Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Distinct β Band Oscillatory Networks Subserving Motor and Cognitive Control during Gait Adaptation

Johanna Wagner, Scott Makeig, Mateusz Gola, Christa Neuper and Gernot Müller-Putz
Journal of Neuroscience 17 February 2016, 36 (7) 2212-2226; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3543-15.2016
Johanna Wagner
1Laboratory of Brain-Computer Interfaces, Institute for Knowledge Discovery, Graz University of Technology, 8010 Graz, Austria,
2Swartz Center for Computational Neuroscience, Institute for Neural Computation, University of California–San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093-0559,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Scott Makeig
2Swartz Center for Computational Neuroscience, Institute for Neural Computation, University of California–San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093-0559,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Scott Makeig
Mateusz Gola
2Swartz Center for Computational Neuroscience, Institute for Neural Computation, University of California–San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093-0559,
3Institute of Psychology, Polish Academy of Science, Warsaw 00-378, Poland, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Christa Neuper
1Laboratory of Brain-Computer Interfaces, Institute for Knowledge Discovery, Graz University of Technology, 8010 Graz, Austria,
4Department of Psychology, University of Graz, 8010 Graz, Austria
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Gernot Müller-Putz
1Laboratory of Brain-Computer Interfaces, Institute for Knowledge Discovery, Graz University of Technology, 8010 Graz, Austria,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Everyday locomotion and obstacle avoidance requires effective gait adaptation in response to sensory cues. Many studies have shown that efficient motor actions are associated with μ rhythm (8–13 Hz) and β band (13–35 Hz) local field desynchronizations in sensorimotor and parietal cortex, whereas a number of cognitive task studies have reported higher behavioral accuracy to be associated with increases in β band power in prefrontal and sensory cortex. How these two distinct patterns of β band oscillations interplay during gait adaptation, however, has not been established. Here we recorded 108 channel EEG activity from 18 participants (10 males, 22–35 years old) attempting to walk on a treadmill in synchrony with a series of pacing cue tones, and quickly adapting their step rate and length to sudden shifts in pacing cue tempo. Independent component analysis parsed each participant's EEG data into maximally independent component (IC) source processes, which were then grouped across participants into distinct spatial/spectral clusters. Following cue tempo shifts, mean β band power was suppressed for IC sources in central midline and parietal regions, whereas mean β band power increased in IC sources in or near medial prefrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. In the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex IC cluster, the β band power increase was stronger during (more effortful) step shortening than during step lengthening. These results thus show that two distinct patterns of β band activity modulation accompany gait adaptations: one likely serving movement initiation and execution; and the other, motor control and inhibition.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Understanding brain dynamics supporting gait adaptation is crucial for understanding motor deficits in walking, such as those associated with aging, stroke, and Parkinson's. Only a few electromagnetic brain imaging studies have examined neural correlates of human upright walking. Here, application of independent component analysis to EEG data recorded during treadmill walking allowed us to uncover two distinct β band oscillatory cortical networks that are active during gait adaptation to shifts in the tempo of an auditory pacing cue: (8–13 Hz) μ rhythm and (13–35 Hz) β band power decreases in central and parietal cortex and (14–20 Hz) β band power increases in frontal brain areas. These results provide a fuller framework for electrophysiological studies of cortical gait control and its disorders.

  • β band oscillations
  • EEG
  • gait adaptation
  • independent component analysis
  • motor inhibition
  • rhythmic auditory cueing

Introduction

Impaired gait adaptability (i.e., a reduced ability to change walking speed or direction as required) (Den Otter et al., 2005; Hofstad et al., 2006) produces a reduced ability to avoid obstacles and an increased risk of falling in affected individuals, including many stroke and Parkinson's patients (Weerdesteyn et al., 2006). Rhythmic auditory cues have been widely used in gait rehabilitation (Thaut and Abiru, 2010). Use of an auditory pacing stimulus stream, including infrequent tempo shifts, has been recommended to identify deficits and train improvements in gait adaptation in stroke patients (Roerdink et al., 2009, 2007). Unfortunately, because of the ill effects of body movements on brain imaging data, the precise temporal brain dynamics of step adaptation remain largely unexplored by neuroimaging studies. Recent signal processing advances, however, allow study of source-resolved EEG dynamics during walking (Gwin et al., 2010, 2011; Gramann et al., 2011; Seeber et al., 2015) and other actions, an approach termed Mobile Brain/Body Imaging by Makeig et al. (2009). Adopting this approach, in several previous studies, we showed that specific gait phases are represented in power amplitude modulations in the lower (24–40 Hz) (Wagner et al., 2012, 2014; Seeber et al., 2014) and upper (70–90 Hz) (Seeber et al., 2015) γ band in central midline areas. Other studies have shown lateralized modulations at scalp electrode channels C3 and C4 (Cheron et al., 2012) and in source-resolved (Gwin et al., 2011) power relative to the gait cycle.

We have also reported μ rhythm and β-band power decreases (desynchronizations) over central sensorimotor and parietal areas during active walking relative to standing or passive walking (i.e., when participants' legs are moved by a robot) (Wagner et al., 2012; Seeber et al., 2014) and during step adaptation to interactive visual feedback in a virtual environment (Wagner et al., 2014). μ and β band power in the motor system decrease during the preparation and voluntary execution of movements (Jasper and Penfield, 1949; Pfurtscheller and Berghold, 1989; Pfurtscheller and Lopes da Silva, 1999), whereas β band power increases in scalp EEG data have been related to movement suppression (Gilbertson et al., 2005; Androulidakis et al., 2007; Zhang et al., 2008; Pogosyan et al., 2009; Joundi et al., 2012; Solis-Escalante et al., 2012).

Electrophysiological studies suggest that β band oscillations may also index top-down signaling. For example, visual top-down attention and behavioral rule switching are related to oscillatory synchronization of β-band local field potentials in monkeys' prefrontal cortex (PFC) and parietal cortex (Buschman et al., 2007, 2012). PFC affects top-down control by sending information about goals and appropriate actions to other brain areas (Miller and Cohen, 2001). Swann et al. (2009) reported that an electrocortical β band power increase in PFC during motor inhibition preceded μ and β band power decreases in motor cortex, whereas in older subjects β band activity increases may have a compensatory effect (Geerligs et al., 2012; Gola et al., 2012, 2013).

Recent studies of gait adaptation using EEG showed increased event-related potential (ERP) (Haefeli et al., 2011) and hemodynamic responses (Suzuki et al., 2004) during preparation for, and performance of, stepping over obstacles, as well as during adaptive walking and precision stepping (Koenraadt et al., 2014). A recent study demonstrated that, during gait adaptation, stroke patients prefer step lengthening over step shortening (Roerdink et al., 2009), suggesting that brain mechanisms controlling voluntary gait deceleration and acceleration differ (Varraine et al., 2000). Recent studies have reported that performing a secondary task during walking induces participants to take longer strides (Lovden et al., 2008; Li et al., 2012; De Sanctis et al., 2014), suggesting that step lengthening requires fewer attentional resources.

To examine whether difficulty in gait adaptation is related to PFC β band activity, we designed a high-density EEG study in which participants walking on a treadmill moving at a steady rate had to adapt their step length and rate to shifts in the tempo of a pacing tone (e.g., shifts requiring both longer and shorter step adaptation). We expected three different processes to accompany step rate and length adaptations to auditory tempo shifts: (1) μ and β band desynchronization in sensorimotor and parietal cortex, reflecting increased motor flexibility and adaptation; (2) increased β band power within the PFC, reflecting additional cognitive control; and (3) scaling of PCF β band power changes with task difficulty, producing a stronger increase in β band power during step shortening than during step lengthening.

Materials and Methods

Participants.

Twenty healthy volunteers with no neurological or motor deficits participated in this study. The data of two subjects were excluded because of heavy EEG artifact. The remaining data of 18 subjects (10 males and 8 females, 22–35 years of age; mean 29.1 years, SD 2.7 years) were used in the analysis. All participants were right handed. Prior studies show that footedness follows handedness in right handers, although not consistently so in left handers (Peters and Durding, 1979). The experimental procedures were approved by the human use committee of the Medical University Graz. Each subject gave informed consent before the experiment.

Experimental design and procedure.

Figures 1 and 2 show the experimental setup and a schematic of the task paradigm, which was adapted from Bank et al. (2011).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Experimental setup. Participant walking on the treadmill with auditory pacing cues delivered through in-ear headphones. During the initial training period, treadmill speed (3–3.5 km/h) was adjusted to the most comfortable pace for each participant and thereafter remained constant.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Experimental paradigm. Throughout the session, treadmill speed remained fixed at a rate comfortable to the participant. During each approximately minute-long trial, participants first walked for ∼10 s without auditory cues, then walked for 10–18 s synchronous with cue tones delivered at their then-prevailing step rate and phase. Thereafter, beginning at a right heel strike, a sudden (accelerated or decelerated) tempo shift occurred in the pacing cue sequence. In response, participants were instructed to adapt their step length, rate, and phase as quickly as possible so as to again synchronize their steps with the cue tones at the new tempo. After 30–70 steps, the next trial began immediately, returning again to 10 s of uncued walking during which participants were instructed to return to their most comfortable step rate.

Training.

Before the experimental procedure, participants practiced walking on the treadmill for 2–3 min. While walking on the treadmill, participants adapted the belt speed to their most comfortable walking speed; this ranged from 3.0 to 3.7 km/h across participants. Belt speed was then fixed at the participant's comfortable walking speed and thereafter remained constant throughout the experiment. Next, participants practiced the gait adaptation task with auditory pacing so as to become familiar with the task until they reached acceptable performance, meaning that they correctly responded to step-advance and step-delay pacing signal tempo shifts by shortening or lengthening their steps so as to synchronize their gait to the new pacing tempo.

Trial structure.

