Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Visuospatial Asymmetries Arise from Differences in the Onset Time of Perceptual Evidence Accumulation

Daniel P. Newman, Gerard M. Loughnane, Simon P. Kelly, Redmond G. O'Connell and Mark A. Bellgrove
Journal of Neuroscience 22 March 2017, 37 (12) 3378-3385; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3512-16.2017
Daniel P. Newman
1Monash Institute for Cognitive and Clinical Neurosciences, School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University, Melbourne, Victoria 3800, Australia,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Daniel P. Newman
Gerard M. Loughnane
2School of Engineering,
3Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Simon P. Kelly
5School of Electrical and Electronic Engineering, University College Dublin, Dublin 4, Ireland
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Redmond G. O'Connell
1Monash Institute for Cognitive and Clinical Neurosciences, School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University, Melbourne, Victoria 3800, Australia,
3Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience, and
4School of Psychology, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin 2, Ireland, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Mark A. Bellgrove
1Monash Institute for Cognitive and Clinical Neurosciences, School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University, Melbourne, Victoria 3800, Australia,
3Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Healthy subjects tend to exhibit a bias of visual attention whereby left hemifield stimuli are processed more quickly and accurately than stimuli appearing in the right hemifield. It has long been held that this phenomenon arises from the dominant role of the right cerebral hemisphere in regulating attention. However, methods that would enable more precise understanding of the mechanisms underpinning visuospatial bias have remained elusive. We sought to finely trace the temporal evolution of spatial biases by leveraging a novel bilateral dot motion detection paradigm. In combination with electroencephalography, this paradigm enables researchers to isolate discrete neural signals reflecting the key neural processes needed for making these detection decisions. These include signals for spatial attention, early target selection, evidence accumulation, and motor preparation. Using this method, we established that three key neural markers accounted for unique between-subject variation in visuospatial bias: hemispheric asymmetry in posterior α power measured before target onset, which is related to the distribution of preparatory attention across the visual field; asymmetry in the peak latency of the early N2c target-selection signal; and, finally, asymmetry in the onset time of the subsequent neural evidence-accumulation process with earlier onsets for left hemifield targets. Our development of a single paradigm to dissociate distinct processing components that track the temporal evolution of spatial biases not only advances our understanding of the neural mechanisms underpinning normal visuospatial attention bias, but may also in the future aid differential diagnoses in disorders of spatial attention.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The significance of this research is twofold. First, it shows that individual differences in how humans direct their attention between left and right space reflects physiological differences in how early the brain starts to accumulate evidence for the existence of a visual target. Second, the novel methods developed here may have particular relevance to disorders of attention, such as unilateral spatial neglect. In the case of spatial neglect, pathological inattention to left space could have multiple underlying causes, including biased attention, impaired decision formation, or a motor deficit related to one side of space. Our development of a single paradigm to dissociate each of these components may aid in supporting more precise differential diagnosis in such heterogeneous disorders.

  • asymmetry
  • decision making
  • EEG
  • evidence accumulation
  • pseudoneglect
  • spatial attention

Introduction

It has long been held that visuospatial attention is lateralized to the right hemisphere (Bogen and Gazzaniga, 1965; Mesulam, 1981; Young and Ratcliff, 1983; De Renzi, 1985). Primary evidence supporting this claim is that the neglect syndrome, which involves a profound spatial inattention for the contralesional hemifield, is more common, severe, and long-lasting following right compared with left hemisphere damage (Stone et al., 1993; Corbetta and Shulman, 2011). Subtle processing advantages for visuospatial stimuli presented to the left hemifield are also seen in most healthy individuals, a phenomenon known as “pseudoneglect” (Bowers and Heilman, 1980; Nicholls et al., 1999; Jewell and McCourt, 2000; Voyer et al., 2012). Recent evidence from genetics (Bellgrove et al., 2005, 2007, 2008, 2009; Newman et al., 2012; Zozulinsky et al., 2014) and structural, functional, and molecular brain imaging (Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2011; Tomer et al., 2013; Chechlacz et al., 2015; Marshall et al., 2015) suggests that substantial individual difference factors contribute to visuospatial biases. Thiebaut de Schotten and colleagues reported that individual differences in the degree of visuospatial bias correlated with the extent of hemispheric asymmetry in white matter frontoparietal pathways, with larger volumes in the right, relative to left, associated with faster response times for left, compared with right, targets (Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2011). One implication of this work is that individual differences in structural hemispheric asymmetries may facilitate faster processing of visuospatial stimuli presented to one or the other visual hemifield.

Perceptual decision making requires multiple neural-processing stages ranging from those for representing, selecting, and accumulating sensory information to those for preparing and executing actions. Visuospatial asymmetries can potentially reflect the contribution of any one of these processing stages, and thus the ability to measure each stage separately is important if we are to understand the neural processes underpinning normal and abnormal visuospatial biases. Recent developments in behavioral/electroencephalographic (EEG) paradigms make it possible to measure discrete neural signals for each of these processing stages, which are each related to forthcoming behavioral performance. Such signals include those that distribute attention across hemifields and are measured via interhemispheric asymmetry in pretarget preparatory α power (Thut et al., 2006; Loughnane et al., 2015); those that enable early target selection and are measured in the form of the contralateral N2c component (Loughnane et al., 2016); those that support evidence accumulation and are measured via the centroparietal positivity (CPP), whose build-up rate predicts reaction time (RT) and scales with sensory evidence strength independent of sensory or motor demands (O'Connell et al., 2012; Kelly and O'Connell, 2013); and those that enable limb-selective motor preparation and are measured via contralateral β-band activity (Donner et al., 2009; O'Connell et al., 2012; de Lange et al., 2013).

Disruption to one or more of these stages when processing left or right hemifield targets could result in slower orienting and decision responses. The development of a single paradigm that can dissociate these neural signals within an individual and make it possible to track temporal evolution of spatial biases could have important theoretical and clinical benefits in terms of improved differential diagnoses in conditions where aberrant visuospatial attention indexes neurological vulnerability, such as unilateral spatial neglect (Corbetta and Shulman, 2011), attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Voeller and Heilman, 1988; Sheppard et al., 1999; Geeraerts et al., 2008; Chan et al., 2009; Bellgrove et al., 2013), and dyslexia (Facoetti et al., 2001; Hari et al., 2001; Liddle et al., 2009; Stenneken et al., 2011).

Here, for the first time, we modeled individual differences in left versus right hemifield RT asymmetry as a function of asymmetries in neural signals representing each of the temporal stages of perceptual decision making to determine which signals explained unique variation in visuospatial bias among healthy individuals.

Materials and Methods

Participants.

