Abstract
Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) of human occipital and posterior parietal cortex can give rise to visual sensations called phosphenes. We used near-threshold TMS with concurrent EEG recordings to measure how oscillatory brain dynamics covary, on single trials, with the perception of phosphenes after occipital and parietal TMS. Prestimulus power and phase, predominantly in the alpha band (8–13 Hz), predicted occipital TMS phosphenes, whereas higher-frequency beta-band (13–20 Hz) power (but not phase) predicted parietal TMS phosphenes. TMS-evoked responses related to phosphene perception were similar across stimulation sites and were characterized by an early (200 ms) posterior negativity and a later (>300 ms) parietal positivity in the time domain and an increase in low-frequency (∼5–7 Hz) power followed by a broadband decrease in alpha/beta power in the time–frequency domain. These correlates of phosphene perception closely resemble known electrophysiological correlates of conscious perception of near-threshold visual stimuli. The regionally differential pattern of prestimulus predictors of phosphene perception suggests that distinct frequencies may reflect cortical excitability in occipital versus posterior parietal cortex, calling into question the broader assumption that the alpha rhythm may serve as a general index of cortical excitability.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Alpha-band oscillations are thought to reflect cortical excitability and are therefore ascribed an important role in gating information transmission across cortex. We probed cortical excitability directly in human occipital and parietal cortex and observed that, whereas alpha-band dynamics indeed reflect excitability of occipital areas, beta-band activity was most predictive of parietal cortex excitability. Differences in the state of cortical excitability predicted perceptual outcomes (phosphenes), which were manifest in both early and late patterns of evoked activity, revealing the time course of phosphene perception. Our findings prompt revision of the notion that alpha activity reflects excitability across all of cortex and suggest instead that excitability in different regions is reflected in distinct frequency bands.
Introduction
Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) can elicit muscle contractions when applied over motor cortex (Merton et al., 1982) and can induce visual sensations (phosphenes) when applied over regions of occipital cortex (Kammer et al., 2005). These phenomena can be used to study variability in the state of cortical excitability of the tissue undergoing stimulation by measuring changes in the magnitude or likelihood of these responses as stimulation parameters are kept constant (Pascual-Leone et al., 1998). When TMS is combined with concurrent EEG recordings, variability in cortical excitability can be linked to variability in ongoing neural activity. Using this approach, prior work has found that the power of oscillatory neural activity in the alpha band (8–13 Hz) before the onset of TMS is negatively correlated with both phosphene perception (Romei et al., 2008a) and the likelihood and magnitude of motor-evoked potentials (Zarkowski et al., 2006; Sauseng et al., 2009). These findings are taken as evidence that cortical excitability is regulated by neuronal oscillations in the alpha range (Jensen and Mazaheri, 2010). Recent studies have also found that the prestimulus alpha phase is predictive of the perception of phosphenes (Dugué et al., 2011) and visual stimuli (Busch et al., 2009; Mathewson et al., 2009; Samaha et al., 2015), leading to the proposal that alpha may impose inhibition in a phasic manner (Mathewson et al., 2011).
Because this approach relies on stimulating brain areas that give rise to observable or reportable responses, previous work has been limited to investigating the excitability of visual and motor cortices. Recently, however, several groups have reported that TMS of posterior parietal cortex (PPC) can also give rise to phosphenes (Marzi et al., 2009; Mazzi et al., 2014; Tapia et al., 2014; Bagattini et al., 2015). Although the phenomenology of PPC phosphenes has been reported to be comparable to those elicited from occipital stimulation (Fried et al., 2011), the physiological correlates of the two have not been compared directly. We therefore undertook characterization of the cortical excitability of PPC and compared it with occipital cortex by applying TMS at phosphene-threshold levels to both areas while recording EEG. If alpha-band dynamics reflect cortical excitability in PPC, then alpha power and/or phase would also be predictive of parietal TMS phosphenes. Alternatively, oscillations in the lower beta-band (13–30 Hz) may play this role for PPC because beta has been proposed as the “natural frequency” of PPC on the basis of the observation that the dominant EEG response to PPC TMS is ∼20 Hz (Rosanova et al., 2009; Ferrarelli et al., 2012). Furthermore, prestimulus dynamics of ∼20 Hz predict the global mean field amplitude of the EEG response to TMS of PPC (Kundu et al., 2014).