During each approximately minute-long trial period, participants first walked without pacing cues for ∼10 s. During the next 8–12 s, they heard a stream of auditory cue tones delivered via in-ear headphones at a tempo matching to their current step tempo (the mean of the step intervals between their six most recent uncued steps). The cue sequence was an alternating series of high and low tones presented so as to match the participant's right and left (or left and right) heel strikes, respectively; this high/low assignment was randomized over subjects. Participants were asked to attempt to synchronize their heel strikes to the cue tones.

Next, relative to a randomly selected right heel strike (after 8–12 s of cued steady-state walking), the tempo of the cue sequence was suddenly increased or decreased by one-sixth of a cycle (60 deg) (as in Roerdink et al., 2009), and this new cue tempo was maintained for 30–70 steps. Participants were instructed to adjust their heel strikes so as to synchronize with the new cue sequence as quickly as possible. The gait adjustments required to restore synchronization with the metronome were as follows: a one-sixth longer step interval and step length in (60° phase shift) step-delay trials, and a one-sixth shorter step interval and step length in (−60° phase shift) step-advance trials. After 30–70 steps at the new step rate, the next trial began immediately with, again, uncued walking, during which participants were instructed to return to their most comfortable step rate.

A total of 60 step-advance and 60 step-delay trials were conducted in 10 blocks of 12 trials, each comprised of 6 step-advance and 6 step-delay trials presented in random order. Between blocks, when asked for by participants, short breaks of 5 min were given during which participants were standing on the treadmill.

Data acquisition.

Seven 16-channel amplifiers (g.tec) were combined so as to record EEG data from 108 electrode channels in the 5% International 10/20 System (EasyCap) (Oostenveld and Praamstra, 2002). Electrode locations that extended below the conventional 10–20 spherical layout included PO9, POO9, OI1, OI2, POO10, PO10, I1, Iz, and I2. Reference and ground electrodes were placed on the left and right mastoids, respectively. All electrode impedances were reduced to <5 kΩ before the recording. Electromyographic (EMG) signals were recorded from the skin over the tibialis anterior muscles of both legs using standard adhesive-fixed disposable Ag/AgCl surface electrodes. These EMG channels were also recorded using left and right mastoids as reference and ground, respectively. The EEG and EMG data were sampled at 512 Hz, high pass filtered >0.1 Hz, and low pass filtered <256 Hz. Foot contacts were measured by mechanical foot switches placed over the calcaneus bone in the heel of each foot. These switches produced event markers for gait cycle heel strike and heel off events. To record the exact timing of the auditory cues, we recorded the auditory stimulation via digital inputs to one of the amplifiers.

Behavioral analysis.

Two participant timing error correction processes have been distinguished: (1) period correction to bring the motor acts (here, heel strikes) into the same tempo as the stimulus sequence (Michon, 1967); and (2) phase correction to make the motor acts coincident with pacing stimulus onsets by compensating for any phase difference (Repp, 2001a,b). To assess sensorimotor synchronization, we thus analyzed phase correction and period correction separately. Phase correction was assessed by computing asynchronies between heel strikes and pacing tone onsets (for an overview, see Repp, 2005; Repp and Su, 2013), whereas period correction was assessed by computing temporal differences between cue intervals (time intervals between consecutive cue onsets) and step intervals (or step onset asynchronies, intervals between consecutive heel strikes).

Phase correction.

For each trial, the relative phase angle difference between each heel onset and the corresponding auditory cue was calculated. Phase was defined as phase = 360° × (tcue − tHS)/Tcue, with tcue (in milliseconds) denoting the time of cue onset, tHS denoting the time of the nearest heel strike, and Tcue denoting the time interval between consecutive ipsilateral step cues (Roerdink et al., 2007). For each trial, preshift coordination between steps and cues was quantified by computing the mean and SD phase angle difference in the five steps immediately preceding the tempo shift. The time course of gait adjustments made by the participant to restore coordination following cue tempo shifts was quantified by calculating the phase difference from baseline for the 14 steps following the shift and dividing by ±60 so that step phase at S0 (the first time-perturbed stimulus) was 60° (Roerdink et al., 2009; Pelton et al., 2010). Trials were excluded from analysis if any of the steps exceeded normalized phase of 180°, which corresponds to a 180° difference from premanipulation performance. Based on these criteria, on average 5 (SD ±6) shift trials were excluded (6% of all trials).

A 2 × 14 repeated-measures ANOVA with factors “tempo shift” (long vs short) and “step number” (step numbers 1–14 following the shift) was used to assess significant differences in fidelity of adaptation between step-advance and step-delay shifts. Post hoc tests were corrected for multiple comparisons a priori to a significance level of p < 0.05 using false discovery rate (Benjamini and Yekutieli, 2001).

EEG analysis.

EEG data analysis was performed using custom scripts written in MATLAB2014a (The MathWorks) incorporating EEGLAB 14.0b functions (Delorme and Makeig, 2004). In Wagner et al. (2012, 2014), we showed that artifact contamination of the EEG during upright walking can be separated from the brain source data using Infomax Independent Component Analysis (ICA) (compare Onton et al., 2006; Gwin et al., 2010).

The EEG data were high-pass filtered at 1 Hz (zero phase FIR filter, order 7500) to minimize slow drifts, and low pass filtered <200 Hz (zero phase FIR filter, order 36). EEG channels with prominent artifacts were identified by visual inspection and removed. On average, 106 channels per participant (SD ±2.2; range 102–108) remained for analysis. The EEG data were then rereferenced to a common average reference. After visually rejecting artifacts in the continuous EEG, the data were partitioned into epochs of 0.5 s and those epochs containing values exceeding the average of the probability distribution of values across the data segments by ±5 SD were rejected. On average, 45 postshift steps per condition (80% of each participant's EEG data) remained in the analysis (SD ±11%; range 71%–89%).

Next, the preprocessed EEG data were decomposed using adaptive mixture ICA (AMICA) (Palmer et al., 2006, 2008). AMICA is a generalization of the Infomax ICA (Bell and Sejnowski, 1995; Makeig et al., 1996) and multiple-mixture (Lee et al., 1999; Lewicki and Sejnowski, 2000) ICA approaches. AMICA performed blind source separation of all concatenated preprocessed data trials for each subject individually, based on the assumed temporal near-independence of the effective EEG sources (Makeig et al., 2002, 2004a).

Using a standardized three-shell boundary element head model implemented in the DIPFIT toolbox within EEGLAB (sccn.ucsd.edu/eeglab), we calculated a best-fitting single equivalent current dipole matched to the scalp projection of each independent component (IC) source (Oostenveld and Oostendorp, 2002; Delorme et al., 2012). Standard electrode locations corresponding to the Extended 10–20 System were aligned with a standard brain model (MNI). We retained ICs for further analysis for which the equivalent dipole model was located within the brain and explained >90% of variance of the IC scalp map.

We visually inspected the remaining IC scalp maps, event-locked time courses, and mean power spectra to identify ICs related to nonbrain source artifacts (eye movement and scalp/neck muscle artifacts). Nonartifactual ICs were retained for further analysis. For these, feature vectors were constructed coding IC differences in dipole locations, scalp projection maps, and power spectral densities (3–45 Hz) (Makeig et al., 2002). Using principal component analysis, these feature vectors were reduced to 10 principal components and clustered using k-means (k = 18). ICs were identified as outliers if their locations in the clustering vector space were >5 SDs from the obtained cluster centers. Only clusters including ICs from more than half of the participants are reported here.

Cortical IC clusters.

Clustering gave three IC source clusters in frontal brain, one cluster in left temporal cortex, one in central midline cortex, and two in parietal areas. None of the obtained clusters included sources from all participants. Therefore, we inspected more closely the ICs included and excluded from each cluster. For the frontal clusters, all ICs with an equivalent dipole in these brain areas exhibited event-related spectral perturbations (ERSPs) consistent with nearby ICs and were therefore included in a frontal source cluster. These clusters “missed” ICs from 5, 3, and 1 subjects, respectively. Parietal ICs omitted from parietal clusters were located either more anterior or more occipital relative to cluster ICs. The one temporal cluster included ICs from three participants whose ERSPs contained broadband spectral perturbation features possibly related to muscle artifact. These ICs were therefore removed from the cluster. ICs in central brain regions were somewhat more heterogeneous. Eleven of the 18 subjects contributed an IC to the central midline cluster. Components from the other 7 participants did not show a similar spectral pattern (4 ICs) or (3 ICs) had scalp maps that looked noisy, such that the associated dipoles explained <90% of variance of the IC scalp map and were thus excluded or were oriented differently than other cluster ICs.

There are several possible explanations for failures of some subjects' data to contribute an IC to an IC cluster. Possibly because of subject differences in channel noise, signal variability, or data quantity, ICA decomposition and/or IC clustering might not have been able to resolve or properly group these brain sources. Possibly, the presence of more spatially variegated artifacts in some subjects reduced the number of degrees of freedom in the data available for isolating brain source processes. Finally, the possibility that ICs “missing” from a cluster index actual differences in brain processing across subjects should not be rejected out of hand (Onton et al., 2005).

ERSPs.

The data were segmented into time epochs relative to onsets of cue tempo shifts (i.e., from −4 to 10 s around the first right shift related heel strike). ERSPs (Makeig, 1993) were computed for each IC. Single-trial spectrograms were computed and time warped to the median step latency (across subjects) using linear interpolation. This procedure aligned time points of right and left heel strikes over trials for the 7 heel strikes preceding and the 15 heel strikes following each tempo shift. Relative changes in spectral power were obtained by computing the mean difference between each single-trial log spectrogram and the mean baseline spectrum (the average log spectrum between −4 s and −0.5 s preceding tempo shifts). Significant deviations from the baseline were detected using a nonparametric bootstrap approach (Delorme and Makeig, 2004). Figure 3 gives an overview of the analysis pipeline.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Schematic overview of the EEG data-processing pipeline. (1) Single-subject data are decomposed by AMICA into maximally IC time courses (traces) with spatially fixed projections (scalp maps). (2) Single equivalent current dipole locations are estimated. (3) Nonbrain artifact sources are identified and removed from further processing. (4) Brain source ICs are clustered across subjects based on their scalp maps, dipole locations, and log power spectra. (5) Single trial spectrograms and mean, base spectrum-removed ERSP for one IC. (6) Single-IC ERSPs and IC cluster-mean ERSP relative to step-advance time shifts for the right frontal cluster.