Data were collected from 80 (42 female) healthy right-handed volunteers, ages 18–37 years (mean, 23.2 years), reporting normal or corrected-to-normal vision, no history of neurological or psychiatric disorder, and no head injury resulting in loss of consciousness. Participants were recruited and tested under identical experimental protocols at either Monash University, Australia (N = 60), or Trinity College Dublin, Ireland (N = 20). The experimental protocol was approved by each university's human research ethics committee before testing, and carried out in accordance with the approved guidelines. Informed consent was obtained from all participants before testing.

Materials and task procedures.

Participants were seated in a darkened room 56 cm from a 21 inch CRT (85 Hz, 1024 × 768 resolution) and asked to perform a variant of the random dot motion task (Newsome et al., 1989; Britten et al., 1992; Kelly and O'Connell, 2013; Loughnane et al., 2016). In this version of the task, participants fixated centrally and monitored two peripheral patches (one in each hemifield) of 150 moving dots for targets, which were defined by a seamless transition from random motion to coherent motion in a downward direction (Fig. 1A). Upon detecting a target, participants made a speeded button press with their right index finger. Peers et al. (2006), using a similar paradigm, found that the response hand had no effect on spatial bias. Targets were separated by intervals of random motion of 3.06, 5.17, or 7.29 s (randomized throughout each block). Participants completed 16 blocks consisting of 18 targets, each block lasting ∼3 min. Participants were given a break of ∼30 s in between each block. An SR Research EyeLink eye tracker (EyeLink version 2.04, SR Research/SMI) recorded eye movements, which were later analyzed to ensure that participants maintained fixation. The center of each random dot motion patch was at a visual angle 10° either side and 4° below the fixation square; each patch covered 8° visual angle and consisted of 150 6 × 6 pixel white dots. The dot stimuli were flickered on and off the screen every 23.5 ms, resulting in a flicker rate of 21.25 frames/s (each “frame” representing two screen refreshes). During random motion, these dots were placed randomly throughout the patch on each frame. During coherent motion, a proportion (50%) of the dots was randomly selected on each frame to be displaced in the downward direction on the following frame, with a motion speed of 6° per second. The paradigm was run using Matlab (MathWorks) and the Psychophysics Toolbox extensions (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997; Cornelissen et al., 2002).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

A, Behavioral paradigm. Participants fixated on the central dot while monitoring the two peripheral patches of randomly moving dots for intermittent periods of coherent downward motion that could occur in either the left or the right hemifield at an unpredictable time. Participants made a speeded button press upon detection of the coherent motion target. B, Pretarget-α power was greater over right than left hemisphere posterior regions. C, Target Hemifield had no significant effect on the N2c or N2i signals. D, Target Hemifield had no significant effect on motor selective LHB desynchronization. Insets throughout the subplots show scalp topography of each of the neural signals, and the error ribbons (C and D) represent 95% CI for mean amplitude.

EEG acquisition and preprocessing.

Continuous EEG was acquired from 64 scalp electrodes using an ActiveTwo system (Biosemi) digitized at 512 Hz (Trinity College Dublin data collection) or using a BrainAmp DC system (Brain Products) digitized at 500 Hz (Monash University data collection). Data were processed using a combination of custom scripts and EEGLAB (Delorme and Makeig, 2004) routines implemented in Matlab (MathWorks). All processing scripts used for the current study can be found on-line (https://github.com/gerontium/big_dots). A 35 Hz low-pass filter was applied off-line using a fourth-order Butterworth filter, noisy channels were interpolated (spherical spline), and the data were rereferenced to the average reference. Data from Trinity College Dublin and Monash University were combined by resampling the data collected in Dublin to 500 Hz. Epochs were then extracted from the continuous data from −700 to 1800 ms around target onset, and baselined with respect to −100 to 0 ms before target onset.

Trials were excluded from analysis if any of the following conditions were met: (1) if RTs were <150 ms (pre-emptive responses) or >1800 ms (responses after coherent motion offset); (2) if the EEG from any channel exceeded ±100 μV during the interval from −500 to 0 ms before target onset for pretarget-α analysis, or during the interval from −100 ms before target onset to 100 ms after RT for the ERP analysis; or (3) if central fixation was broken by blinking or eye movement >3° left or right of center, either from −500 to 0 ms before target onset for pretarget-α analysis or during the interval between 100 ms before target onset and 100 ms after response for the ERP analysis.

Power in the α band was calculated using temporal spectral evolution (TSE) methods, following Thut and colleagues (2006). Each epoch was bandpass filtered to α range (8–13 Hz), rectified (converted to absolute values), and trimmed to exclude the 200 ms at the beginning and end of the epoch to eliminate filter warm-up artifacts. Data were then smoothed by averaging inside a moving window of 100 ms, moving forward in 50 ms increments. α Measurements were pooled from two symmetric parietal regions of interest (ROIs; left hemisphere: CP5, CP3, P5, P3; right hemisphere: CP4, CP6, P6, P8; Thut et al., 2006; ter Huurne et al., 2013). Pretarget-α power was defined as the mean power from −500 to 0 ms before target onset (Newman et al., 2016) and aggregated to participant means.

The N2c and N2i components were measured contralateral and ipsilateral to the target location, respectively, at electrodes P7 and P8 (Loughnane et al., 2016), while the CPP was measured at peak electrodes Pz (O'Connell et al., 2012; Kelly and O'Connell, 2013; Murphy et al., 2015; Twomey et al., 2015; Loughnane et al., 2016).

The N2c, N2i, and CPP signals were aggregated to average waveforms as a function of target hemifield for each participant. N2 latency was then identified as the time point with the most negative amplitude value in the stimulus-locked waveform between 150 and 400 ms for the N2c and 200–450 ms for N2i, while N2 amplitude was measured as the mean amplitude inside a 100 ms window centered on the stimulus-locked grand average peak (N2c: 266 ms; N2i: 340 ms; Loughnane et al., 2016). CPP build-up rate was defined as the slope of a straight line fitted to the response-locked waveform (O'Connell et al., 2012; Kelly and O'Connell, 2013; Loughnane et al., 2016) with the time window defined individually for each participant as the 100 ms before the maximum CPP amplitude preresponse. Onset latency of the CPP was measured by performing running sample point by sample point t tests against zero across each participant's stimulus-locked CPP waveforms. CPP onset was defined as the first point at which the amplitude reached significance at the 0.05 level for ≥15 consecutive points (Foxe and Simpson, 2002; Kelly et al., 2008; Loughnane et al., 2016).