Our approach also allowed us to investigate the neural correlates of TMS-evoked phosphene perception. Prior work using visual stimuli has focused on two ERP components that vary with awareness: an early posterior “visual-awareness negativity” (VAN) and a late central-parietal positivity (LP) (for review, see Koivisto and Revonsuo, 2010; Railo et al., 2011). Our study allowed us to investigate whether these components generalize to the perception of phosphenes. Because spontaneous and TMS-evoked brain activity fluctuates from trial-to-trial in a continuous manner, we hypothesized that the perceptual states linked to these fluctuations would also vary continuously. We tested this hypothesis by measuring the strength of phosphene perception on a continuous scale ranging from “clearly present” to “clearly absent.” These reports served as responses for single-trial regression analyses relating phosphene perception to fluctuations in EEG voltage, power, and phase. Our findings reveal the temporal and oscillatory dynamics of neural activity related monotonically to phosphene perception induced by occipital and parietal TMS and suggest that, whereas alpha-band oscillations reflect cortical excitability of occipital cortex, beta power reflects excitability of parietal cortex.
Materials and Methods
Subjects.
Seventeen subjects were recruited for the experiment for monetary compensation. After phosphene screening, which entailed single-pulse stimulation of right occipital and right parietal cortex at up to 90% of maximum stimulator output, 10 subjects (8 male; 24–33 years old) who perceived phosphenes reliably at both stimulation sites were retained for EEG recording. This proportion is consistent with reports of 60–80% of subjects who perceive occipital TMS phosphenes without extensive training and resulted in a sample size comparable to previous TMS–EEG experiments examining phosphenes (Romei et al., 2008a; Dugué et al., 2011; Bagattini et al., 2015). All subjects were recruited from the University of Wisconsin–Madison community. The University of Wisconsin–Madison Health Sciences Institutional Review Board approved the study protocol. All subjects gave informed consent and were screened for the presence of neurological and psychiatric conditions and other risk factors related to the application of TMS.
Stimulation and phosphene thresholding.
As described previously (Marzi et al., 2009; Fried et al., 2011), stimulation of right occipital cortex was delivered over electrode O2 and stimulation of right PPC was delivered over electrode P4. The coil handle was oriented medial to lateral, away from the inion, during occipital stimulation and ventromedial to dorsolateral, pointing away from the inion for parietal stimulation (see arrows in Fig. 1B). The final coordinates of stimulation were then determined functionally by slightly adjusting the coil until phosphenes were elicited reliably. Phosphene perception was considered reliable if participants reported them in the visual field contralateral to stimulation and if they ceased with decreased stimulation intensity. To ensure consistent stimulation throughout the experiment, the final stimulation coordinates were saved and visualized using a navigated brain stimulation (NBS) system (Nexstim) that uses infrared-based frameless stereotaxy to map the position of the coil and the subject's head within the reference space of the individual's high-resolution T1-weighted anatomical MRI (acquired with a GE MR750 3 T scanner for each subject before the experiment). During EEG recording, the coil was held in place by the experimenter, who monitored the NBS system to maintain consistent stimulation coordinates. Phosphene thresholds were defined for each subject and stimulation site before the experiment as the stimulator intensity that lead to a 50% rate of phosphene reports (yes/no verbal response) after 10 single-pulse stimulations. This was accomplished by increasing stimulator intensity until a phosphene was reported and then adjusting stimulator intensity in 1% steps until 5/10 phosphenes were reported. As noted in prior work (Fried et al., 2011; Tapia et al., 2014; Bagattini et al., 2015), parietal TMS phosphene thresholds required higher stimulator intensities (M = 83.2% of maximum stimulator output, SD = 9.2) than occipital (M = 71.9%, SD = 8.1). A thin layer of foam was placed between the coil and the EEG cap to help minimize TMS artifacts and auditory artifacts due to bone conduction (Massimini et al., 2005). This resulted in a higher intensity stimulator output (compared with prior work), to achieve phosphene thresholds. TMS was delivered with a Magstim Super Rapid2 magnetic stimulator fit with a focal bipulse, figure of eight 70 mm stimulating coil.