To compute significant differences between step-advance and step-delay shifts, individual frequency bands were selected per subject by determining ranges from 8–13 Hz (α), 14–20 Hz (lower β), and 21–35 Hz (upper β) and choosing the frequency band with maximally varying power modulation over time. For statistical analysis, ERSPs relative to step-advance and step-delay shifts were computed by time warping single trial spectrograms to the same (group median) step latencies and subtracting the overall mean log spectrum for both conditions computed from −4 s to −0.5 s before the tempo shift. For statistical analysis, we selected four time windows centered on the first left (L1) and second right (R2) steps following tempo shifts.

A 2 × 4 repeated-measures within-subject ANOVA with factors “tempo shift” (long vs short) and “step number” (4 time windows) was computed for each IC cluster and each frequency range (α, lower β, upper β) in the (individually subject-selected) dominant frequency range. Multiple comparisons were addressed by controlling for false discovery rate (Benjamini and Yekutieli, 2001) with an a priori significance level of p < 0.05. In cases in which the assumption of sphericity was violated, significance values were corrected using Greenhouse-Geisser.

ERPs.

To understand better the origin of ERSPs relative to the tempo shifts in the auditory cue sequence (cue tempo shift) (e.g., whether the tempo shift ERSP features were more related to the auditory perception of the cue tempo shifts or to the behavioral modification of step timing), we analyzed averaged ERPs time-locked to the presentation of the first tempo-adjusted cue tones. In particular, this analysis revealed the latency following cue onset at which participants' EEG signals first showed a reaction to the cue tempo shift, suggestive of the time point at which participants perceived the cue tempo shift. Comparing this latency to the latencies of the step-locked ERSP features of interest might indicate whether these ERSP features were related to the perception of the cue tempo shift itself or to the implementation of the responsive behavioral adaptation. Accordingly, ERPs were computed relative to (1) the first cue tone indicating a tempo shift (Rcue) and (2) the step (Rstep) nearest in time to the Rcue. We also assessed ERPs relative to cues and steps in a control condition consisting of steady-state walking to an auditory cue sequence without tempo shift (cue no shift). We computed cluster mean ERPs and their 90% confidence intervals for IC processes in frontal, temporal, and central midline clusters, respectively. Next, to assess differences between cue- and step-locked ERPs, we shifted step-locked ERPs by the median latency between Rstep and Rcue, so that latency 0 in step-locked ERP plots corresponded to the median Rcue latency.

On the cortical origin of ERSPs during walking.

Two recent studies (Castermans et al., 2014; Kline et al., 2015) investigated movement-related artifacts in the EEG during upright walking and found contamination of the EEG data at frequencies from 1 to 150 Hz. Castermans et al. (2014) found similar time/frequency properties in accelerometer signals recorded from the head during walking and in EEG for frequency bands extending up to 150 Hz. To record pure movement artifacts from EEG electrodes on the human head, Kline et al. (2015) blocked electrophysiological signals by wearing a silicone swim cap as a nonconductive layer under a standard EEG cap. They observed movement artifacts, depending on walking speed and electrode location, from 1 to 150 Hz. In a follow-up analysis (Snyder et al., 2015), the same authors applied ICA to this pure artifact data. They showed that 99% of the obtained sources lacked dipolar characteristics and were located outside the head, whereas only 1% of the sources were localized inside the brain with residual variance <15%. These component processes, however, had scalp maps, time courses, power spectra, and time-frequency changes much more typical of movement artifacts rather than brain source processes. Thus, movement artifacts do induce broadband frequency changes in signals recorded at scalp electrodes, but these nonbrain artifacts can be reliably identified and separated from brain source signals using ICA.

Scalp and neck muscle activities can also contaminate EEG signals during movement (Gramann et al., 2010; Gwin et al., 2010, 2011; Seeber et al., 2014). Scalp EMG artifacts appear across frequencies >20 Hz (Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2013; Castermans et al., 2014). By contrast, many EEG and electrocorticography studies show that electrocortical oscillations relative to motor or cognitive processing synchronize or decrease in narrow frequency bands, including μ (7–12 Hz) and β (15–30 Hz) during restricted lower limb movements (Pfurtscheller et al., 1997; Crone et al., 1998; Miller, 2007; Müller-Putz et al., 2007) and walking (Wagner et al., 2012, 2014; Severens et al., 2012; Seeber et al., 2014). Broadband high-frequency power changes (within and beyond the γ band) have been suggested to play a distinct role in cortical processing (Onton and Makeig, 2009; Miller et al., 2014). Because cortical signals, biological noise, and mechanical artifacts possess distinct properties in waveform, frequency range, and spatial distribution, proper application of signal processing methods allows separation of these sources (Makeig et al., 1996; Gwin et al., 2010; Wagner et al., 2012; Seeber et al., 2014, 2015; Snyder et al., 2015).

In two previous studies (Wagner et al., 2012, 2014), we showed that considering carefully the location, time course, and the frequency spectra of the single independent components separated by ICA decomposition allows identification of sources related to brain activity. For a more detailed report on the artifacts during walking, see Wagner et al. (2012). Our previous results (Wagner et al., 2012, 2014; Seeber et al., 2014, 2015) on β band suppression and high γ increases related to walking are in line with findings in the literature on the functional role of these rhythms in motor execution (Pfurtscheller et al., 1997; Crone et al., 1998; Miller, 2007; Müller-Putz et al., 2007). Additionally, the relatively slow walking speeds used in our studies have been found in previous studies not be heavily affected by artifacts (Gwin et al., 2010). This accumulated evidence indicates that the observed frequency-specific power modulations in the EEG presented in our current and previous studies are of cortical origin.

Results

Behavioral analysis

The step onset asynchrony (StOA) before tempo shifts was on average 641 ms (SD ±5.3 ms). Following tempo shifts, a new stable StOA was generally achieved within 3 ± 1 steps, as shown in Figure 4C. Step-delay shifts (60°) produced increases in both StOA (754 ms, calculated for the first 15 steps following shifts; SD ±14 ms) and step length, whereas step-advance shift (−60°) produced decreases in StOA (mean time between steps, 523 ms; SD ±13 ms) and step length. These changes were fairly large, on average 18% relative to baseline, slightly larger than the expected one-sixth (16.7%), although not significantly so.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

ERSP image and behavioral adaptation following tempo shifts. A, Cluster mean scalp projection map and equivalent dipole locations of cluster ICs (blue spheres) and their centroid (red sphere) visualized in the MNI template brain and centered in r-dlPFC. B, Cue latency histograms (above) and cluster mean ERSP image (below) for the r-dlPFC IC source cluster in step-advance (step shortening) trials. Single-trial spectrograms were computed between −4 and 10 s around the first time-shifted cue (0 s indicates the target right heel strike). To construct the group-mean ERSP for each subject, the single-trial EEG spectrograms were first time warped to the group-median latencies of the heel strikes during the imaged interval (red vertical lines in the cue latency histograms). Relative changes in spectral power were obtained by subtracting the mean log spectrum in the interval −4 s to −0.5 s before the shifts. Nonsignificant changes from baseline are masked in gray. Vertical lines indicate right and left heel strikes. R's and L's indicate right and left foot placements, respectively. Dashed horizontal lines indicate α (8–13 Hz), lower β (13–20 Hz), and upper β (20–35 Hz) bands. The ERSP plot shows, first, a synchronization in the β band between the second and third heel strikes following the tempo shift, and then later a desynchronization with respect to baseline. C, Behavioral record: Median step onset asynchronies (StOAs) (blue and red traces) for each subject in the two conditions (step advance and delay), and cue onset asynchronies (COAs) (gray traces) in milliseconds. D, Difference between StOA and COA at each step; this reflects adaptation of step frequency to the perturbed pacing cue tempo. E, Time intervals (in milliseconds) between heel strikes and nearest cue onsets reflect sensorimotor synchronization performance (e.g., step adaptation to the tempo-shifted cue sequence). F, Absolute step-cue phase difference (in degrees of the baseline cue cycle).

Period correction

The evolution of adjustments in step rate to the two new pacing tempi for all participants are depicted in Figure 4D, which also shows the mean difference between StOA and cue onset asynchrony (COA). On average, step period adaptation was achieved within the first two steps (between L1 and R2). During the four following steps, an overcorrection (R2) occurred, after which participants stepped in near-exact synchrony to the new pacing sequence.

Phase correction

Negative StOA − COA differences indicate that, during steady state, cue-synchronous walking step onsets slightly preceded stimulus onsets. During walking at the preferred cadence (e.g., steps −7 to −1 before the shift), we observed that heel strikes were consistently ahead of the cues (mean difference, − 59 ms; SD ±43 ms) as shown by comparing the cue latency histograms with the median step latencies in the ERSP plot in Figure 4B and as evidenced by the negative pre-shift heel strike minus cue difference in Figure 4E. This difference matches a well-known phenomenon called “phase lead” in finger tap synchronization studies. It has been suggested that this difference approximates the difference between the times of delivery of the sensory information from the tip of the finger and the auditory information from cue onset to the cortical areas in which their timing is compared. Thus, to establish synchrony at the level of central representations, finger taps should precede the auditory signals (Aschersleben and Prinz, 1995, 1997). Adjustment of steps relative to cues was achieved within 6 steps following tempo shifts. As shown in Figure 4E, the phase lead between steps and cues (measured from steps 7–15 following the shift) became larger in step-delay (step lengthening) trials (mean, −122 ms; SD ±61 ms) and became positive in step-advance (step shortening) trials (mean, 19 ms; SD ±36 ms).