Left hemisphere β power (LHB; 20–35 Hz) was measured from the standard motor site C3, using short-time Fourier transform (STFT) with a 286 ms window size and 20 ms step size (O'Connell et al., 2012). The LHB signal was baselined with respect to −100 to 0 ms before target onset and aggregated to average waveforms as a function of the target hemifield for each participant. LHB amplitude was defined as the mean amplitude of the stimulus-locked waveform during the 300–500 ms window. The LHB build-up rate was defined as the slope of a straight line fitted to the response-locked waveform from −300 to −50 ms before response. Note STFT and TSE methods for measuring LHB and α power yielded highly comparable results.

Inferential analysis.

Analysis of the effects of Hemisphere (left, right) on pretarget-α power, and of Target Hemifield (left, right) on the N2c/i, CPP, β, and RT measures were conducted using repeated-measures ANOVA. In cases where the assumption of normality was violated, the p value (ppermuted) of a permutation test for the effect of Hemifield based on 1000 permutations is also reported.

To assess the extent that each of the neural signals (pretarget α, N2c/i, CPP, β) could explain variation in visuospatial attention asymmetry between individuals, we calculated an index of each individual's visuospatial attention asymmetry as follows: RT asymmetry = (left target RT − right target RT)/(left + right target RT). This index gives positive values when RTs are faster for right relative to the left targets (rightward spatial bias) and negative values when the opposite is true (leftward bias). Corresponding neural signal indices were also calculated for each of the target-evoked signals (i.e., N2c, CPP, and β) as follows: signal asymmetry = (left target signal − right target signal)/(left + right target signal). A similar formula was used to calculate hemispheric asymmetry for pretarget parietal α power: α asymmetry = (right ROI α − left ROI α)/(right + left ROI α). This index gives positive values when α activity is greater over the right hemisphere ROI relative to the left hemisphere ROI and negative values when α activity is greater over the left hemisphere ROI relative to the right. If no asymmetry exists then the index gives a zero value (for comparable use of asymmetry indices, see Thut et al., 2006).

After forcing the control variables (test location, sex, age) into a linear regression model predicting RT asymmetry, the neural signal asymmetry indices were each added sequentially into consecutive regression models in a hierarchical fashion with their entry order determined by their temporal order in the perceptual decision-making process. This allowed us to model individual differences in RT asymmetry, as a function of asymmetries in neural signals representing each temporal stage of perceptual decision making, from a pretarget preparatory signal (α power), to target selection (N2c), to evidence accumulation (CPP), to motor preparation (LHB). The hierarchical entry method allowed us to assess whether each of the different signals improved the model fit for RT asymmetry over and above the signals that temporally precede it. Only those signals that explained unique variation in RT asymmetry were then selected for forced input into a final simplified RT-asymmetry model, which made it possible to obtain accurate parameter estimates not influenced by other signals shown not to improve model fit.

Analyses in the current manuscript focused on visuospatial biases at the level of individual participants, rather than within-participant absolute RT. Nevertheless, in accordance with past work, each of the neural signals in the current study was significantly correlated with absolute RT (e.g., pretarget α: Thut et al., 2006, Loughnane et al., 2015; the N2c component: Loughnane et al., 2016; the CPP: O'Connell et al., 2012; Kelly and O'Connell, 2013; limb-selective LHB: Donner et al., 2009; O'Connell et al., 2012; de Lange et al., 2013).

Results

Target-detection accuracy was at ceiling (mean, 98.7%; range: 92–100%) and a factorial permutation test showed no effect of Target Hemifield on accuracy (p = 0.852). As expected, RTs were significantly faster for left hemifield targets (mean, 561 ms; SD = 91) than right hemifield targets [mean, 576 ms; SD = 105; F(1,79) = 11.09, p = 0.001, ηG2 (generalized η-squared) = 0.006, ppermuted = 0.001], which is characteristic of the phenomenon of pseudoneglect (Fig. 2A). A significant effect of Hemisphere showed that pretarget-α power was greater over the right (mean, 1.98 μV; SD = 0.95) compared with left parietal regions (mean, 1.85 μV; SD = 0.97; F(1,79) = 11.47, p = 0.001, ηG2 = 0.004, ppermuted = 0.001; Fig. 1B). Although the N2c was noticeably larger following left hemifield targets, and the N2i larger following right hemifield targets, these differences did not produce significant main effects of Hemifield (N2i latency F(1,79) = 0.15, p = 0.695, ppermuted = 0.691; N2i amplitude F(1,79) = 1.26, p = 0.265; N2c latency F(1,79) = 0.005, p = 0.939, ppermuted = 0.946; N2c amplitude F(1,79) = 0.08, p = 0.784; Fig. 1C). Combining N2c and N2i into a Hemisphere (contralateral, ipsilateral) × Hemifield (left, right) ANOVA confirmed that the contralateral signal is larger and earlier than the ipsilateral signal (amplitude F(1,79) = 62.74, p < 0.001, ηG2 = 0.13; latency F(1,79) = 94.22, p < 0.001, ppermuted < 0.001, ηG2 = 0.21; Fig. 1C). However, there was no significant main effect of Hemifield (amplitude F(1,79) = 0.40, p = 0.528; latency F(1,79) = 0.16, p = 0.695, ppermuted = 0.688) or Hemisphere × Hemifield interaction (amplitude F(1,79) = 0.62, p = 0.434; latency F(1,79) = 0.09, p = 0.769, ppermuted = 0.743).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

A, CPP onset and RT both were significantly faster for left than right hemifield targets. Violin plots show conditional distributions while the purple dot-and-whisker plots show mean and SE. Vertical lines project the means down to B. B, Stimulus-locked CPP waveform shows earlier onset for left than right hemifield targets. Inset shows the scalp topography of the CPP. Error ribbons represent 95% CI for mean amplitude.

The CPP signal began significantly earlier for left hemifield targets (mean, 319 ms; SD = 72) than for right hemifield targets (mean, 341 ms; SD = 65; F(1,79) = 5.54, p = 0.025, ηG2 = 0.025), suggesting an earlier onset of the evidence accumulation process for left hemifield targets, consistent with the left hemifield RT advantage (Fig. 2A,B). There was, however, no effect of Hemifield on the build-up rate of the CPP signal (F(1,79) = 1.26, p = 0.266) or on the motor-selective LHB signal (LHB amplitude F(1,79) = 0.68, p = 0.413, ppermuted = 0.418; LHB slope F(1,79) = 0.19, p = 0.661, ppermuted = 0.66; Fig. 1D).

Next, the neural signal asymmetry indices were each added to a model predicting RT asymmetry in a hierarchical fashion with their entry order determined by their temporal order in the perceptual decision-making process. This method allowed us to assess whether each of the different signals improved the model fit for RT asymmetry over and above the preceding signal. Note that none of the neural asymmetry predictors were highly correlated with each other.