Experimental session.
Each subject completed eight blocks of 100 trials each. Single-pulse TMS was delivered to occipital cortex on half of the blocks and to parietal cortex on the remaining half. Occipital and parietal stimulation blocks were interleaved and the order was counterbalanced across participants. The task is depicted in Figure 1A. Each trial began with a random intertrial interval between 1000 and 1500 ms during which a dark gray fixation cross was presented atop a black background. After each pulse of TMS, subjects' maintained central fixation for 1 s, at which point a dark grayscale appeared. The left and right end points of the semicontinuous scale were respectively labeled “clearly absent” and “clearly present.” (Although no digits appeared along the response display, responses were recorded by the computer as an integer value ranging from “0” at the extreme left of the bar to “100” at the extreme right.) Subjects were instructed to rate the presence of phosphenes by sliding a computer mouse with their right hand to the appropriate position on the scale and clicking the mouse. A perpendicular bar marked the current scale position that was reset to the center of the scale on each trial, which subjects were informed corresponded to an “unsure” response. Therefore, this scale captured the subjective certainty or clarity of phosphenes in a monotonic fashion, with increasing values indicating greater subjective certainty in the presence of phosphenes. As a check of basic task compliance, TMS was not delivered on 5% of all trials (randomly determined), which, if subjects understood the scale, we expect would result in reports near zero.
EEG acquisition and preprocessing.
EEG was recorded from 60 Ag/AgCl electrodes connected to a TMS-compatible amplifier (Nexstim). This amplifier avoids saturation from the TMS pulse with a sample-and-hold circuit that holds amplifier output constant from 100 μs before to 2 ms after stimulus. Impedance at each electrode was kept at <5 kΩ. A single electrode placed on the forehead was used as the reference and eye movements were recorded with two additional electrodes placed below and on the outer side of the right eye. Data were acquired at a rate of 1450 Hz with 16-bit resolution. To reduce contamination of the EEG by auditory responses from TMS, masking white noise was played through inserted earplugs throughout the experimental session, as in previous experiments in our laboratory (Kundu et al., 2014). EEG was processed offline with custom MATLAB scripts (version R2014b) and with the EEGLAB toolbox version 13.5 (Delorme and Makeig, 2004). Recordings were inspected visually and noisy channels (3.9 on average) were spherically interpolated. Data were downsampled to 500 Hz and re-referenced to the average of all electrodes. Because, for some subjects, residual high-amplitude artifacts persisted for 20–30 ms after TMS, the data from −10 to 40 ms surrounding each TMS pulse was removed and interpolated via robust splines (Garcia, 2010). These artifacts resembled those caused by stimulation of cranial muscles and occurred in some subjects due to the choice of stimulation site being optimized for eliciting phosphenes, making the window from −10 to 40 ms difficult to interpret. A one-pass zero-phase Hamming windowed-sinc FIR filter between 0.5 and 50 Hz was applied to the data (EEGLAB function “pop_eegfiltnew.m”) and epochs spanning −1500 to 1500 relative to TMS onset were extracted. A prestimulus baseline of −200 to −10 was then subtracted from each trial. Individual trials were then inspected visually and those containing muscle artifacts or ocular artifacts occurring contemporaneously with TMS onset were removed, resulting in an average of 322 occipital stimulation and 323 parietal stimulation trials remaining per subject. Independent components analysis using the INFOMAX algorithm (EEGLAB function “binica.m”) was used to remove remaining ocular artifacts not coinciding with TMS, as well as artifactual components clearly related to TMS. An average of 2.7 ocular artifacts and 6.8 TMS-related artifacts were removed per subject. Raw data and commented code used for all preprocessing and analysis are freely available for download through the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/6qu3b/).
Time–domain analysis.