Calculation of the normalized relative phase revealed differences in adaptation relative to positive and negative step rate changes, respectively. The direction of the tempo shift significantly affected synchronization accuracy (significant interaction, F(13,221) = 5.2, p = 0.00025). Post hoc tests revealed a significantly larger initial phase deviation at steps L1 and R2 in step-advance compared with step-delay trials. Also, as shown in Figure 4F, when participants reached stable postshift cue synchronization, asynchronies between steps and cues were significantly larger in step-advance than in step-delay trials. This suggests that participants had fewer difficulties in adapting to step-delay compared with step-advance shifts.

Cortical IC source clusters

Together, three IC source clusters in frontal brain, one cluster in left temporal cortex, one in central midline cortex, and two in parietal areas showed event-related changes in α and β band power following cue tempo shifts. The numbers of subjects and sources contained in each cluster as well as Tailarach coordinates of the cluster centroids are given in Table 1.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

IC clusters and cluster centroid locations

ERP features

Cue-locked ERPs in the left temporal and in the right frontal clusters (Fig. 5A) clearly show an “N100-like” negative deflection near 100 ms (right frontal cluster) or 150 ms (left temporal cluster) during all three conditions (cue advance, cue delay, and cue no shift) (Fig. 5A). An early negative (N100) peak in anterior scalp electrode channels has been related to auditory processing (Parasuraman, 1980; Makeig et al., 1997). Cluster ERPs for the frontal central and right frontal cluster include positive deflections near 250 ms in cue delay and at 300–400 ms in cue advance tempo shifts, although not in the cue no shift condition. A frontal positive deflection near 300 ms after a target stimulus onset [P300 or late positive component (LPC)] typically follows the detection of novel, rare, or unpredicted sounds (Makeig et al., 2004; Debener et al., 2005; Luck and Kappenman, 2011). In the central midline cluster, we observed a negative peak in the ERP near 250 ms following cue delay tempo shifts. A small but significant negative deflection 100 ms later occurred following cue advance shifts (in Fig. 5, the 90% confidence interval does not include zero). This peak approximates the location and timing of the feedback related negativity (FRN) following presentation of feedback indicating that the participant has committed a response error (Holroyd and Coles, 2002; Hajcak et al., 2006).

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

ERPs time-locked to cues and steps at tempo shifts in cue advance, cue delay, and control cue no tempo shift conditions for frontal, temporal, and central midline clusters. Zero marks (A) the first cue indicating a tempo shift (Rcue), (B) the step (Rstep) related to Rcue, or (A, B) simply the cue/step during steady-state walking (control condition). Dashed vertical lines mark right (Rstep) and left (Lstep) heel strikes. Continuous vertical lines mark cues (Rcue, Lcue) related to Rstep and Lstep, and the expected occurrence of cues (expcue) at tempo shifts. Event-locked time courses indicate averaged cluster ERPs relative to cue/step -advance (pink), -delay (blue), and -no tempo shift (black). Envelopes represent 90% confidence intervals around means. A, During all three conditions (cue advance, cue delay, and cue no shift), cue-locked ERPs feature a prominent negative deflection near 100 ms (right frontal cluster) or 150 ms (left temporal cluster). Frontal central and right frontal cluster ERPs include a positive peak near 250 ms after tempo shifts for cue delay and 300–400 ms for cue advance but are absent for the cue no shift condition. In the central midline cluster, following cue delay tempo shifts, a negative peak appears near 250 ms. Following cue advance tempo shifts, a small but significant negative deflection occurs 100 ms later. B, Step-locked ERPs exhibit similar but smaller ERP peaks 250–300 ms after Rstep.

Compared with cue-locked ERPs, step-locked ERPs lacked N100-like ERP peaks and exhibited smaller negative and positive peaks in the 250–300 ms interval (Fig. 5B). Cue-locked ERP peaks following cue delay tempo shifts were significantly larger than step-locked ERP features, suggesting that the observed P300- and FRN-like ERP peaks are related to the detection of the cue tempo shift. Following cue advance tempo shifts, significant differences between cue-locked and step-locked ERPs appeared only for N100-like ERP components but not for P300- and FRN-like ERP components. One possible explanation would be that, during cue advance tempo shifts, the premature cue tone as well as the step about to be taken are perceived as deviant. Thus, processing of cue advance tempo shifts is possibly related to both the steps and the cues.

The N100-like negative deflections in cue-locked ERPs in the left temporal and right frontal cluster for all conditions (cue advance, cue delay, and cue no shift) indicate that processing of the auditory cue events was ongoing at 100 ms after Rcue. The P300- and FRN-like ERP peaks following cue advance and cue delay tempo shifts, not present in the no shift cue condition, indicate that participants perceived the cue tempo shifts by 250–300 ms after Rcue onsets.

ERSPs

Figure 4A, B shows cluster location and mean cluster spectral shifts time-locked to step-advance shifts for the right frontal cortex cluster. Comparing spectral shifts (Fig. 4B) and behavioral data (Fig. 4C–F), a right frontal β band increase coincides with the main adaptation period during steps 2–4 following the shift. While, as shown in Figures 4B, 6, and 7, during the first four steps after the shift frontal and temporal clusters exhibited significant transient increases in α and/or β power (13–20 Hz in frontal clusters, 7–20 Hz in the left temporal cluster), μ and β band power in central midline and parietal clusters decreased during this time period (Figs. 8, 9). Interestingly, for the left and right parietal clusters (Fig. 9), this desynchronization lasted at least 15 steps following tempo shifts and was most pronounced just before contralateral heel strikes. The parietal cluster desynchronization was prominent and long lasting in two frequency bands: μ (7–12 Hz) and high β (18–30 Hz).

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Equivalent dipole source locations and cluster-mean scalp projections and ERSP images for IC source clusters centered in temporal and frontal cortex. From left to right in each row, Mean scalp projection; equivalent dipole locations of cluster ICs; cue onset histogram and cluster-mean ERSP images time-locked to (left) step lengthening (step delay) and (right) shortening (step advance) tempo shifts as marked by the onset of the first time-shifted cue (the nearest cue to the right heel strike at 0 s). Single-trial log spectrograms were time warped to median step latencies before averaging. Mean log power at each frequency from −4 s to −0.5 s before the tempo shift was subtracted to obtain relative changes in log spectral power. Nonsignificant changes from baseline are masked in gray. All three IC clusters show increases in mean β band power between the second and fourth heel strikes following the shift.

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

Significant ERSP differences between adaptations to step-advance and step-delay tempo shifts for an IC cluster centered in right dorsolateral frontal cortex. Top, Mean scalp projection and equivalent dipole locations of cluster ICs. Bottom (above), Cue tone onset histograms and (below left, center) cluster-mean ERSP images for step-delay and step-advance trials, respectively, time-locked (0 s) to the right heel strike nearest to the first tempo-shifted cue tone, and (right) the difference between these two adaptation responses. Significance of condition differences was estimated using a bootstrap approach corrected for multiple comparisons using false discovery rate. Nonsignificant differences are masked in gray. The difference ERSP shows a stronger β band power increase near the second and third post-shift steps in the step-advance condition than in (subjectively easier) step-delay adaptations.

Figure 8.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 8.

Equivalent dipole source locations and cluster-mean scalp projections and ERSP images for an IC source cluster centered in the supplementary motor area. Cluster-mean ERSP images time-locked to step lengthening and step shortening tempo shifts include desynchronization in the upper β band (25–35 Hz).

Figure 9.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 9.

Equivalent dipole source locations and cluster-mean scalp projections and ERSP images for IC source clusters in left and right parietal cortex. Cluster-mean ERSP images for step-delay and step-advance conditions were computed and visualized as in Figure 4. Single-trial spectrograms were time warped to median step latencies for heel strikes −7 to 15 before and after tempo shift onsets. ERSP images show long-lasting, shift-induced decreases in α and β band power maximal just before heel strikes of the foot contralateral to the cortical location.

In central medial sources (Fig. 8), however, the desynchronization was not as strong but included higher frequencies (up to 35 Hz) and began immediately following tempo shifts. Only the right frontal cluster showed a significant ERSP difference between step-delay and step-advance trials; this was in the lower β band (14–20 Hz) (F(1,18) = 9.67, p = 0.006). This significant difference is highlighted in Figure 10, showing median β band ERSP time course and its confidence intervals in step-advance and step-delay shift conditions for each frontal cluster. The difference-ERSP map for the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (r-dlPFC) cluster (Fig. 7) also showed a significantly larger increase in β band power in step-advance compared with step-delay trials during the first left and second right foot steps following tempo shifts.

Figure 10.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 10.

β-band spectral perturbations in frontal clusters. Individual β frequency bands were selected for each individual subject as the frequency within the range 14–20 Hz having maximal ERSP power variance. For statistical analysis, mean β-band ERSP time courses time-locked to step-advance and step-delay tempo shifts were computed. Heel strike events were time warped to the same (median) latencies in step-delay and step-advance shift trials. Blue and pink lines and areas represent mean β band power as a function of trial latency and its 95% confidence interval. Only the right frontal cluster shows a significant difference between step advances and step delays (note the nonoverlapping confidence intervals).

Discussion

Use of a novel sensorimotor gait synchronization task, coupled with advanced EEG source signal processing methods, revealed two different β band oscillatory networks involved in orchestrating motor adjustments during gait adaptation. Our results combined μ and β band power desynchronization (blocking) in motor and parietal cortex (PPC) with a concurrent β band power increase in PFC. This indicates two distinct patterns of β band activity during gait control: (1) a motor cortical μ and β band decrease expressing motor execution and motor readiness related to gait movements (as in Pfurtscheller and Lopes da Silva, 1999; Neuper et al., 2006; Wagner et al., 2012, 2014; Seeber et al., 2014); and (2) a frontal β band increase related to cognitive top-down control (Swann et al., 2009; as in Buschman et al., 2012).

