WC
    (2008) Metaplasticity: tuning synapses and networks for plasticity. Nat Rev Neurosci 9:387, doi:10.1038/nrn2356, pmid:18401345.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Bear MF,
    2. Huber KM,
    3. Warren ST
    (2004) The mGluR theory of fragile X mental retardation. Trends Neurosci 27:370–377, doi:10.1016/j.tins.2004.04.009, pmid:15219735.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Boric K,
    2. Muñoz P,
    3. Gallagher M,
    4. Kirkwood A
    (2008) Potential adaptive function for altered long-term potentiation mechanisms in aging hippocampus. J Neurosci 28:8034–8039, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2036-08.2008, pmid:18685028.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    1. Bramham CR
    (2008) Local protein synthesis, actin dynamics, and LTP consolidation. Curr Opin Neurobiol 18:524–531, doi:10.1016/j.conb.2008.09.013, pmid:18834940.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Casimiro TM,
    2. Sossa KG,
    3. Uzunova G,
    4. Beattie JB,
    5. Marsden KC,
    6. Carroll RC
    (2011) mGluR and NMDAR activation internalize distinct populations of AMPARs. Mol Cell Neurosci 48:161–170, doi:10.1016/j.mcn.2011.07.007, pmid:21807099.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Cavuş I,
    2. Teyler T
    (1996) Two forms of long-term potentiation in area CA1 activate different signal transduction cascades. J Neurophysiol 76:3038–3047, pmid:8930253.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    1. Coultrap SJ,
    2. Freund RK,
    3. O'Leary H,
    4. Sanderson JL,
    5. Roche KW,
    6. Dell'Acqua ML,
    7. Bayer KU
    (2014) Autonomous CaMKII mediates both LTP and LTD using a mechanism for differential substrate site selection. Cell Rep 6:431–437, doi:10.1016/j.celrep.2014.01.005, pmid:24485660.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Fidzinski P,
    2. Shor O,
    3. Behr J
    (2008) Target-cell-specific bidirectional synaptic plasticity at hippocampal output synapses. Eur J Neurosci 27:1111–1118, doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06089.x, pmid:18312585.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Grover LM,
    2. Teyler TJ
    (1990) Two components of long-term potentiation induced by different patterns of affeent activation. Nature 347:477–479, doi:10.1038/347477a0, pmid:1977084.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Hsieh H,
    2. Boehm J,
    3. Sato C,
    4. Iwatsubo T,
    5. Tomita T,
    6. Sisodia S,
    7. Malinow R
    (2006) AMPAR removal underlies Abeta-induced synaptic depression and dendritic spine loss. Neuron 52:831–843, doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.10.035, pmid:17145504.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Huber KM,
    2. Gallagher SM,
    3. Warren ST,
    4. Bear MF
    (2002) Altered synaptic plasticity in a mouse model of fragile X mental retardation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 99:7746–7750, doi:10.1073/pnas.122205699, pmid:12032354.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. ↵
    1. Huber KM,
    2. Kayser MS,
    3. Bear MF
    (2000) Role for rapid dendritic protein synthesis in hippocampal mGluR-dependent long-term depression. Science 288:1254–1257, doi:10.1126/science.288.5469.1254, pmid:10818003.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  13. ↵
    1. Huganir RL,
    2. Nicoll RA
    (2013) AMPARs and synaptic plasticity: the last 25 years. Neuron 80:704–717, doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.025, pmid:24183021.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Kanterewicz BI,
    2. Urban NN,
    3. McMahon DB,
    4. Norman ED,
    5. Giffen LJ,
    6. Favata MF,
    7. Scherle PA,
    8. Trzskos JM,
    9. Barrionuevo G,
    10. Klann E
    (2000) The extracellular signal-regulated kinase cascade is required for NMDA receptor-independent LTP in area CA1 but not area CA3 of the hippocampus. J Neurosci 20:3057–3066, pmid:10777769.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Kim HH,
    2. Lee KH,
    3. Lee D,
    4. Han YE,
    5. Lee SH,
    6. Sohn JW,
    7. Ho WK
    (2015) Costimulation of AMPA and metabotropic glutamate receptors underlies phospholipase C activation by glutamate in hippocampus. J Neurosci 35:6401–6412, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4208-14.2015, pmid:25904792.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. ↵
    1. Lee HK,
    2. Min SS,
    3. Gallagher M,
    4. Kirkwood A
    (2005) NMDA receptor-independent long-term depression correlates with successful aging in rats. Nat Neurosci 8:1657–1659, doi:10.1038/nn1586, pmid:16286930.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Lüscher C,
    2. Huber KM
    (2010) Group 1 mGluR-dependent synaptic long-term depression: mechanisms and implications for circuitry and disease. Neuron 65:445–459, doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.016, pmid:20188650.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Ménard C,
    2. Quirion R
    (2012a) Group 1 metabotropic glutamate receptor function and its regulation of learning and memory in the aging brain. Front Pharmacol 3:182, doi:10.3389/fphar.2012.00182, pmid:23091460.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Ménard C,
    2. Quirion R
    (2012b) Successful cognitive aging in rats: a role for mGluR5 glutamate receptors, homer 1 proteins and downstream signaling pathways. PLoS One 7:e28666, doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0028666, pmid:22238580.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Morgan SL,
    2. Teyler TJ
    (2001) Electrical stimuli patterned after the theta-rhythm induce multiple forms of LTP. J Neurophysiol 86:1289–1296, pmid:11535677.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    1. Morris RG
    (2013) NMDA receptors and memory encoding. Neuropharmacology 74:32–40, doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.04.014, pmid:23628345.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Nosyreva ED,
    2. Huber KM
    (2006) Metabotropic receptor-dependent long-term depression persists in the absence of protein synthesis in the mouse model of fragile X syndrome. J Neurophysiol 95:3291–3295, doi:10.1152/jn.01316.2005, pmid:16452252.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  23. ↵
    1. Oliet SH,
    2. Malenka RC,
    3. Nicoll RA
    (1997) Two distinct forms of long-term depression coexist in CA1 hippocampal pyramidal cells. Neuron 18:969–982, doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80336-0, pmid:9208864.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Park S,
    2. Park JM,
    3. Kim S,
    4. Kim JA,
    5. Shepherd JD,
    6. Smith-Hicks CL,
    7. Chowdhury S,
    8. Kaufmann W,
    9. Kuhl D,
    10. Ryazanov AG,
    11. Huganir RL,
    12. Linden DJ,
    13. Worley PF
    (2008) Elongation factor 2 and fragile X mental retardation protein control the dynamic translation of Arc/Arg3.1 essential for mGluR-LTD. Neuron 59:70–83, doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2008.05.023, pmid:18614030.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Plath N,
    2. Ohana O,
    3. Dammermann B,
    4. Errington ML,
    5. Schmitz D,
    6. Gross C,
    7. Mao X,
    8. Engelsberg A,
    9. Mahlke C,
    10. Welzl H,
    11. Kobalz U,
    12. Stawrakakis A,
    13. Fernandez E,
    14. Waltereit R,
    15. Bick-Sander A,
    16. Therstappen E,
    17. Cooke SF,
    18. Blanquet V,
    19. Wurst W,
    20. Salmen B,
    21. et al.