To relate continuously varying perceptual ratings to continuously varying voltage across time, we performed nonparametric robust regression on single-trial data. For each time point, electrode, and subject, regression coefficients that describe the monotonic relationship between voltage and phosphene presence ratings were estimated according to the following linear model: where X is the design matrix containing one column for the intercept (all ones) and one column for perceptual ratings across trials; the superscripts “T” and “−1” indicate the matrix transpose and inverse, respectively; and D is the vector of voltage data from all trials at a given time point. The resulting beta coefficient representing the slope of the voltage–phosphene relationship was then converted into a z-statistic relative to a subject-specific null hypothesis distribution obtained by repeatedly shuffling the mapping between ratings and voltage (see “Statistics” section). Before regression, the voltage data were smoothed with a 20 ms sliding average window and both phosphene ratings and voltage were rank scored to mitigate the influence of outlying data while testing for a monotonic relationship (this is equivalent to computing a Spearman's correlation coefficient). These normalized beta coefficients were then averaged over a cluster of posterior electrodes (visualized in Fig. 2B, inset) to improve the signal-to-noise ratio. To validate this procedure against a more traditional time–domain analysis approach, trial-averaged ERPs were also computed by sorting trials into “phosphene present” and “phosphene absent” bins if they were greater than (less than) the 55th (45th) percentile of the scale (to exclude middle “unsure” trials). As described previously (Pitts et al., 2014b; Railo et al., 2015; Shafto and Pitts, 2015), the LP/P3 potential was examined at the central parietal electrode Pz and the VAN was examined at occipital electrode Oz. Time windows for each component were determined from data orthogonal to later statistical contrasts by inspecting the condition-averaged ERP. VAN was averaged over a window that spanned 180–220 ms and the LP over a window that spanned 300–800 ms.
Time–frequency analysis.
The same single-trial regression approach was used to relate time–frequency power to phosphene perception. Time–frequency decomposition was performed by convolving data from each trial with a family of complex Morlet wavelets spanning 2–50 Hz in 1.23 Hz steps, with wavelet cycles increasing linearly between 3 and 8 cycles as a function of frequency. Power was obtained by squaring the absolute value of the resulting complex time series and was converted to percentage signal change relative to a prestimulus baseline of −600 to −100 ms to adjust for power law scaling. After regression, normalized beta coefficients were then averaged over the same cluster of posterior electrodes that was used in the time–domain regression analysis (see above). Due to the temporal smearing inherent in time–frequency decomposition, caution must be used when analyzing prestimulus effects, particularly with respect to phase (Zoefel and Heil, 2013), which can result from a confound due to contamination of prestimulus data by poststimulus differences. Therefore, we focused further on prestimulus power and phase by performing an FFT on data segments cut from −1000 to −50 ms before TMS onset. Before the FFT, these segments were linearly detrended, multiplied by a Hamming window, and zero padded (frequency resolution: 0.1 Hz). Power was extracted by squaring the absolute value of the Fourier coefficients and phase was obtained by taking the angle (MATLAB function “angle.m”). Single-trial prestimulus power was related to phosphene perception as before and was also binned into high and low ratings, again, to illustrate differences in a more traditional manner. For this analysis, power was log10 transformed and averaged over the alpha band (8–13 Hz) and low-beta band (13–20 Hz).
Because phase is a circular variable, it cannot be related to a linear variable by means of ordinary linear regression. We therefore computed a recently introduced measure of circular-linear association called “weighted intertrial phase clustering” (wITPC) (Cohen and Cavanagh, 2011; Cohen and Voytek, 2013; Cohen, 2014) to relate prestimulus phase to phospehene ratings. wITPC is computed as the resultant vector length, or intertrial phase clustering (also called the phase-locking factor or intertrial coherence), of phase angles across trials once the length of each vector has been weighted by the linear variable of interest (an example of this computation with data from one subject is shown in Fig. 4B). wITPC was computed for each subject, TMS condition, electrode, and frequency by multiplying the unit length complex-valued phase angle on each trial by the corresponding trial's perceptual rating, averaging those complex numbers across trials, and taking the absolute value to obtain the resultant vector length. Because wITPC will be non-normally distributed and the magnitude will be strongly determined by the scale of the linear (weighting) variable, it is necessary to normalize this quantity with respect to a null distribution obtained by shuffling trial labels. Positive normalized wITPC values indicate that phase modulates phosphene perception, or, in other words, that certain perceptual ratings are more likely than chance to occur at certain phase angles of the measured oscillation.
Statistics.