Observed μ (7–13 Hz) and β band desynchronization (13–30 Hz) (Figs. 8, 9) in the motor system after a shift in tempo of the pacing cue sequence may reflect an increased disposition for motor adjustments, as suggested by Engel and Fries (2010), possibly guided by the PFC as proposed by Miller and Cohen (2001; Siegel et al., 2012). The fact that we observed temporary increase in a frontal β band oscillatory network (13–20 Hz; Figs. 6, 7, 10) during step tempo adaptation may reflect action monitoring and top-down signaling from PFC to sensorimotor cortex. Our results also show that β band power in r-dlPFC was modulated by task difficulty. This lateralized frontal β band power increase during step shortening may represent neurocognitive response inhibition processes that actively involve or are accompanied by locally synchronous cortical β band oscillations.

Frontal β band oscillations in motor control

Near the second and third heel strikes following shift onsets (Figs. 6, 7, 10), we observed an increase in β band power in EEG sources localized to left, central, and right frontal cortex (Table 1). As proposed by Miller and Cohen (2001), these regions may play an important role in the top-down signaling of current behavioral goals to guide adjustment of motor plans. In our task, the participant's goal was to adapt their gait to cue-induced shifts in step tempo involving either a (more difficult) step cue phase advance or an (easier) phase delay. Comparison between decelerations and accelerations revealed significantly higher β power in the r-dlPFC during step-advance trials (Figs. 7, 10). This result is in line with our hypothesis and results of the study by Swann et al. (2009) showing a relationship between β band (∼16 Hz) power increase in the right inferior frontal gyrus and successful response inhibition in a stop signal task. Because in our task step shortening (in step-advance trials) required inhibiting execution of the accustomed full stride action, it is probable that this process requires more explicit motor inhibition than step lengthening.

Indeed, our behavioral results show that participants performed significantly more accurate step delay (step lengthening) than step advance (step shortening) performance (i.e., they stepped closer in time to the step-delay cues). This is in line with finger tapping studies showing that motor adaptation is somewhat faster to decelerations than to accelerations (Michon, 1967). Our results are the first neurophysiological evidence for previous hypotheses that behavioral preference for longer step adaptation responses in stroke patients (Roerdink et al., 2009) and dual task paradigms (Lovden et al., 2008; Li et al., 2012; De Sanctis et al., 2014) is associated with the higher demands of cognitive control and inhibition involved in step shortening.

Studies have shown that, whereas r-dlPFC is associated with response inhibition (Swann et al., 2009; Aron et al., 2014), medial and left PFC may relate to error detection (Rubia et al., 2003; Ridderinkhof et al., 2004) and to motor adjustment (Miller and Cohen, 2001; Kübler et al., 2006; Cavanagh et al., 2009, 2010; Wittfoth et al., 2009), respectively. This lateral dissociation is in line with our results as only the r-dlPFC source cluster showed a difference between step-advance and step-delay trials, whereas medial and left PFC showed β band power increased during both gait accelerations and decelerations.

It has been proposed that frontal cortex interacts with basal ganglia structures during motor suppression and that this interaction occurs via β band oscillations (Kühn et al., 2004; Lalo et al., 2008). β band synchrony within the basal ganglia-cortical loop promotes tonic activity that is detrimental to voluntary movement, thus providing further evidence for the role of β band oscillations in motor inhibition (Jenkinson and Brown, 2011).

The results of our ERP analysis indicate that participants perceived the cue tempo shift after 100 ms (Fig. 5) and responded to it as early as 250 ms following the Rcue. In contrast, the β band changes we observed began as late as 500 ms following Rcue and were maintained up to 2000 ms, too late to be related to the pure perception of the cue tempo shift and corresponding to the period when most of the behavioral step adaptation was taking place. Fujioka et al. (2009), for example, showed a β band power rebound much earlier (100 ms and lasting up to 450 ms) following the omission of a sound from a steady rhythmic sequence. Thus, the frontal β band synchronization following Rcue events we observed was not likely related to the simple detection or processing of the cue tempo shift, but likely reflects top-down application of the perceived cue tempo shift to behavioral step adaptation.

Feedback motor control in parietal and central-midline cortices

The parietal source cluster ERSPs showed a prominent and long-lasting decrease in two frequency bands, μ (7–12 Hz) and β (18–30 Hz) in (Fig. 9), whereas β band power decrease in medial central regions was brief and also involved higher frequencies (up to 35 Hz) (Fig. 8). This may suggest that these clusters index different perceptual processes and/or different aspects of gait adaptation. μ and β band power desynchronization in left and right PPC was strongest close in time to contralateral heel strikes during the first up to 17 steps after cue tempo shifts. This may index brain activity involved in the process of matching of heel strikes to auditory cue onsets. Because auditory, visual, and tactile information converges in parietal regions, PPC has been proposed as sensorimotor interface responsible for multisensory integration with ongoing movements (Buneo et al., 2002; Buneo and Andersen, 2006). Thus, PPC may play a key role for anticipatory motor control by sensorimotor feedback matching (Thaut et al., 2008; Krause et al., 2012, 2014).

Krause et al. (2014) have shown that inhibition of the PPC interrupts a matching process of anticipated and real sensorimotor feedback during synchronization but not during continuation tapping. In a previous study, we showed a decrease in μ and β band power in PPC during visually guided gait adaptation in a virtual environment, suggesting that the PPC is involved in matching steps to visual input (Wagner et al., 2014). Taking into account our current results, the known role of the PPC in mapping visual representations with motor output in space (Buneo and Andersen, 2006) may extend to matching times of occurrence of (expected) auditory and motor events.

Participants performed most adaptation within the first three steps following cue tempo shifts, including a rapid adaptation of step period and beginning a gradual reduction in the phase difference between steps and cues. Finger tapping studies have also shown that a small sudden tempo change in the pacing sequence tends to be followed by rapid adaptation of the tapping period accompanied by slow adaptation of tapping phase (Thaut et al., 1998). Thus, the transient frontal β band power increase we observe may index step period adaptation, whereas the longer-lasting parietal μ and β band decreases may relate to step phase correction.

β band oscillations in temporal prediction

Interestingly, β band activity in both sensory and motor systems seems to reflect anticipatory processes. In the motor system, an upcoming action is reflected in decreased motor cortical β band activity that may begin several seconds before execution (Donner et al., 2009), and β band corticospinal coherence increases with decreasing likelihood of an upcoming action (Schoffelen et al., 2005). In auditory cortex, violation of top-down expectation (e.g., the omission of a sound from a regular beat sequence) induces a β band power increase (Fujioka et al., 2009) that may represent an expectation mismatch response.

Similar to these findings, our results also revealed an α and lower β band power (7–20 Hz) increase in a temporal cortex source cluster following tempo shifts (Fig. 6). Synchronization-continuation tapping studies indicate that β band power is linked to the development of subjective time and to the guidance of internally driven motor sequences (Bartolo et al., 2014; Bartolo and Merchant, 2015). Precise sensorimotor timing and anticipation of subsequent events are essential to quick adjustment of movements with respect to external changes. Thus, the β band activity we observed could point to wider roles of β band oscillations in the updating of internal representations for upcoming actions and events (Engel and Fries, 2010; Arnal and Giraud, 2012) to prepare corrective motor adjustments.

Summary and future directions

In conclusion, our results show two distinct patterns of β band oscillatory activity changes following step pacing cue tempo perturbations and suggest that these accompany or subserve motor and cognitive control processes, respectively. To our knowledge, this is the first study to show neural correlates of these two distinct mechanisms acting concurrently in the service of gait adaptation. The size of the β band power increase in or near r-dlPFC was modulated according to adaptation difficulty, possibly reflecting engagement of additional movement control resources and providing the first evidence that gait adaptation strategies involving step shortening require more cortical inhibition/control compared with step lengthening. Taking into account our results, deficits in gait adaptation are most probably due to an impairment of prefrontal control guiding motor inhibition. This idea has been previously tested in two TMS studies (del Olmo et al., 2007; Lee et al., 2014). Authors stimulated r-dlPFC using repetitive TMS at 10 Hz (no other frequencies were tested). Both studies showed improvement in the temporal regularity and pace of gait and tapping, but not of Parkinson symptoms. Our results (along with those of Swann et al., 2009) suggest that stimulation at a higher frequency (16–17 Hz) might prove more effective. Such interventions might be used for voluntary motor suppression training with real-time feedback of motor cortical β oscillations and motor-evoked potentials (as proposed by Majid et al., 2015).

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by Future Labs Reloaded 2013 of the Faculty of Computer Science at the Graz University of Technology, Austria, OeAD (Austrian Agency for International Mobility) Marietta Blau Grant to J.W., the BMWF (Austrian Federal Ministry of Science and Research), Polish Ministry of Science Mobility Plus Grant 1057/MOB/2013/0 to M.G., and a gift to University of California–San Diego by the Swartz Foundation (Old Field, NY). We thank Dr. Teodoro Solis-Escalante for programming the auditory paradigm; and Daniel Hackhofer and Laura Schulz for assistance during the experiments.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Johanna Wagner, Laboratory of Brain-Computer Interfaces, Institute for Knowledge Discovery, Graz University of Technology, Stremayrgasse 16, 8010 Graz, Austria. joa.wagn{at}gmail.com