    (2006) Arc/Arg3.1 is essential for the consolidation of synaptic plasticity and memories. Neuron 52:437–444, doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.08.024, pmid:17088210.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Selig DK,
    2. Lee HK,
    3. Bear MF,
    4. Malenka RC
    (1995) Reexamination of the effects of MCPG on hippocampal LTP, LTD, and depotentiation. J Neurophys 74:1075–1082, pmid:7500133.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  27. ↵
    1. Stevens CF,
    2. Tonegawa S,
    3. Wang Y
    (1994) The role of calcium-calmodulin kinase II in three forms of synaptic plasticity. Curr Biol 4:687–693, doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(00)00153-6, pmid:7953554.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Volk LJ,
    2. Daly CA,
    3. Huber KM
    (2006) Differential roles for group 1 mGluR subtypes in induction and expression of chemically induced hippocampal long-term depression. J Neurophysiol 95:2427–2438, doi:10.1152/jn.00383.2005, pmid:16421200.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Waung MW,
    2. Huber KM
    (2009) Protein translation in synaptic plasticity: mGluR-LTD, Fragile X. Curr Opin Neurobiol 19:319–326, doi:10.1016/j.conb.2009.03.011, pmid:19411173.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Waung MW,
    2. Pfeiffer BE,
    3. Nosyreva ED,
    4. Ronesi JA,
    5. Huber KM
    (2008) Rapid translation of Arc/Arg3.1 selectively mediates mGluR-dependent LTD through persistent increases in AMPAR endocytosis rate. Neuron 59:84–97, doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2008.05.014, pmid:18614031.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Yang S,
    2. Megill A,
    3. Ardiles AO,
    4. Ransom S,
    5. Tran T,
    6. Koh MT,
    7. Lee HK,
    8. Gallagher M,
    9. Kirkwood A
    (2013) Integrity of mGluR-LTD in the associative/commissural inputs to CA3 correlates with successful aging in rats. J Neurosci 33:12670–12678, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1086-13.2013, pmid:23904603.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    1. Zhang W,
    2. Wu J,
    3. Ward MD,
    4. Yang S,
    5. Chuang YA,
    6. Xiao M,
    7. Li R,
    8. Leahy DJ,
    9. Worley PF
    (2015) Structural basis of arc binding to synaptic proteins: implications for cognitive disease. Neuron 86:490–500, doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.030, pmid:25864631.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 36 (5)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 36, Issue 5
3 Feb 2016
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Metabotropic Glutamate Receptors Induce a Form of LTP Controlled by Translation and Arc Signaling in the Hippocampus
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Metabotropic Glutamate Receptors Induce a Form of LTP Controlled by Translation and Arc Signaling in the Hippocampus
Hui Wang, Alvaro O. Ardiles, Sunggu Yang, Trinh Tran, Rafael Posada-Duque, Gonzalo Valdivia, Min Baek, Yang-An Chuang, Adrian G. Palacios, Michela Gallagher, Paul Worley, Alfredo Kirkwood
Journal of Neuroscience 3 February 2016, 36 (5) 1723-1729; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0878-15.2016

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Metabotropic Glutamate Receptors Induce a Form of LTP Controlled by Translation and Arc Signaling in the Hippocampus
Hui Wang, Alvaro O. Ardiles, Sunggu Yang, Trinh Tran, Rafael Posada-Duque, Gonzalo Valdivia, Min Baek, Yang-An Chuang, Adrian G. Palacios, Michela Gallagher, Paul Worley, Alfredo Kirkwood
Journal of Neuroscience 3 February 2016, 36 (5) 1723-1729; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0878-15.2016
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • Arc
  • bidirectional
  • metabotropic glutamate receptors
  • protein synthesis
  • synaptic plasticity
  • translation

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Heteromodal Cortical Areas Encode Sensory-Motor Features of Word Meaning
  • Pharmacologically Counteracting a Phenotypic Difference in Cerebellar GABAA Receptor Response to Alcohol Prevents Excessive Alcohol Consumption in a High Alcohol-Consuming Rodent Genotype
  • Neuromuscular NMDA Receptors Modulate Developmental Synapse Elimination
Show more Brief Communications
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.