Level one (subject-level) statistics were performed for all time and time–frequency domain single-trial analysis described above by randomly permuting the mapping between phosphene ratings and neural data 1000 times, each time recomputing the relevant statistic (beta coefficient or wITPC). The statistic associated with the true data mapping was then converted to a z-statistic relative to the mean and SD of the permuted data. This resulted in a z-value for each analysis, subject, TMS condition, channel, time point, or time–frequency point. This approach incorporates knowledge about variability in the subject-level effects into the subsequent group-level analyses. Level two (group-level) statistics and significance values were also computed by means of nonparametric permutation tests in combination with threshold free cluster enhancement (TFCE) to address multiple comparisons across time and frequency points. To estimate group-level null hypothesis distributions, on each of 5000 permutations, z-scores from a random subset of subjects were multiplied by −1 and a t test against zero was computed. This is equivalent to randomly swapping the order of the condition subtraction; for example, A-B versus B-A (Maris and Oostenveld, 2007). The t-statistic resulting from the true data mapping was then subject to TFCE as implemented in the LIMO EEG package (Pernet et al., 2011), which uses the algorithm developed by Smith and Nichols (2009). Each permutated t-statistic was also submitted to TFCE, forming a distribution of cluster extents expected under the null hypothesis. Only cluster extents in the real data exceeding an α rate of 0.05% were considered statistically significant. This procedure has been shown to control well the familywise error rate across multiple comparisons while taking into account the autocorrelation of electrophysiological data (Pernet et al., 2015). All tests were two-tailed.
Results
Phosphene ratings
The average rating of phosphene presence (±SD) was 42.5 ± 8.9 (of 100) after occipital stimulation and 41.8 ± 4.8 after stimulation, confirming that our thresholding procedure was effective. As shown in Figure 1C, subjects made use of the full range of the scale, although with varying degrees of gradedness (i.e., some subjects used the end point values more frequently than others). When sorted into 11 bins (as in Fig. 1C), the shape of the group-averaged histogram had four local maxima that were the same for occipital and parietal TMS. These peaks were at ratings of 4.5, 22.7, 68.18, and 95.5, consistent with previous work that identified 4 “categories” of perceptual experience underlying the use of continuous perceptual scales (Ramsøy and Overgaard, 2004; Overgaard et al., 2006). As expected, ratings on no-TMS trials were near zero (1.9 ± 3), indicating basic task compliance and ensuring that subjects understood the directionality of the scale. These results suggest that additional information can be gained by allowing subjective perceptual responses to take a nonbinary form.
Time–domain correlates of phosphene perception
ERP's contrasting high and low perceptual ratings are presented in Figure 2A. High phosphene presence trials were associated with larger TMS-evoked VAN amplitudes after both occipital (t(1,9) = −4.39, p = 0.002) and parietal (t(1,9) = −3.79, p = 0.004) TMS. This effect was similar across stimulation conditions, but had a more temporal–occipital scalp distribution in the occipital TMS condition and more central occipital–parietal distribution in the parietal TMS condition. High ratings were also associated with an enhanced LP potential over posterior electrodes for occipital (t(1,9) = 4.68, p = 0.001) and parietal (t(1,9) = 5.49, p < 0.001) TMS. These results were also borne out of the single-trial regression analysis (Fig. 2B). In addition to significant negative correlations in the VAN time range (180–220 ms) and positive correlations in the LP time range (300–800), the single-trial analyses revealed a robust prestimulus correlation during the occipital TMS condition. This prestimulus correlation had a reversing polarity over time, with a period of ∼100 ms, suggesting that the prestimulus alpha-band phase influenced phosphene perception. The scalp distributions of these significant regression parameters highly resemble those attained from the ERP analysis, with the early (200 ms) effect being maximal over occipital–temporal electrodes after occipital TMS and maximal over central occipital–parietal electrodes after parietal TMS and the late effect being parietal–maximal in both stimulation conditions.