When compared with an intercept-only model, the linear regression model including the control variables (test location, sex, age) offered no significant improvement in modeling RT asymmetry (R2 = 0.042, F(3,76) = 1.27, p = 0.29). The addition of pretarget-α asymmetry significantly improved model fit (R2 = 0.086, Rchange2 = 0.044, F(1,75) = 4.02, p = 0.048). The addition of N2c-amplitude asymmetry did not improve the model (R2 = 0.086, Rchange2 = 0.00, F(1,74) = 0.01, p = 0.918), while N2c-latency asymmetry provided a significant improvement (R2 = 0.133, Rchange2 = 0.047, F(1,73) = 4.30, p = 0.041). Adding CPP-onset asymmetry also gave a significant improvement to the model (R2 = 0.21, Rchange2 = 0.08, F(1,72) = 7.43, p = 0.008), while CPP-slope asymmetry did not (R2 = 0.22, Rchange2 = 0.01, F(1,71) = 0.99, p = 0.324). Neither LHB-amplitude asymmetry (R2 = 0.23, Rchange2 = 0.01, F(1,70) = 0.84, p = 0.363) nor LHB-slope asymmetry (R2 = 0.25, Rchange2 = 0.02, F(1,96) = 1.64, p = 0.204) offered any improvement to the RT-asymmetry model. Overall, this hierarchical regression analysis shows that the pretarget-α, N2c-latency, and CPP-onset asymmetry indices each exert a partially independent influence on RT asymmetry.

An algorithm for forward/backward stepwise model selection using the Akaike information criterion (Venables and Ripley, 2002) supported the results from the hierarchical method, confirming that the only neural asymmetry signals to improve the model for RT asymmetry are pretarget-α asymmetry, N2c-latency asymmetry, and CPP-onset asymmetry. Thus, these three signals only were then forced into a final linear regression model of RT asymmetry (R2 = 0.19, F(3,76) = 5.82, p = 0.001) and their resulting parameter estimates are shown in Table 1 below. This simplified model was necessary to obtain accurate parameter estimates for pretarget-α, N2c-latency, and CPP-onset asymmetry that are not influenced by the other, noninformative, signals.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Parameter estimates from the final linear regression model of RT asymmetry, which included only those neural asymmetry measures that improved model fit

Table 1 shows pretarget-α asymmetry (p = 0.057) and N2c-latency asymmetry (p = 0.067) fell short of the nominal statistical significance threshold for their effect on RT asymmetry when forced into the final linear regression model. This result is ambiguous since it conflicts with the results of the hierarchical regression method used above, which found that, along with CPP-onset asymmetry, both pretarget-α and N2c-latency asymmetry added significant improvement to the RT-asymmetry model. Likewise, the forward/backwards stepwise method selected pretarget α, N2c latency, and CPP onset as the best predictors for the model. Therefore, to confirm whether each of the three neural asymmetry measures had a significant effect on RT asymmetry, we performed “robust regression” based on 5000 bootstrap replicates to calculate the 95% CI around slope estimates for the final model (Table 2).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Robust regression analysis

The robust regression (Table 2) confirmed that pretarget-α asymmetry, N2c-latency asymmetry, and CPP-onset asymmetry all have a significant positive relationship with RT asymmetry. These effects are in the expected direction based on past research (Thut et al., 2006; Loughnane et al., 2015). The positive relationship between pretarget-α asymmetry and RT asymmetry (Fig. 3A) indicates that those participants with more positive pretarget-α asymmetry (i.e., relatively greater pretarget-α power over their right than left hemisphere) tended to have more positive RT asymmetry (i.e., relatively slower RTs for left hemifield targets or relatively faster RTs for right hemifield targets). The positive relationship between N2c-latency asymmetry and RT asymmetry (Fig. 3B) indicates that participants with more negative N2c-latency asymmetry (i.e., earlier N2c latency for left than right hemifield targets) also tended to have more negative RT asymmetry (i.e., tended to respond relatively faster to left than right hemifield targets). Finally, CPP-onset asymmetry had the strongest relationship with RT asymmetry (Tables 1, 2). This positive relationship (Fig. 3C) indicates that participants who had more leftward CPP-onset bias (i.e., CPP onset relatively earlier for left than right hemifield targets) also tended to have more prominent leftward RT asymmetry (i.e., tended to respond faster to left than right hemifield targets). This result suggests that earlier onset of the evidence accumulation process for left hemifield targets leads to a left hemifield RT advantage. That CPP-onset asymmetry was the strongest predictor of RT asymmetry is not due solely to its temporal proximity to response execution. This is the case since neither CPP slope nor LHB asymmetry, which occur closer to response, explained unique variance in RT asymmetry.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Pretarget-α, N2c-latency, and CPP-onset asymmetry indices each exert a partially independent influence on RT asymmetry. A, The positive relationship indicates that those participants with relatively greater pretarget-α power over their right than over their left hemisphere also tended to have relatively slower RTs for left hemifield targets than for right hemifield targets. B, Participants with earlier N2c latency for left than for right hemifield targets also tended to have relatively faster RTs to left than to right hemifield targets. C, Participants who had earlier CPP onsets for left than right hemifield targets also tended to have faster RTs for left than right hemifield targets. Note that the relationships shown in A and B are of small effect size. However, a “robust regression analysis” confirmed that their slope is significantly >0 (Table 2).

Discussion

This study asked whether individual differences in visuospatial asymmetry are indexed by functional asymmetries in electrocortical activity. We used a perceptual decision-making framework, with bilaterally presented dot motion targets, to track the temporal evolution of discrete EEG signals contributing to visuospatial biases. In line with previous reports, we found that hemispheric differences in pretarget posterior α-band activity accounted for individual differences in visuospatial bias. But further to this, we also uncovered independent contributions from target-selection and evidence-accumulation signals that have not been previously reported. An asymmetry index derived from the peak latency of the N2c, a target-selection signal, explained unique variation in visuospatial bias among individuals. The CPP signal, which tracks perceptual evidence accumulation, began earlier for left than for the right hemifield targets and the degree of this asymmetry also explained individual differences in visuospatial bias.

Previous electrophysiology work on visuospatial attention has focused on hemispheric asymmetries in pretarget-α power (Worden et al., 2000; Sauseng et al., 2005; Thut et al., 2006; Gould et al., 2011; Newman et al., 2013; ter Huurne et al., 2013; Capilla et al., 2014; Loughnane et al., 2015). These studies have shown that processing advantages for the left relative to the right hemifield, particularly in cued target-detection paradigms, are related to more desynchronization of α over right, relative to left, posterior regions. We support this finding, albeit with a relatively small effect size, in the context of an uncued fixation-controlled paradigm. Although absolute α power was greater over the right posterior hemisphere at the group grand average (Slagter et al., 2016), individual differences in the hemispheric α-asymmetry index related to RT asymmetry in the direction expected. The positive relationship between α and RT asymmetry indicates that individuals with relatively less spontaneous pretarget-α power over their right than left hemisphere tended to have relatively faster RTs for left than right hemifield targets, as expected. It is interesting therefore that the grand averages show greater right hemisphere α and faster left hemifield RTs at the group level. It is possible that this is caused by a separate source of right hemisphere α modulation, which is not related to spatial bias and which merges on the scalp with the signal produced by α generators for spatial attention.