References

  1. ↵
    1. Androulidakis AG,
    2. Doyle LM,
    3. Yarrow K,
    4. Litvak V,
    5. Gilbertson TP,
    6. Brown P
    (2007) Anticipatory changes in beta synchrony in the human corticospinal system and associated improvements in task performance. Eur J Neurosci 25:3758–3765, doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05620.x, pmid:17610595.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Arnal LH,
    2. Giraud AL
    (2012) Cortical oscillations and sensory predictions. Trends Cogn Sci 16:390–398, doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.05.003, pmid:22682813.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Aron AR,
    2. Robbins TW,
    3. Poldrack RA
    (2014) Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends Cogn Sci 18:177–185, doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.003, pmid:24440116.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Aschersleben G,
    2. Prinz W
    (1995) Synchronizing actions with events: the role of sensory information. Percept Psychophys 57:305–317, doi:10.3758/BF03213056, pmid:7770322.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Aschersleben G,
    2. Prinz W
    (1997) Delayed auditory feedback in synchronization. J Mot Behav 29:35–46, doi:10.1080/00222899709603468, pmid:20037008.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Bank PJ,
    2. Roerdink M,
    3. Peper CE
    (2011) Comparing the efficacy of metronome beeps and stepping stones to adjust gait: steps to follow! Exp Brain Res 209:159–169, doi:10.1007/s00221-010-2531-9, pmid:21221956.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Bartolo R,
    2. Prado L,
    3. Merchant H
    (2014) Information processing in the primate basal ganglia during sensory-guided and internally driven rhythmic tapping. J Neurosci 34:3910–3923, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2679-13.2014, pmid:24623769.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    1. Bartolo R,
    2. Merchant H
    (2015) β oscillations are linked to the initiation of sensory-cued movement sequences and the internal guidance of regular tapping in the monkey. J Neurosci 35:4635–4640, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4570-14.2015, pmid:25788680.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    1. Bell AJ,
    2. Sejnowski TJ
    (1995) An information-maximization approach to blind separation and blind deconvolution. Neural Comput 7:1129–1159, doi:10.1162/neco.1995.7.6.1129, pmid:7584893.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Benjamini Y,
    2. Yekutieli D
    (2001) The control of the false discovery rate in multiple testing under dependency. Ann Stat 29:1165–1188, doi:10.1214/aos/1013699998.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  11. ↵
    1. Buneo CA,
    2. Jarvis MR,
    3. Batista AP,
    4. Andersen RA
    (2002) Direct visuomotor transformations for reaching. Nature 416:632–636, doi:10.1038/416632a, pmid:11948351.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Buneo CA,
    2. Andersen RA
    (2006) The posterior parietal cortex: sensorimotor interface for the planning and online control of visually guided movements. Neuropsychologia 44:2594–2606, doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.10.011, pmid:16300804.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Buschman TJ,
    2. Miller EK
    (2007) Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science 315:1860–1862, doi:10.1126/science.1138071, pmid:17395832.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    1. Buschman TJ,
    2. Denovellis EL,
    3. Diogo C,
    4. Bullock D,
    5. Miller EK
    (2012) Synchronous oscillatory neural ensembles for rules in the prefrontal cortex. Neuron 76:838–846, doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.029, pmid:23177967.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Castermans T,
    2. Duvinage M,
    3. Cheron G,
    4. Dutoit T
    (2014) About the cortical origin of the low-delta and high-gamma rhythms observed in EEG signals during treadmill walking. Neurosci Lett 561:166–170, doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2013.12.059, pmid:24412128.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Cavanagh JF,
    2. Cohen MX,
    3. Allen JJ
    (2009) Prelude to and resolution of an error: EEG phase synchrony reveals cognitive control dynamics during action monitoring. J Neurosci 29:98–105, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4137-08.2009, pmid:19129388.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. ↵
    1. Cavanagh JF,
    2. Frank MJ,
    3. Klein TJ,
    4. Allen JJ
    (2010) Frontal theta links prediction errors to behavioral adaptation in reinforcement learning. Neuroimage 49:3198–3209, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.080, pmid:19969093.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Cheron G,
    2. Duvinage M,
    3. De Saedeleer C,
    4. Castermans T,
    5. Bengoetxea A,
    6. Petieau M,
    7. Seetharaman K,
    8. Hoellinger T,
    9. Dan B,
    10. Dutoit T,
    11. Sylos Labini F,
    12. Lacquaniti F,
    13. Ivanenko Y
    (2012) From spinal central pattern generators to cortical network: integrated BCI for walking rehabilitation. Neural Plast 2012:375148, doi:10.1155/2012/375148, pmid:22272380.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Crone NE,
    2. Miglioretti DL,
    3. Gordon B,
    4. Sieracki JM,
    5. Wilson MT,
    6. Uematsu S,
    7. Lesser RP
    (1998) Functional mapping of human sensorimotor cortex with electro-corticographic spectral analysis. Brain 121:2271–2299, doi:10.1093/brain/121.12.2271, pmid:9874480.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. ↵
    1. Debener S,
    2. Makeig S,
    3. Delorme A,
    4. Engel AK
    (2005) What is novel in the novelty oddball paradigm? Functional significance of the novelty P3 event-related potential as revealed by independent component analysis. Cogn Brain Res 22:309–321, doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.09.006, pmid:15722203.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. del Olmo MF,
    2. Bello O,
    3. Cudeiro J
    (2007) Transcranial magnetic stimulation over dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in Parkinson's disease. Clin Neurophysiol 118:131–139, doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2006.09.002, pmid:17097342.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Delorme A,
    2. Makeig S
    (2004) EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods 134:9–21, doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009, pmid:15102499.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Delorme A,
    2. Palmer J,
    3. Onton J,
    4. Oostenveld R,
    5. Makeig S
    (2012) Independent EEG sources are dipolar. PLoS One 7:e30135, doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0030135, pmid:22355308.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Den Otter AR,
    2. Geurts AC,
    3. de Haart M,
    4. Mulder T,
    5. Duysens J
    (2005) Step characteristics during obstacle avoidance in hemiplegic stroke. Exp Brain Res 161:180–192, doi:10.1007/s00221-004-2057-0, pmid:15517222.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. De Sanctis P,
    2. Butler JS,
    3. Malcolm BR,
    4. Foxe JJ
    (2014) Recalibration of inhibitory control systems during walking-related dual-task interference: a mobile brain-body imaging (MOBI) study. Neuroimage 94:55–64, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.016, pmid:24642283.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Donner TH,
    2. Siegel M,
    3. Fries P,
    4. Engel AK
    (2009) Build-up of choice predictive activity in human motor cortex during perceptual decision making. Curr Biol 19:1581–1585, doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.066, pmid:19747828.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Engel AK,
    2. Fries P
    (2010) Beta-band oscillations: signaling the status quo? Curr Opin Neurobiol 20:156–165, doi:10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.015, pmid:20359884.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Fujioka T,
    2. Trainor LJ,
    3. Large EW,
    4. Ross B
    (2009) Beta and gamma rhythms in human auditory cortex during musical beat processing. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1169:89–92, doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04779.x, pmid:19673759.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Geerligs L,
    2. Saliasi E,
    3. Maurits NM,
    4. Lorist MM
    (2012) Compensation through increased functional connectivity: neural correlates of inhibition in old and young. J Cogn Neurosci 24:2057–2069, doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00270, pmid:22816367.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Gilbertson T,
    2. Lalo E,
    3. Doyle L,
    4. Di Lazzaro V,
    5. Cioni B,
    6. Brown P
    (2005) Existing motor state is favored at the expense of new movement during 13–35 Hz oscillatory synchrony in the human corticospinal system. J Neurosci 25:7771–7779, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1762-05.2005, pmid:16120778.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  31. ↵
    1. Gola M,
    2. Kamiñski J,
    3. Brzezicka A,
    4. Wróbel A
    (2012) Beta band oscillations as a correlate of alertness: changes in aging. Int J Psychophysiol 85:62–67, doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.09.001, pmid:21925215.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Gola M,
    2. Magnuski M,
    3. Szumska I,
    4. Wróbel A
    (2013) EEG beta band activity is related to attention and attentional deficits in the visual performance of elderly subjects. Int J Psychophysiol 89:334–341, doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.05.007, pmid:23688673.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Gramann K,
    2. Gwin JT,
    3. Bigdely-Shamlo N,
    4. Ferris DP,
    5. Makeig S
    (2010) Visual evoked responses during standing and walking. Front Hum Neurosci 4:202, doi:10.3389/fnhum.2010.00202, pmid:21267424.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Gwin JT,
    2. Gramann K,
    3. Makeig S,
    4. Ferris DP
    (2010) Removal of movement artifact from high-density EEG recorded during walking and running. J Neurophysiol 103:3526–3534, doi:10.1152/jn.00105.2010, pmid:20410364.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. ↵
    1. Gwin JT,
    2. Gramann K,
    3. Makeig S,
    4. Ferris DP
    (2011) Electrocortical activity is coupled to gait cycle phase during treadmill walking. Neuroimage 54:1289–1296, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.066, pmid:20832484.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Haefeli J,
    2. Vögeli S,
    3. Michel J,
    4. Dietz V
    (2011) Preparation and performance of obstacle steps: interaction between brain and spinal neuronal activity. Eur J Neurosci 33:338–348, doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07494.x, pmid:21070395.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Hajcak G,
    2. Moser JS,
    3. Holroyd CB,
    4. Simons RF
    (2006) The feedback-related negativity reflects the binary evaluation of good versus bad outcomes. Biol Psychol 71:148–154, doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.04.001, pmid:16005561.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Hofstad CJ,
    2. van der Linde H,
    3. Nienhuis B,
    4. Weerdesteyn V,
    5. Duysens J,
    6. Geurts AC
    (2006) High failure rates when avoiding obstacles during treadmill walking in patients with a transtibial amputation. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 87:1115–1122, doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2006.04.009, pmid:16876558.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Holroyd CB,
    2. Coles MG
    (2002) The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychol Rev 109:679–709, doi:10.1037/0033-295X.109.4.679, pmid:12374324.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Jasper H,
    2. Penfield W
    (1949) Electrocorticograms in man: effect of voluntary movement upon the electrical activity of the precentral gyrus. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkrankheiten 183:163–174, doi:10.1007/BF01062488.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  41. ↵
    1. Jenkinson N,
    2. Brown P
    (2011) New insights into the relationship between dopamine, beta oscillations and motor function. Trends Neurosci 34:611–618, doi:10.1016/j.tins.2011.09.003, pmid:22018805.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Joundi RA,
    2. Jenkinson N,
    3. Brittain JS,
    4. Aziz TZ,
    5. Brown P
    (2012) Driving oscillatory activity in the human cortex enhances motor performance. Curr Biol 22:403–407, doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.024, pmid:22305755.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Kline JE,
    2. Huang HJ,
    3. Snyder KL,
    4. Ferris DP
    (2015) Isolating gait-related movement artifacts in electroencephalography during human walking. J Neural Eng 12:46022, doi:10.1088/1741-2560/12/4/046022, pmid:26083595.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Koenraadt KL,
    2. Roelofsen EG,
    3. Duysens J,
    4. Keijsers NL
    (2014) Cortical control of normal gait and precision stepping: an fNIRS study. Neuroimage 85:415–422, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.070, pmid:23631980.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Krause V,
    2. Bashir S,
    3. Pollok B,
    4. Caipa A,
    5. Schnitzler A,
    6. Pascual-Leone A
    (2012) 1 Hz rTMS of the left posterior parietal cortex (PPC) modifies sensorimotor timing. Neuropsychologia 50:3729–3735, doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.10.020, pmid:23103789.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Krause V,
    2. Weber J,
    3. Pollok B
    (2014) The posterior parietal cortex (PPC) mediates anticipatory motor control. Brain Stimul 7:800–806, doi:10.1016/j.brs.2014.08.003, pmid:25216648.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Kübler A,
    2. Dixon V,
    3. Garavan H
    (2006) Automaticity and reestablishment of executive control: an fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci 18:1331–1342, doi:10.1162/jocn.2006.18.8.1331, pmid:16859418.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Kühn AA,
    2. Williams D,
    3. Kupsch A,
    4. Limousin P,
    5. Hariz M,
    6. Schneider GH,
    7. Yarrow K,
    8. Brown P
    (2004) Event-related beta desynchronization in human subthalamic nucleus correlates with motor performance. Brain 127:735–746, doi:10.1093/brain/awh106, pmid:14960502.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  49. ↵
    1. Lalo E,
    2. Thobois S,
    3. Sharott A,
    4. Polo G,
    5. Mertens P,
    6. Pogosyan A,
    7. Brown P
    (2008) Patterns of bidirectional communication between cortex and basal ganglia during movement in patients with Parkinson disease. J Neurosci 28:3008–3016, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5295-07.2008, pmid:18354004.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  50. ↵
    1. Lee TW,
    2. Lewicki MS,
    3. Girolami M,
    4. Sejnowski TJ
    (1999) Blindsource separation of more sources than mixtures using overcomplete representations. IEEE Signal Processing Lett 6:87–90, doi:10.1109/97.752062.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  51. ↵
    1. Lee SY,
    2. Kim MS,
    3. Chang WH,
    4. Cho JW,
    5. Youn JY,
    6. Kim YH
    (2014) Effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on freezing of gait in patients with Parkinsonism. Restor Neurol Neurosci 32:743–753, doi:10.3233/RNN-140397, pmid:25079979.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Lewicki MS,
    2. Sejnowski TJ
    (2000) Learning overcomplete representations. Neural Comput 12:337–365, doi:10.1162/089976600300015826, pmid:10636946.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Li KZ,
    2. Abbud GA,
    3. Fraser SA,
    4. DeMont RG
    (2012) Successful adaptation of gait in healthy older adults during dual-task treadmill walking. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 19:150–167, doi:10.1080/13825585.2011.628375, pmid:22111520.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Lövdén M,
    2. Schaefer S,
    3. Pohlmeyer AE,
    4. Lindenberger U
    (2008) J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci, Walking variability and working-memory load in aging: a dual-process account relating cognitive control to motor control performance, 63, pp P121–P128, doi:10.1093/geronb/63.3.P121, pmid:18559676.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  55. ↵
