Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Development/Plasticity/Repair

Development of the Cerebral Cortex across Adolescence: A Multisample Study of Inter-Related Longitudinal Changes in Cortical Volume, Surface Area, and Thickness

Christian K. Tamnes, Megan M. Herting, Anne-Lise Goddings, Rosa Meuwese, Sarah-Jayne Blakemore, Ronald E. Dahl, Berna Güroğlu, Armin Raznahan, Elizabeth R. Sowell, Eveline A. Crone and Kathryn L. Mills
Journal of Neuroscience 22 March 2017, 37 (12) 3402-3412; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3302-16.2017
Christian K. Tamnes
1Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, 0317 Oslo, Norway,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Megan M. Herting
2Department of Preventive Medicine, Keck School of Medicine, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California 90032,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Anne-Lise Goddings
3Institute of Child Health and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Rosa Meuwese
5Brain and Development Research Center, Institute of Psychology, and
6Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Leiden University, 2300 RA Leiden, The Netherlands,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Sarah-Jayne Blakemore
4Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London WC1N 1EH United Kingdom,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Ronald E. Dahl
7Institute of Human Development, University of California Berkeley, Berkeley, California 94720,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Ronald E. Dahl
Berna Güroğlu
5Brain and Development Research Center, Institute of Psychology, and
6Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Leiden University, 2300 RA Leiden, The Netherlands,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Armin Raznahan
8Child Psychiatry Branch, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland 20814,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Elizabeth R. Sowell
9Children's Hospital of Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California 90027, and
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Eveline A. Crone
5Brain and Development Research Center, Institute of Psychology, and
6Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Leiden University, 2300 RA Leiden, The Netherlands,
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Kathryn L. Mills
10Department of Psychology, University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon 97403
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Kathryn L. Mills
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Before we can assess and interpret how developmental changes in human brain structure relate to cognition, affect, and motivation, and how these processes are perturbed in clinical or at-risk populations, we must first precisely understand typical brain development and how changes in different structural components relate to each other. We conducted a multisample magnetic resonance imaging study to investigate the development of cortical volume, surface area, and thickness, as well as their inter-relationships, from late childhood to early adulthood (7–29 years) using four separate longitudinal samples including 388 participants and 854 total scans. These independent datasets were processed and quality-controlled using the same methods, but analyzed separately to study the replicability of the results across sample and image-acquisition characteristics. The results consistently showed widespread and regionally variable nonlinear decreases in cortical volume and thickness and comparably smaller steady decreases in surface area. Further, the dominant contributor to cortical volume reductions during adolescence was thinning. Finally, complex regional and topological patterns of associations between changes in surface area and thickness were observed. Positive relationships were seen in sulcal regions in prefrontal and temporal cortices, while negative relationships were seen mainly in gyral regions in more posterior cortices. Collectively, these results help resolve previous inconsistencies regarding the structural development of the cerebral cortex from childhood to adulthood, and provide novel insight into how changes in the different dimensions of the cortex in this period of life are inter-related.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Different measures of brain anatomy develop differently across adolescence. Their precise trajectories and how they relate to each other throughout development are important to know if we are to fully understand both typical development and disorders involving aberrant brain development. However, our understanding of such trajectories and relationships is still incomplete. To provide accurate characterizations of how different measures of cortical structure develop, we performed an MRI investigation across four independent datasets. The most profound anatomical change in the cortex during adolescence was thinning, with the largest decreases observed in the parietal lobe. There were complex regional patterns of associations between changes in surface area and thickness, with positive relationships seen in sulcal regions in prefrontal and temporal cortices, and negative relationships seen mainly in gyral regions in more posterior cortices.

  • brain development
  • gray matter
  • morphometry
  • MRI
  • replication

Introduction

Insight into postnatal human brain development has been greatly enhanced over the last two decades by the use of imaging methods, particularly magnetic resonance imaging (MRI; Jernigan et al., 2011; Blakemore, 2012; Giedd et al., 2015). There are, however, still fundamental disagreements across available studies regarding the developmental patterns and precise trajectories for cortical volume and its distinct components, surface area and thickness (Mills and Tamnes, 2014). To try to resolve the inconsistencies and provide clues about the processes driving the changes in the different dimensions of the cerebral cortex from childhood to adulthood, we investigated the development of cortical structure concurrently in four separate longitudinal samples, and directly assessed how changes in different cortical measures are inter-related.