The absence of sudden stimulus intensity changes in the current paradigm eliminated visual-evoked transients from the event-related EEG trace. This helped provide after target onset a clearer view of the neural signals that contribute to decision formation, increasing their signal-to-noise ratio and aiding in their correlation with behavior. On the other hand, transient stimuli (e.g., a checkerboard appearing on screen) can elicit the P1 component and other visual evoked potentials than occur earlier those recorded in the current study. Those visual evoked potentials are modulated by spatial attention and reflect activity of both striate and extrastriate regions (Robinson and Rugg, 1988; Luck, 1995; Clark and Hillyard, 1996; Di Russo et al., 2002, 2008). Thus, the measurement of such signals may also prove informative for investigating stimulus-driven asymmetries in spatial-attention bias.

Recently, Loughnane et al. (2016) used this paradigm to demonstrate that early target-selection signals reliably precede the onset of the neural evidence-accumulation process during perceptual decision formation. The N2 selection signal, which consists of distinct contralateral and ipsilateral components, was found to predict behavioral performance (RT and accuracy) and the build-up rate of the CPP evidence-accumulation signal, even in the absence of distractor stimuli (Loughnane et al., 2016). Our novel finding that those with relatively earlier N2c latency over their right than left hemispheres also tend to have relatively faster RTs for left than right hemifield targets suggests that the speed of contralateral target-selection processes is a factor in individual differences in spatial bias.

CPP onset, which began earlier for left than for right hemifield targets, showed a clear effect of Target Hemifield. Furthermore, the left versus right hemifield asymmetry index for CPP onset showed the strongest relationship with RT asymmetry. The CPP signal was recently characterized as a domain-general signal representing a build-to-threshold decision variable, with dynamics that predict the timing and accuracy of perceptual reports independently of specific sensory or motor requirements (O'Connell et al., 2012; Kelly and O'Connell, 2013; Murphy et al., 2015; Twomey et al., 2015; Loughnane et al., 2016). Here we build on mechanistically principled characterization of the relevant signals to investigate neural asymmetry indices and thereby advance our understanding of neural correlates of visuospatial bias. Since all of the neural signals used here are robustly related to absolute RTs, it is informative that only a subset of their asymmetry indexes was related to individual differences in RT asymmetry.

The strongest of these indexes was CPP onset, which reflects the onset timing of an evidence-accumulation process (O'Connell et al., 2012; Kelly and O'Connell, 2013; Loughnane et al., 2016; Twomey et al., 2016). The significantly earlier CPP onset for left hemifield targets indicates that the evidence-accumulation process tends to start earlier for those targets, and this likely facilitates the improved processing of left compared with right hemifield targets, commonly described as pseudoneglect (Bowers and Heilman, 1980; Nicholls et al., 1999; Jewell and McCourt, 2000; Voyer et al., 2012). Although the CPP peaks over the central parietal scalp area, its dynamics bear a striking similarity to neural firing rate signals recorded in lateral parietal areas, such as the lateral intraparietal cortex, during primate neurophysiology studies using similar visual stimuli (Shadlen and Newsome, 2001). Since the right parietal cortex is heavily implicated in visuospatial attention in humans (Corbetta and Shulman, 2011), it will be of interest for future perceptual decision work to use combined EEG/fMRI to explore the anatomical source of the CPP signal in humans.

Previous studies have found that contralateral limb-selective β activity, before a motor response, shows similar build-to-threshold dynamics as the CPP (Donner et al., 2009; O'Connell et al., 2012; de Lange et al., 2013). These dynamics in the β signal are only seen when a motor response is required, whereas the CPP represents perceptual decision formation independently of specific motor requirements (O'Connell et al., 2012). Here the LHB signal did not differ between left and right hemifield targets and did not relate to individual differences in visuospatial bias. That CPP onset was related to the target hemifield and to individual differences in visuospatial bias, while LHB was not, may be due to the poorer temporal resolution of the LHB power measure, compared with the high resolution of CPP amplitude. Future work should counterbalance the response hand between blocks, within individual participants, to allow the measurement of a broad-band lateralized readiness potential index (Gratton et al., 1988; de Jong et al., 1988; Eimer, 1998), which would have the same high temporal resolution as the CPP.

It should be noted that the exact results of the current study can only be generalized to the healthy right-handed population (the 90% majority; Sun and Walsh, 2006). This was, however, a deliberate experimental control. Studies of lateralization of function have shown that although handedness varies naturally in the population, left-handedness can sometimes be determined by early developmental effects (Gutwinski et al., 2011; Willems et al., 2014). Consequently, including a left-handed cohort would necessitate substantial increases in the sample size and measures to ensure that left-handedness was not related to developmental differences. Therefore, this study was restricted to right-handed participants, in line with most other studies of spatial attention and with cognitive neuroscience studies more generally (Willems et al., 2014).

Although applied to healthy right-handed adults, the general approach described here may have important implications for studying disorders involving abnormal spatial-orienting biases. In the case of unilateral spatial neglect, for example, pathological inattention to left space could have multiple underlying causes, including biased attention, impaired decision formation, or a motor deficit related to one side of space. Yet current clinical tests for spatial neglect rely on classic paper-and-pencil tests, such as line bisection or cancellation tasks, which confound sensory, attentional, and motoric processes. Our development of a single electrophysiology paradigm that can dissociate each of these components to track the temporal evolution of spatial biases may aid differential diagnosis in such heterogeneous disorders of attention. An unprecedented level of specificity regarding deficient processes may yet be gained in future research by applying these methods to disorders involving aberrant visuospatial biases, such as unilateral spatial neglect, ADHD, and dyslexia (Voeller and Heilman, 1988; Sheppard et al., 1999; Facoetti et al., 2001; Hari et al., 2001; Geeraerts et al., 2008; Chan et al., 2009; Liddle et al., 2009; Corbetta and Shulman, 2011; Stenneken et al., 2011).