    1. Luck SJ,
    2. Kappenman ES
    (2011) The Oxford handbook of event-related potential components (Oxford UP, Oxford).
  56. ↵
    1. Majid DS,
    2. Lewis C,
    3. Aron AR
    (2015) Training voluntary motor suppression with real-time feedback of motor evoked potentials. J Neurophysiol 113:3446–3452, doi:10.1152/jn.00992.2014, pmid:25744889.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  57. ↵
    1. Makeig S
    (1993) Auditory event-related dynamics of the EEG spectrum and effects of exposure to tones. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 86:283–293, doi:10.1016/0013-4694(93)90110-H, pmid:7682932.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Makeig S,
    2. Bell AJ,
    3. Jung TP,
    4. Sejnowski TJ
    (1996) in Advances in neural information processing systems, Independent component analysis of electroencephalographic data, eds Touretzky D, Mozer M, Hasselmo M (Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA), pp 145–151.
  59. ↵
    1. Makeig S,
    2. Jung TP,
    3. Bell AJ,
    4. Ghahremani D,
    5. Sejnowski TJ
    (1997) Blind separation of auditory event-related brain responses into independent components. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 94:10979–10984, doi:10.1073/pnas.94.20.10979, pmid:9380745.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  60. ↵
    1. Makeig S,
    2. Westerfield M,
    3. Jung TP,
    4. Enghoff S,
    5. Townsend J,
    6. Courchesne E,
    7. Sejnowski TJ
    (2002) Dynamic brain sources of visual evoked responses. Science 295:690–694, doi:10.1126/science.1066168, pmid:11809976.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  61. ↵
    1. Makeig S,
    2. Debener S,
    3. Onton J,
    4. Delorme A
    (2004) Mining event-related brain dynamics. Trends Cogn Sci 8:204–210, doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.03.008, pmid:15120678.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    1. Makeig S,
    2. Gramann K,
    3. Jung TP,
    4. Sejnowski TJ,
    5. Poizner H
    (2009) Linking brain, mind and behavior. Int J Psychophysiol 73:95–100, doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.11.008, pmid:19414039.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    1. Michon JA
    (1967) Timing in temporal tracking (Institute for Perception RVO-TNO, Soesterberg, The Netherlands).
  64. ↵
    1. Miller KJ,
    2. Honey CJ,
    3. Hermes D,
    4. Rao RP,
    5. denNijs M,
    6. Ojemann JG
    (2014) Broadband changes in the cortical surface potential track activation of functionally diverse neuronal populations. Neuroimage 85:711–720, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.070, pmid:24018305.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Miller R
    (2007) Theory of the normal waking EEG: from single neurons to waveforms in the alpha, beta and gamma frequency ranges. Int J Psychophysiol 64:18–23, doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.07.009, pmid:16997407.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Miller EK,
    2. Cohen JD
    (2001) An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci 24:167–202, doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167, pmid:11283309.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Müller-Putz GR,
    2. Zimmermann D,
    3. Graimann B,
    4. Nestinger K,
    5. Korisek G,
    6. Pfurtscheller G
    (2007) Event-related beta EEG-changes during passive and attempted foot movements in paraplegic patients. Brain Res 1137:84–91, doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2006.12.052, pmid:17229403.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. Muthukumaraswamy SD
    (2013) High-frequency brain activity and muscle artifacts in MEG/EEG: a review and recommendations. Front Hum Neurosci 7:138, doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00138, pmid:23596409.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    1. Neuper C,
    2. Wörtz M,
    3. Pfurtscheller G
    (2006) ERD/ERS patterns reflecting sensorimotor activation and deactivation. Prog Brain Res 159:211–222, doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(06)59014-4, pmid:17071233.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    1. Onton J,
    2. Delorme A,
    3. Makeig S
    (2005) Early visual event-related potential activity in extra-striate cortex (Society for Psychophysiological Research, Lisbon) Sept. 22–25, 2005.
  71. ↵
    1. Onton J,
    2. Westerfield M,
    3. Townsend J,
    4. Makeig S
    (2006) Imaging human EEG dynamics using independent component analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 30:808–822, doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.007, pmid:16904745.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  72. ↵
    1. Onton J,
    2. Makeig S
    (2009) High-frequency broadband modulations of electroencephalographic spectra. Front Hum Neurosci 3:61, doi:10.3389/neuro.09.061.2009, pmid:20076775.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    1. Oostenveld R,
    2. Oostendorp TF
    (2002) Validating the boundary element method for forward and inverse EEG computations in the presence of a hole in the skull. Hum Brain Mapp 17:179–192, doi:10.1002/hbm.10061, pmid:12391571.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  74. ↵
    1. Palmer JA,
    2. Kreutz-Delgado K,
    3. Makeig S
    (2006) in Lecture notes in computer science, Super-Gaussian mixture source model for ICA, eds Rosca J, Erdogmus D, Príncipe JC, Haykin S (Springer, Berlin), pp 854–861.
  75. ↵
    1. Palmer JA,
    2. Makeig S,
    3. Kreutz-Delgado K,
    4. Rao BD
    (2008) IEEE International Conference on Acoustics and Signal Processing (Las Vegas), Newton method for the ICA mixture model, pp 1805–1808.
  76. ↵
    1. Parasuraman R
    (1980) Effects of information processing demands on slow negative shift latencies and N100 amplitude in selective and divided attention. Biol Psychol 11:217–233, doi:10.1016/0301-0511(80)90057-5, pmid:7272394.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    1. Pelton TA,
    2. Johannsen L,
    3. Huiya C,
    4. Wing AM
    (2010) Hemiparetic stepping to the beat: asymmetric response to metronome phase shift during treadmill gait. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 24:428–434, doi:10.1177/1545968309353608, pmid:19952366.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  78. ↵
    1. Peters M,
    2. Durding BM
    (1979) Footedness of left-and right-handers. Am J Psychol, 133–142.
  79. ↵
    1. Pfurtscheller G,
    2. Berghold A
    (1989) Patterns of cortical activation during planning of voluntary movement. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 72:250–258, doi:10.1016/0013-4694(89)90250-2, pmid:2465128.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    1. Pfurtscheller G,
    2. Lopes da Silva FL
    (1999) Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles. Clin Neurophysiol 110:1842–1857, doi:10.1016/S1388-2457(99)00141-8, pmid:10576479.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  81. ↵
    1. Pfurtscheller G,
    2. Neuper C,
    3. Andrew C,
    4. Edlinger G
    (1997) Foot and hand area mu rhythms. Int J Psychophysiol 26:121–135, doi:10.1016/S0167-8760(97)00760-5, pmid:9202999.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  82. ↵
    1. Pogosyan A,
    2. Gaynor LD,
    3. Eusebio A,
    4. Brown P
    (2009) Boosting cortical activity at beta-band frequencies slows movement in humans. Curr Biol 19:1637–1641, doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.074, pmid:19800236.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  83. ↵
    1. Repp BH
    (2001a) Phase correction, phase resetting, and phase shifts after subliminal timing perturbations in sensorimotor synchronization. J Exp Psychol 27:600, doi:10.1037/0096-1523.27.3.600, pmid:11424648.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  84. ↵
    1. Repp BH
    (2001b) Processes underlying adaptation to tempo changes in sensorimotor synchronization. Hum Mov Sci 20:277–312, doi:10.1016/S0167-9457(01)00049-5, pmid:11517673.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  85. ↵
    1. Repp BH
    (2005) Sensorimotor synchronization: a review of the tapping literature. Psychonom Bull Rev 12:969–992, doi:10.3758/BF03206433, pmid:16615317.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  86. ↵
    1. Repp BH,
    2. Su YH
    (2013) Sensorimotor synchronization: a review of recent research (2006–2012). Psychonom Bull Rev 20:403–452, doi:10.3758/s13423-012-0371-2, pmid:23397235.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  87. ↵
    1. Ridderinkhof KR,
    2. Ullsperger M,
    3. Crone EA,
    4. Nieuwenhuis S
    (2004) The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science 306:443–447, doi:10.1126/science.1100301, pmid:15486290.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  88. ↵
    1. Roerdink M,
    2. Lamoth CJ,
    3. Kwakkel G,
    4. van Wieringen PC,
    5. Beek PJ
    (2007) Gait coordination after stroke: benefits of acoustically paced treadmill walking. Phys Ther 87:1009–1022, doi:10.2522/ptj.20050394, pmid:17553922.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  89. ↵
    1. Roerdink M,
    2. Lamoth CJ,
    3. van Kordelaar J,
    4. Elich P,
    5. Konijnenbelt M,
    6. Kwakkel G,
    7. Beek PJ
    (2009) Rhythm perturbations in acoustically paced treadmill walking after stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 23:668–678, doi:10.1177/1545968309332879, pmid:19307435.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  90. ↵
    1. Rubia K,
    2. Smith AB,
    3. Brammer MJ,
    4. Taylor E
    (2003) Right inferior prefrontal cortex mediates response inhibition while mesial prefrontal cortex is responsible for error detection. Neuroimage 20:351–358, doi:10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00275-1, pmid:14527595.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  91. ↵
    1. Schoffelen JM,
    2. Oostenveld R,
    3. Fries P
    (2005) Neuronal coherence as a mechanism of effective corticospinal interaction. Science 308:111–113, doi:10.1126/science.1107027, pmid:15802603.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  92. ↵
    1. Seeber M,
    2. Scherer R,
    3. Wagner J,
    4. Solis-Escalante T,
    5. Müller-Putz GR
    (2014) EEG beta suppression and low gamma modulation are different elements of human upright walking. Front Hum Neurosci 8:485, doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00485, pmid:25071515.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  93. ↵
    1. Seeber M,
    2. Scherer R,
    3. Wagner J,
    4. Solis-Escalante T,
    5. Müller-Putz GR
    (2015) High and low gamma EEG oscillations in central sensorimotor areas are conversely modulated during the human gait cycle. Neuroimage 112:318–326, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.045, pmid:25818687.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  94. ↵
    1. Severens M,
    2. Nienhuis B,
    3. Desain P,
    4. Duysens J
    (2012) IEEE International Conference of Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (San Diego, CA), Feasibility of measuring event related desynchronization with electroencephalography during walking, pp 2764–2767.
  95. ↵
    1. Siegel M,
    2. Donner TH,
    3. Engel AK
    (2012) Spectral fingerprints of large-scale neuronal interactions. Nat Rev Neurosci 13:121–134, doi:10.1038/nrn3137, pmid:22233726.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  96. ↵
    1. Snyder KL,
    2. Kline JE,
    3. Huang HJ,
    4. Ferris DP
    (2015) Independent component analysis of gait-related movement artifact recorded using EEG electrodes during treadmill walking. Front Hum Neurosci 9:639, doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00639, pmid:26648858.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  97. ↵
    1. Solis-Escalante T,
    2. Müller-Putz GR,
    3. Pfurtscheller G,
    4. Neuper C
    (2012) Cue-induced beta rebound during withholding of overt and covert foot movement. Clin Neurophysiol 123:1182–1190, doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2012.01.013, pmid:22349305.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  98. ↵
    1. Suzuki M,
    2. Miyai I,
    3. Ono T,
    4. Oda I,
    5. Konishi I,
    6. Kochiyama T,
    7. Kubota K
    (2004) Prefrontal and premotor cortices are involved in adapting walking and running speed on the treadmill: an optical imaging study. Neuroimage 23:1020–1026, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.002, pmid:15528102.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  99. ↵
    1. Swann N,
    2. Tandon N,
    3. Canolty R,
    4. Ellmore TM,
    5. McEvoy LK,
    6. Dreyer S,
    7. DiSano M,
    8. Aron AR
    (2009) Intracranial EEG reveals a time-and frequency-specific role for the right inferior frontal gyrus and primary motor cortex in stopping initiated responses. J Neurosci 29:12675–12685, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3359-09.2009, pmid:19812342.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  100. ↵
    1. Thaut MH,
    2. Miller RA,
    3. Schauer LM
    (1998) Multiple synchronization strategies in rhythmic sensorimotor tasks: phase vs period correction. Biol Cybern 79:241–250, doi:10.1007/s004220050474, pmid:9810681.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  101. ↵
    1. Thaut MH,
    2. Demartin M,
    3. Sanes JN
    (2008) Brain networks for integrative rhythm formation. PLoS One 3:e2312, doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0002312, pmid:18509462.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  102. ↵
    1. Thaut MH,
    2. Abiru M
    (2010) Rhythmic auditory stimulation in rehabilitation of movement disorders: a review of current research. Music Percept 27:263–269, doi:10.1525/mp.2010.27.4.263.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  103. ↵
    1. Varraine E,
    2. Bonnard M,
    3. Pailhous J
    (2000) Intentional on-line adaptation of stride length in human walking. Exp Brain Res 130:248–257, doi:10.1007/s002219900234, pmid:10672479.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  104. ↵
    1. Wagner J,
    2. Solis-Escalante T,
    3. Grieshofer P,
    4. Neuper C,
    5. Müller-Putz G,
    6. Scherer R
    (2012) Level of participation in robotic-assisted treadmill walking modulates midline sensorimotor EEG rhythms in able-bodied subjects. Neuroimage 63:1203–1211, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.019, pmid:22906791.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  105. ↵
    1. Wagner J,
    2. Solis-Escalante T,
    3. Scherer R,
    4. Neuper C,
    5. Müller-Putz G
    (2014) It's how you get there: walking down a virtual alley activates premotor and parietal areas. Front Hum Neurosci 8:93, doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00093, pmid:24611043.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  106. ↵
    1. Weerdesteyn V,
    2. Rijken H,
    3. Geurts AC,
    4. Smits-Engelsman BC,
    5. Mulder T,
    6. Duysens J
    (2006) A five-week exercise program can reduce falls and improve obstacle avoidance in the elderly. Gerontology 52:131–141, doi:10.1159/000091822, pmid:16645293.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  107. ↵
    1. Wittfoth M,
    2. Schardt DM,
    3. Fahle M,
    4. Herrmann M
    (2009) How the brain resolves high conflict situations: double conflict involvement of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 44:1201–1209, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.026, pmid:18951983.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  108. ↵
    1. Zhang Y,
    2. Chen Y,
    3. Bressler SL,
    4. Ding M
    (2008) Response preparation and inhibition: the role of the cortical sensorimotor beta rhythm. Neuroscience 156:238–246, doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.06.061, pmid:18674598.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 36 (7)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 36, Issue 7
17 Feb 2016
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Distinct β Band Oscillatory Networks Subserving Motor and Cognitive Control during Gait Adaptation
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Distinct β Band Oscillatory Networks Subserving Motor and Cognitive Control during Gait Adaptation
Johanna Wagner, Scott Makeig, Mateusz Gola, Christa Neuper, Gernot Müller-Putz
Journal of Neuroscience 17 February 2016, 36 (7) 2212-2226; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3543-15.2016