Time–frequency power correlates of phosphene perception
The relationship between single-trial phosphene perception and oscillatory power across time and frequency space is shown in Figure 3. The analysis of occipital TMS data revealed a negative relationship between prestimulus low-frequency power (∼5–13 Hz) and perception that had a posterior scalp distribution. TMS-evoked low-frequency power (∼3–6 Hz) between ∼140 and 400 ms over lateral occipital electrodes was positively correlated with phosphene perception, whereas later (∼250–900 ms) posterior alpha/beta power was robustly negatively correlated with perceptual ratings. Regarding parietal TMS, no clear effect of prestimulus low-frequency power was observed; rather, prestimulus high-alpha/low-beta power (10–24 Hz) was negatively related to phosphene perception. The topography of this effect was more widespread, with both a posterior and a central–frontal distribution. The relationship between TMS-evoked power and phosphene perception after parietal TMS closely resembled that of occipital stimulation: early low-frequency power was positively predictive of perceptual ratings, followed by a broadband alpha/beta component that was negatively related to phosphene perception.
Prestimulus oscillatory power predicts phosphene perception
Figure 4A shows a complementary analysis of power focused just on the prestimulus interval to avoid any contamination from poststimulus, TMS-induced responses (Zoefel and Heil, 2013). Power spectra of prestimulus data from trials with high and low phosphene ratings show a clear alpha-band peak with a power that was higher on low perceptual rating occipital TMS trials (t(1,9) = −3.82, p = 0.004). In contrast, during parietal TMS, prestimulus beta power was significantly lower on trials with high phosphene presence ratings (t(1,9) = −2.92, p = 0.017). A repeated-measures ANOVA predicting power from phosphene ratings (low, high), TMS site (occipital, parietal), and frequency (alpha, beta) revealed a significant 2-way interaction (F(1,9) = 21.15, p = 0.001), indicating that the effect of power on phosphene perception depended on stimulation site and frequency. The results of the single-trial regression analysis (Fig. 4B) revealed a clear distinction between the prestimulus predictors of phosphenes after occipital and parietal TMS, with broad-band low-frequency power (3–13 Hz) negatively correlating with occipital TMS phosphene perception and higher-frequency beta-band (12–22 Hz) power negatively correlating with parietal TMS phosphene perception. The topographies of the regression coefficients closely resembled those obtained from the time–frequency analysis described above, with both having maximal effects over posterior as well as frontal sensors. The interaction between stimulation site and frequency band was assessed with a paired t test on the double subtraction of normalized beta coefficients during occipital and parietal TMS between the alpha- and beta-bands. Consistent with the 2-way interaction reported above, this revealed that the relationship between prestimulus power and phosphene perception depended on stimulation site and frequency (t(1,9) = 4.74, p = 0.001).
Prestimulus alpha phase predicts phosphenes after occipital TMS
Figure 4C depicts an example of wITPC computation (Cohen, 2014) using a single subject's prestimulus alpha phase during occipital TMS to predict phosphene perception (see “Time–frequency analysis” section for more details). The results of this procedure applied across frequencies revealed a significant phase–perception relationship in the alpha band (peak at 11.6 Hz) during occipital TMS, which was marginally larger than that observed during parietal TMS (t(1,9) = 2.24, p = 0.052). This effect was maximal over both occipital and frontal electrodes. No significant phase–perception effects survived correction for multiple comparisons during parietal TMS. The effect of prestimulus alpha phase on phosphene reports was further demonstrated by binning trials into 7 bins according to prestimulus phase at 11.6 Hz (normalized to percentage change relative to the mean across all bins). As shown in Figure 5, this revealed a clear modulation of perceptual rating according to prestimulus phase during occipital TMS (F(6,54) = 3.23, p = 0.008), but not parietal TMS (F(6,54) = 1.88, p = 0.102), as determined from a one-way ANOVA predicting phosphene reports from phase bin. A further 2-by-7 ANOVA including an interaction term between phase bin and TMS site revealed a significant interaction (F(6,54) = 6.53, p < 0.001), indicating that the prestimulus alpha phase effect depended on stimulation site. Although this interaction could be driven either by a true lack of a phase relationship during parietal TMS or by a comparable phase relationship just centered on a different preferred phase, we think the latter is unlikely given the lack of significant alpha phase modulation during parietal TMS in addition to the results of the between-site difference in the wITPCz analysis, which is insensitive to preferred phase.