Notes

Supplemental material for this article is available at https://figshare.com/articles/Supplemental_Material_Visuospatial_asymmetries_arise_from_differences_in_the_onset_time_of_perceptual_evidence_accumulation/4591027. In accordance with past work, each of the neural signals in the current study was significantly correlated with absolute RTs (Supplemental Fig. 1). Note that none of the neural asymmetry predictors were highly correlated with each other (Supplemental Fig. 2). This material has not been peer reviewed. Analysis scripts and paradigm code (https://github.com/gerontium/big_dots) as well as the raw data (https://figshare.com/s/8d6f461834c47180a444) are open access and available under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 3.0 International License.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by grants from the Australian Research Council (FT130101488, DP150100986). We thank Hayley Pickering and Roisin White for their help with data collection.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Mark A. Bellgrove at the above address. mark.bellgrove{at}monash.edu

References

  1. ↵
    1. Bellgrove MA,
    2. Hawi Z,
    3. Kirley A,
    4. Fitzgerald M,
    5. Gill M,
    6. Robertson IH
    (2005) Association between dopamine transporter (DAT1) genotype, left-sided inattention, and an enhanced response to methylphenidate in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 30:2290–2297. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1300839 pmid:16123773
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Bellgrove MA,
    2. Chambers CD,
    3. Johnson KA,
    4. Daibhis A,
    5. Daly M,
    6. Hawi Z,
    7. Lambert D,
    8. Gill M,
    9. Robertson IH
    (2007) Dopaminergic genotype biases spatial attention in healthy children. Mol Psychiatry 12:786–792. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4002022 pmid:17549062
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Bellgrove MA,
    2. Barry E,
    3. Johnson KA,
    4. Cox M,
    5. Daibhis A,
    6. Daly M,
    7. Hawi Z,
    8. Lambert D,
    9. Fitzgerald M,
    10. McNicholas F,
    11. Robertson IH,
    12. Gill M,
    13. Kirley A,
    14. Dáibhis A
    (2008) Spatial attentional bias as a marker of genetic risk, symptom severity, and stimulant response in ADHD. Neuropsychopharmacology 33:2536–2545. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1301637 pmid:18046306
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Bellgrove MA,
    2. Johnson KA,
    3. Barry E,
    4. Mulligan A,
    5. Hawi Z,
    6. Gill M,
    7. Robertson I,
    8. Chambers CD
    (2009) Dopaminergic haplotype as a predictor of spatial inattention in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 66:1135–1142. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.120 pmid:19805704
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Bellgrove MA,
    2. Eramudugolla R,
    3. Newman DP,
    4. Vance A,
    5. Mattingley JB
    (2013) Influence of attentional load on spatial attention in acquired and developmental disorders of attention. Neuropsychologia 51:1085–1093. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.01.019 pmid:23391561
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Bogen JE,
    2. Gazzaniga MS
    (1965) Cerebral commissurotomy in man: minor hemisphere dominance for certain visuo-spatial functions. J Neurosurg 23:394–399. doi:10.3171/jns.1965.23.4.0394
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  7. ↵
    1. Bowers D,
    2. Heilman KM
    (1980) Pseudoneglect: effects of hemispace on a tactile line bisection task. Neuropsychologia 18:491–498. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(80)90151-7 pmid:6777712
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Brainard DH
    (1997) The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spat Vis 10:433–436. doi:10.1163/156856897X00357 pmid:9176952
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Britten KH,
    2. Shadlen MN,
    3. Newsome WT,
    4. Movshon JA
    (1992) The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance. J Neurosci 12:4745–4765. pmid:1464765
    OpenUrlAbstract
  10. ↵
    1. Capilla A,
    2. Schoffelen JM,
    3. Paterson G,
    4. Thut G,
    5. Gross J
    (2014) Dissociated α-band modulations in the dorsal and ventral visual pathways in visuospatial attention and perception. Cereb Cortex 24:550–561. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhs343 pmid:23118197
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    1. Chan E,
    2. Mattingley JB,
    3. Huang-Pollock C,
    4. English T,
    5. Hester R,
    6. Vance A,
    7. Bellgrove MA
    (2009) Abnormal spatial asymmetry of selective attention in ADHD. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 50:1064–1072. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02096.x pmid:19457046
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Chechlacz M,
    2. Gillebert CR,
    3. Vangkilde SA,
    4. Petersen A,
    5. Humphreys GW
    (2015) Structural variability within frontoparietal networks and individual differences in attentional functions: an approach using the theory of visual attention. J Neurosci 35:10647–10658. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0210-15.2015 pmid:26224851
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  13. ↵
    1. Clark VP,
    2. Hillyard SA
    (1996) Spatial selective attention affects early extrastriate but not striate components of the visual evoked potential. J Cogn Neurosci 8:387–402. doi:10.1162/jocn.1996.8.5.387 pmid:23961943
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Corbetta M,
    2. Shulman GL
    (2011) Spatial neglect and attention networks. Annu Rev Neurosci 34:569–599. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113731 pmid:21692662
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Cornelissen FW,
    2. Peters EM,
    3. Palmer J
    (2002) The Eyelink Toolbox: eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 34:613–617. doi:10.3758/BF03195489 pmid:12564564
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. De Renzi E
    (1985) Disorders of space, exploration and cognition. New York: Wiley.
  17. ↵
    1. de Jong R,
    2. Wierda M,
    3. Mulder G,
    4. Mulder LJ
    (1988) Use of partial stimulus information in response processing. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 14:682–692. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.14.4.682 pmid:2974876
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. de Lange FP,
    2. Rahnev DA,
    3. Donner TH,
    4. Lau H
    (2013) Prestimulus oscillatory activity over motor cortex reflects perceptual expectations. J Neurosci 33:1400–1410. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1094-12.2013 pmid:23345216
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. Delorme A,
    2. Makeig S
    (2004) EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods 134:9–21. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 pmid:15102499
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Di Russo F,
    2. Martínez A,
    3. Sereno MI,
    4. Pitzalis S,
    5. Hillyard SA
    (2002) Cortical sources of the early components of the visual evoked potential. Hum Brain Mapp 15:95–111. doi:10.1002/hbm.10010 pmid:11835601
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Di Russo F,
    2. Aprile T,
    3. Spitoni G,
    4. Spinelli D
    (2008) Impaired visual processing of contralesional stimuli in neglect patients: a visual-evoked potential study. Brain 131:842–854. doi:10.1093/brain/awm281 pmid:18024488
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    1. Donner TH,
    2. Siegel M,
    3. Fries P,
    4. Engel AK
    (2009) Buildup of choice-predictive activity in human motor cortex during perceptual decision making. Curr Biol 19:1581–1585. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.066 pmid:19747828