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Distinct β Band Oscillatory Networks Subserving Motor and Cognitive Control during Gait Adaptation
Johanna Wagner, Scott Makeig, Mateusz Gola, Christa Neuper, Gernot Müller-Putz
Journal of Neuroscience 17 February 2016, 36 (7) 2212-2226; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3543-15.2016
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • β band oscillations
  • EEG
  • gait adaptation
  • independent component analysis
  • motor inhibition
  • rhythmic auditory cueing

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Articles

  • Choice Behavior Guided by Learned, But Not Innate, Taste Aversion Recruits the Orbitofrontal Cortex
  • Maturation of Spontaneous Firing Properties after Hearing Onset in Rat Auditory Nerve Fibers: Spontaneous Rates, Refractoriness, and Interfiber Correlations
  • Insulin Treatment Prevents Neuroinflammation and Neuronal Injury with Restored Neurobehavioral Function in Models of HIV/AIDS Neurodegeneration
Show more Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Learning a Model of Shape Selectivity in V4 Cells Reveals Shape Encoding Mechanisms in the Brain
  • A Fluid Self-Concept: How the Brain Maintains Coherence and Positivity across an Interconnected Self-Concept While Incorporating Social Feedback
  • A Texture Statistics Encoding Model Reveals Hierarchical Feature Selectivity across Human Visual Cortex
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.