Discussion
We applied TMS to occipital and PPC at phosphene thresholds while recording EEG. We replicated two previous findings demonstrating a negative relationship between prestimulus alpha power and phosphene reports (Romei et al., 2008a) and a dependence of phosphene perception on prestimulus alpha phase (Dugué et al., 2011). Notably, when phosphenes were induced through stimulation of PPC, we found that prestimulus beta-band power (13–20 Hz) was negatively correlated with phosphene perception and no phase dependence was observed. TMS-evoked responses differentiated levels of phosphene perception ∼180 ms after TMS onset at both stimulation sites and was associated with an early negativity and a later central parietal positivity. Time–frequency responses indicated that phosphene perception was associated with increased power in the theta range, followed by a sustained decrease of power in the alpha/beta range for both stimulation sites.
Rethinking alpha power as a general index of cortical excitability
Prevailing theory regards alpha-band oscillations as reflecting mechanisms of inhibition that can be routed across brain areas so as to suppress activity in task-irrelevant neural populations (Klimesch et al., 2007; Romei et al., 2008b; Jensen and Mazaheri, 2010; Foxe and Snyder, 2011; Haegens et al., 2011b; de Pesters et al., 2016). Given the link between alpha dynamics and excitability in occipital (Romei et al., 2008a; Dugué et al., 2011) and motor cortex (Zarkowski et al., 2006; Sauseng et al., 2009) and observations of attentional modulation of alpha power in somatosensory (Haegens et al., 2011a), auditory (Banerjee et al., 2011), and retinotopic regions (Worden et al., 2000; Sauseng et al., 2005; Samaha et al., 2016b), it is thought that alpha oscillations reflect a general mechanism of functional inhibition across cortex (Payne and Sekuler, 2014). Although we replicate prior findings linking phosphene perception to prestimulus alpha dynamics during occipital TMS, the finding that prestimulus beta power was predictive of PPC TMS phosphenes is in direct contrast to the notion that alpha reflects excitability across all of cortex.
Several lines of evidence also suggest that the idea of alpha as reflecting inhibition across cortex is overly simplistic. For instance, visual-evoked multiunit and gamma-band activity in macaque inferotemporal cortex correlate positively with prestimulus alpha power in the local field potential, which was also found to increase when attention was paid to visual, as opposed to auditory, input (Mo et al., 2011). Similarly, in humans, predictions about an upcoming stimulus have been shown to increase alpha power yet result in larger evoked responses, with the two processes being positively correlated (Mayer et al., 2016; Samaha et al., 2016a). A possible explanation for our finding that prestimulus beta power, rather than alpha, predicted PPC TMS phosphenes is suggested by recent work reporting that the dominant frequency evoked by TMS to occipital cortex is ∼10 Hz, whereas the dominant frequency of the PPC TMS-evoked response is ∼20 Hz (Rosanova et al., 2009; Ferrarelli et al., 2012). Prestimulus beta dynamics have been shown recently to predict the global mean field amplitude of the evoked response after parietal TMS (Kundu et al., 2014). Therefore, beta-band oscillations may reflect the dominant frequency of PPC and alpha may reflect the dominant frequency of occipital cortex. We extend this work by showing that alpha and beta reflect directly the excitability of occipital cortex and PPC, respectively.
Neural correlates of consciousness and phosphene perception
Phosphene perception for both stimulation sites was associated with enhanced VAN and LP components in the time domain (Fig. 2). These two components have been studied intensively using visual stimuli (for reviews, see: Koivisto and Revonsuo, 2010; Railo et al., 2011; Rutiku et al., 2016), but disagreement about which reflects the neural correlate of consciousness persists (Aru et al., 2012; Railo et al., 2015). The LP potential (sometimes called the P3) has been argued to reflect the neural correlate of consciousness by proponents of the neuronal global workspace theory (Salti et al., 2015). In this context, the LP is thought to reflect widespread activation of a network of prefrontal and parietal regions that comprise the global workspace and underlie consciousness (Dehaene and Changeux, 2011). In contrast, the VAN is typically distributed over posterior sensors, though it may have a frontal component as well (Railo et al., 2015), and is observed earlier, typically between 180 and 260 ms (Koivisto et al., 2008; Railo et al., 2015), although it is often delayed in response to low-contrast stimuli (Railo et al., 2011; Tagliabue et al., 2016). The VAN is sometimes interpreted as reflecting recurrent processes in visual cortex hypothesized to underlie awareness (Fahrenfort et al., 2007; Koivisto et al., 2008). Support for the VAN as a correlate of consciousness has come from research suggesting that attention and/or introspection may account for the LP, but not VAN, modulation with awareness (Pitts et al., 2014b, 2014a; Shafto and Pitts, 2015) and from work demonstrating superior decodability of visibility from VAN versus LP time windows (Andersen et al., 2016). Activity in the LP time window, however, has been shown to contain stimulus specific information that covaries with subjective awareness (Salti et al., 2015).