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Eimer M
    (1998) The lateralized readiness potential as an on-line measure of central response activation processes. Behav Res Methods Instruments Comput 30:146–156. doi:10.3758/BF03209424
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  24. ↵
    1. Facoetti A,
    2. Turatto M,
    3. Lorusso ML,
    4. Mascetti GG
    (2001) Orienting of visual attention in dyslexia: evidence for asymmetric hemispheric control of attention. Exp Brain Res 138:46–53. doi:10.1007/s002210100700 pmid:11374082
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Foxe JJ,
    2. Simpson GV
    (2002) Flow of activation from V1 to frontal cortex in humans: a framework for defining “early” visual processing. Exp Brain Res 142:139–150. doi:10.1007/s00221-001-0906-7 pmid:11797091
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Geeraerts S,
    2. Lafosse C,
    3. Vaes N,
    4. Vandenbussche E,
    5. Verfaillie K
    (2008) Dysfunction of right-hemisphere attentional networks in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 30:42–52. pmid:17852596
    OpenUrlPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Gould IC,
    2. Rushworth MF,
    3. Nobre AC
    (2011) Indexing the graded allocation of visuospatial attention using anticipatory alpha oscillations. J Neurophysiol 105:1318–1326. doi:10.1152/jn.00653.2010 pmid:21228304
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    1. Gratton G,
    2. Coles MG,
    3. Sirevaag EJ,
    4. Eriksen CW,
    5. Donchin E
    (1988) Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 14:331–344. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.14.3.331 pmid:2971764
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Gutwinski S,
    2. Löscher A,
    3. Mahler L,
    4. Kalbitzer J,
    5. Heinz A,
    6. Bermpohl F
    (2011) Understanding left-handedness. Dtsch Arztebl Int 108:849–853. doi:10.3238/arztebl.2011.0849 pmid:22259638
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Hari R,
    2. Renvall H,
    3. Tanskanen T
    (2001) Left minineglect in dyslexic adults. Brain 124:1373–1380. doi:10.1093/brain/124.7.1373 pmid:11408332
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  31. ↵
    1. Jewell G,
    2. McCourt ME
    (2000) Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia 38:93–110. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00045-7 pmid:10617294
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Kelly SP,
    2. O'Connell RG
    (2013) Internal and external influences on the rate of sensory evidence accumulation in the human brain. J Neurosci 33:19434–19441. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3355-13.2013 pmid:24336710
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  33. ↵
    1. Kelly SP,
    2. Gomez-Ramirez M,
    3. Foxe JJ
    (2008) Spatial attention modulates initial afferent activity in human primary visual cortex. Cereb Cortex 18:2629–2636. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn022 pmid:18321874
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    1. Liddle EB,
    2. Jackson GM,
    3. Rorden C,
    4. Jackson SR
    (2009) Lateralized temporal order judgement in dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 47:3244–3254. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.08.007 pmid:19682473
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Loughnane GM,
    2. Shanley JP,
    3. Lalor EC,
    4. O'Connell RG
    (2015) Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence of opposing lateral visuospatial asymmetries in the upper and lower visual fields. Cortex 63:220–231. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2014.09.003 pmid:25282061
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Loughnane GM,
    2. Newman DP,
    3. Bellgrove MA,
    4. Lalor EC,
    5. Kelly SP,
    6. O'Connell RG
    (2016) Target selection signals influence perceptual decisions by modulating the onset and rate of evidence accumulation. Curr Biol 26:496–502. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2015.12.049 pmid:26853360
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Luck SJ
    (1995) Multiple mechanisms of visual-spatial attention: recent evidence from human electrophysiology. Behav Brain Res 71:113–123. pmid:8747179
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Marshall TR,
    2. Bergmann TO,
    3. Jensen O
    (2015) Frontoparietal structural connectivity mediates the top-down control of neuronal synchronization associated with selective attention. PLOS Biol 13:e1002272. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002272 pmid:26441286
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Mesulam MM
    (1981) A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect. Ann Neurol 10:309–325. pmid:7032417
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Murphy PR,
    2. Robertson IH,
    3. Harty S,
    4. O'Connell RG
    (2015) Neural evidence accumulation persists after choice to inform metacognitive judgments. Elife pii:e11946. doi:10.7554/eLife pmid:26687008
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Newman DP,
    2. O'Connell RG,
    3. Nathan PJ,
    4. Bellgrove MA
    (2012) Dopamine transporter genotype predicts attentional asymmetry in healthy adults. Neuropsychologia 50:2823–2829. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.08.012 pmid:22940645
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Newman DP,
    2. O'Connell RG,
    3. Bellgrove MA
    (2013) Linking time-on-task, spatial bias and hemispheric activation asymmetry: a neural correlate of rightward attention drift. Neuropsychologia 51:1215–1223. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.03.027 pmid:23583973
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Newman DP,
    2. Lockley SW,
    3. Loughnane GM,
    4. Martins AC,
    5. Abe R,
    6. Zoratti MT,
    7. Kelly SP,
    8. O'Neill MH,
    9. Rajaratnam SM,
    10. O'Connell RG,
    11. Bellgrove MA
    (2016) Ocular exposure to blue-enriched light has an asymmetric influence on neural activity and spatial attention. Sci Rep 6:27754. doi:10.1038/srep27754 pmid:27291291
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Newsome WT,
    2. Britten KH,
    3. Movshon JA
    (1989) Neuronal correlates of a perceptual decision. Nature 341:52–54. doi:10.1038/341052a0 pmid:2770878
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Nicholls ME,
    2. Bradshaw JL,
    3. Mattingley JB
    (1999) Free-viewing perceptual asymmetries for the judgement of brightness, numerosity and size. Neuropsychologia 37:307–314. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00074-8 pmid:10199644
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. O'Connell RG,
    2. Dockree PM,
    3. Kelly SP
    (2012) A supramodal accumulation-to-bound signal that determines perceptual decisions in humans. Nat Neurosci 15:1729–1735. doi:10.1038/nn.3248 pmid:23103963
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Peers PV,
    2. Cusack R,
    3. Duncan J
    (2006) Modulation of spatial bias in the dual task paradigm: evidence from patients with unilateral parietal lesions and controls. Neuropsychologia 44:1325–1335. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.033 pmid:16522325
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Pelli DG
    (1997) The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis 10:437–442. pmid:9176953
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Robinson DL,
    2. Rugg MD
    (1988) Latencies of visually responsive neurons in various regions of the rhesus monkey brain and their relation to human visual responses. Biol Psychol 26:111–116. pmid:3061476