In the present study, both components were robustly correlated with phosphene perception in both stimulation conditions. Although we cannot unambiguously identify the phosphene-related activity reported here as the VAN and LP components that have been identified in studies using visual stimuli, we do see marked similarities in timing, scalp topography, waveform polarity, and the direction of perception-related effects. Because the waveforms in our study reflect not only proximal (i.e., 40–100 ms) brain activity induced solely by TMS (i.e., “TMS-evoked potentials”; TEP), but also perceptual- and cognitive-related activity underlying the perceptual experience of phosphenes and the perceptual decision making processes involved in performing the task, we would expect perception-related effects beyond the initial TEP, which is consistent with the timing of visual awareness (Dehaene and Changeux, 2011; Railo et al., 2011). Although the timing of the effects that we observed is consistent with previous ERP correlates of phosphene perception (Taylor et al., 2010; Dugué et al., 2011; Bagattini et al., 2015), we did not replicate the earliest (∼100 ms) effects observed by Bagattini et al. (2015).
Interestingly, the topography of LP modulation was maximal over central–parietal sensors for both stimulation conditions, whereas the VAN modulation was slightly more pronounced over lateral temporal–occipital sensors after occipital TMS and had a central occipital–parietal distribution after PPC TMS, a pattern closely resembling that observed in a recent ERP experiment with occipital and parietal TMS phosphenes (Bagattini et al., 2015). This implies that the early activity associated with phosphene perception may be generated in nearby but distinguishable cortical regions depending on the initial site of stimulation. This is intriguing because it implies that there may be several, rather than a single, neural correlates of phosphene perception. It further suggests that the VAN “component” is not necessarily a unitary phenomenon reflecting the activation of a single neural region related to visual awareness. In fact, it has been demonstrated recently that hemianopic patients with complete loss of primary visual cortex in one hemisphere are still capable of perceiving phosphenes in their blind field when ipsilesional PPC is stimulated (Mazzi et al., 2014; Bagattini et al., 2015). This strongly undermines the presumed visual–cortical origin of PPC TMS phosphenes and supports the notion that the VAN associated with PPC TMS need not be visual–cortical in origin and need not reflect recurrent processing involving visual cortex, both of which are often assumed of the VAN component (Railo et al., 2011, 2015; Andersen et al., 2016).
We also found that phosphene perception in both stimulation conditions was positively related to power in the theta range beginning ∼140 ms and negatively related to alpha/beta power extending from 250 ms until the response screen (Fig. 3). Whereas this early positive theta correlation likely reflects the same phase-locked activity underlying the VAN modulation, which occurs during the same time frame, the later negative correlation with alpha/beta power reflects dynamics that are not phase locked with TMS onset and are thus not observed in the ERP, a signature of a truly oscillatory neural process (Cohen and Donner, 2013). Poststimulus alpha/beta desynchronization has long been linked to perceptual processing (Pfurtscheller et al., 1994; Sewards and Sewards, 1999; Babiloni et al., 2006) and is thought to reflect disinhibition of widespread cortical networks involved in perceptual inference and decision making (Klimesch et al., 2007; Donner and Siegel, 2011). Here, we show that this well known signature of perceptual processing extends to phosphene perception and is strikingly similar regardless of the cortical origin of stimulation.
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health grant RO1MH095984 (to B.R.P.) and the Belgian National Fund for Scientific Research (FNRS) and Human Brain Project grant EU-H2020-FETFLAGSHIP-HBP-SGA1-GA720270 (to O.G.).
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Jason Samaha, Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin, 1202 W. Johnson St., Madison, WI 53703. jsamaha{at}wisc.edu