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Sauseng P,
    2. Klimesch W,
    3. Stadler W,
    4. Schabus M,
    5. Doppelmayr M,
    6. Hanslmayr S,
    7. Gruber WR,
    8. Birbaumer N
    (2005) A shift of visual spatial attention is selectively associated with human EEG alpha activity. Eur J Neurosci 22:2917–2926. pmid:16324126
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Shadlen MN,
    2. Newsome WT
    (2001) Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol 86:1916–1936. pmid:11600651
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  52. ↵
    1. Sheppard DM,
    2. Bradshaw JL,
    3. Mattingley JB,
    4. Lee P
    (1999) Effects of stimulant medication on the lateralisation of line bisection judgements of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 66:57–63. pmid:9886453
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  53. ↵
    1. Slagter HA,
    2. Prinssen S,
    3. Reteig LC,
    4. Mazaheri A
    (2016) Facilitation and inhibition in attention: functional dissociation of pre-stimulus alpha activity, P1, and N1 components. Neuroimage 125:25–35. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.058 pmid:26436713
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Stenneken P,
    2. Egetemeir J,
    3. Schulte-Körne G,
    4. Müller HJ,
    5. Schneider WX,
    6. Finke K
    (2011) Slow perceptual processing at the core of developmental dyslexia: a parameter-based assessment of visual attention. Neuropsychologia 49:3454–3465. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.08.021 pmid:21903119
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Stone SP,
    2. Halligan PW,
    3. Greenwood RJ
    (1993) The incidence of neglect phenomena and related disorders in patients with an acute right or left hemisphere stroke. Age Ageing 22:46–52. pmid:8438666
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  56. ↵
    1. Sun T,
    2. Walsh CA
    (2006) Molecular approaches to brain asymmetry and handedness. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:655–662. pmid:16858393
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. ter Huurne N,
    2. Onnink M,
    3. Kan C,
    4. Franke B,
    5. Buitelaar J,
    6. Jensen O
    (2013) Behavioral consequences of aberrant alpha lateralization in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 74:227–233. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.001 pmid:23507001
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Thiebaut de Schotten M,
    2. Dell'Acqua F,
    3. Forkel SJ,
    4. Simmons A,
    5. Vergani F,
    6. Murphy DG,
    7. Catani M
    (2011) A lateralized brain network for visuospatial attention. Nat Neurosci 14:1245–1246. doi:10.1038/nn.2905 pmid:21926985
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Thut G,
    2. Nietzel A,
    3. Brandt SA,
    4. Pascual-Leone A
    (2006) α-Band electroencephalographic activity over occipital cortex indexes visuospatial attention bias and predicts visual target detection. J Neurosci 26:9494–9502. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0875-06.2006 pmid:16971533
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  60. ↵
    1. Tomer R,
    2. Slagter HA,
    3. Christian BT,
    4. Fox AS,
    5. King CR,
    6. Murali D,
    7. Davidson RJ
    (2013) Dopamine asymmetries predict orienting bias in healthy individuals. Cereb Cortex 23:2899–2904. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhs277 pmid:22941721
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  61. ↵
    1. Twomey DM,
    2. Murphy PR,
    3. Kelly SP,
    4. O'Connell RG
    (2015) The classic P300 encodes a build-to-threshold decision variable. Eur J Neurosci 42:1636–1643. doi:10.1111/ejn.12936 pmid:25925534
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    1. Twomey DM,
    2. Kelly SP,
    3. O'Connell RG
    (2016) Abstract and effector-selective decision signals exhibit qualitatively distinct dynamics before delayed perceptual reports. J Neurosci 36:7346–7352. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4162-15.2016 pmid:27413146
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  63. ↵
    1. Venables WN,
    2. Ripley BD
    (2002) Modern Applied Statistics with S (4th ed.). New York: Springer.
  64. ↵
    1. Voeller KK,
    2. Heilman KM
    (1988) Attention deficit disorder in children: a neglect syndrome? Neurology 38:806–808. doi:10.1212/WNL.38.5.806 pmid:3362382
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  65. ↵
    1. Voyer D,
    2. Voyer SD,
    3. Tramonte L
    (2012) Free-viewing laterality tasks: a multilevel meta-analysis. Neuropsychology 26:551–567. doi:10.1037/a0028631 pmid:22731609
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Willems RM,
    2. Van der Haegen L,
    3. Fisher SE,
    4. Francks C
    (2014) On the other hand: including left-handers in cognitive neuroscience and neurogenetics. Nat Rev Neurosci 15:193–201. doi:10.1038/nrn3679 pmid:24518415
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Worden MS,
    2. Foxe JJ,
    3. Wang N,
    4. Simpson GV
    (2000) Anticipatory biasing of visuospatial attention indexed by retinotopically specific alpha-band electroencephalography increases over occipital cortex. J Neurosci 20:RC63. pmid:10704517
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  68. ↵
    1. Young AW,
    2. Ratcliff G
    (1983) Visuospatial abilities of the right hemisphere. In: Functions of the right cerebral hemisphere (Young AW, ed). London: Academic.
  69. ↵
    1. Zozulinsky P,
    2. Greenbaum L,
    3. Brande-Eilat N,
    4. Braun Y,
    5. Shalev I,
    6. Tomer R
    (2014) Dopamine system genes are associated with orienting bias among healthy individuals. Neuropsychologia 62:48–54. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.07.005 pmid:25038551
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 37 (12)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 37, Issue 12
22 Mar 2017
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Visuospatial Asymmetries Arise from Differences in the Onset Time of Perceptual Evidence Accumulation
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Visuospatial Asymmetries Arise from Differences in the Onset Time of Perceptual Evidence Accumulation
Daniel P. Newman, Gerard M. Loughnane, Simon P. Kelly, Redmond G. O'Connell, Mark A. Bellgrove
Journal of Neuroscience 22 March 2017, 37 (12) 3378-3385; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3512-16.2017

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Visuospatial Asymmetries Arise from Differences in the Onset Time of Perceptual Evidence Accumulation
Daniel P. Newman, Gerard M. Loughnane, Simon P. Kelly, Redmond G. O'Connell, Mark A. Bellgrove
Journal of Neuroscience 22 March 2017, 37 (12) 3378-3385; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3512-16.2017
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Notes
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • asymmetry
  • decision making
  • EEG
  • evidence accumulation
  • pseudoneglect
  • spatial attention

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Regional Excitatory–Inhibitory Balance Relates to Self-Reference Effect on Recollection via the Precuneus/Posterior Cingulate Cortex–Medial Prefrontal Cortex Connectivity
  • Attention Alters Population Spatial Frequency Tuning
  • Sequence Diversity and Encoded Enzymatic Differences of Monocistronic L1 ORF2 mRNA Variants in the Aged Normal and Alzheimer's Disease Brain
Show more Research Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Attention Alters Population Spatial Frequency Tuning
  • Complex Impact of Stimulus Envelope on Motor Synchronization to Sound
  • The Molecular Substrates of Second-Order Conditioned Fear in the Basolateral Amygdala Complex
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.