Previous results are particularly contradictory with regard to the development of cortical thickness, with some studies reporting increases until late childhood, while others finding continuous thinning from early or midchildhood (Walhovd et al., 2016). Inconsistencies across studies of development of cortical structure may have resulted from varying sample characteristics, image acquisition, image processing, including quality-control (QC) procedures and software used, and/or statistical analyses and curve fitting (Fjell et al., 2010; Sullivan et al., 2011; Aubert-Broche et al., 2013; Ducharme et al., 2016; Mills et al., 2016). One approach to try to clarify these inconsistencies is to conduct multisample studies following current standards and recommendations for processing and analysis. Here, we build upon a recent such study in which we reported replicable models for gross structural brain development between childhood and adulthood (Mills et al., 2016).

Cortical volume is determined by surface area and thickness, and these components are influenced by different evolutionary (Geschwind and Rakic, 2013), genetic (Chen et al., 2013; Kremen et al., 2013), and cellular (Chenn and Walsh, 2002) processes, and show unique changes across different stages of life (Brown et al., 2012; Storsve et al., 2014; Wierenga et al., 2014; Lyall et al., 2015; Amlien et al., 2016). Knowledge about the relative contributions of surface area and thickness to developmental cortical volume changes, and the relationship between changes in surface area and thickness during adolescence, may provide important, although indirect, clues for understanding the biological processes underlying development of cortical structure. In prenatal and perinatal life, the primary processes driving surface-area expansion and thickening are cortical column generation and genesis of neurons within columns, respectively (Rakic, 1988; Bhardwaj et al., 2006). The processes underlying changes in cortical structure throughout childhood and adolescence are less well understood, although we know that the protracted human brain development involves increasing caliber and myelination of axons (Yakovlev and Lecours, 1967; Benes, 1989; Benes et al., 1994), and that early synaptogenesis is followed by pruning (Huttenlocher and Dabholkar, 1997; Petanjek et al., 2011).

To increase our confidence in current interpretations about how the cerebral cortex grows and to gain knowledge that might help us understand the processes driving its development, the present study aimed to (1) characterize the regional developmental trajectories of cortical volume, surface area, and thickness across adolescence in four separate longitudinal samples and (2) directly test how changes in the distinct cortical components are inter-related. Each independent dataset was analyzed separately to examine the consistency and replicability of the results across sample and image-acquisition specifics.

Materials and Methods

Participants.

This study used four separate datasets: Child Psychiatry Branch (CPB), Pittsburgh (PIT), Neurocognitive Development (NCD), and Braintime (BT). Each of these included data about typically developing participants collected at four separate sites (National Institutes of Health, University of Pittsburgh, University of Oslo, Leiden University) in three countries (United States, Norway, Netherlands). All datasets were collected using accelerated longitudinal designs. Each separate study was approved by a local review board. For the CPB dataset, participants and scans were selected from a pool of >1000 scans for their quality and number of time points per individual. For the PIT, NCD, and BT datasets, respectively, 126, 111, and 299 participants were recruited and scanned at baseline. Of these, 20, 26, and 45 dropped out at follow up, and an additional 33, 9, and 45 were excluded based on the QC of the MRI data (see below). The final CPB, PIT, NCD, and BT datasets included 30, 73, 76, and 209 participants, respectively. In total, the present study includes 388 participants (199 females) and 854 scans covering the age range of 7–29 years old (Table 1). Details regarding participant recruitment have been described previously for each sample separately (Tamnes et al., 2013; Herting et al., 2014; Mills et al., 2014b; Braams et al., 2015) and together (Mills et al., 2016).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Participant demographics and MRI acquisition parameters for each sample

Image acquisition and processing.

T1-weighted anatomical scans were obtained at four different sites using different scanners and sequences. Only key parameters are summarized here (Table 1), as details regarding image acquisition at each site were described in detail previously (Mills et al., 2016). At each site, a radiologist reviewed all scans for gross abnormalities. Image processing was performed with FreeSurfer 5.3 (RRID:SCR_001847), which is documented and freely available online (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/). The technical details of the procedures were described in detail previously (Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 1999, 2002). The processing stream includes motion correction (Reuter et al., 2010), removal of nonbrain tissue using a hybrid watershed/surface-deformation procedure (Ségonne et al., 2004), automated Talairach transformation, nonparametric nonuniform intensity normalization (Sled et al., 1998), tessellation of the gray–white matter boundary, automated topology correction (Fischl et al., 2001; Ségonne et al., 2007), and surface deformation following intensity gradients to optimally place the gray–white and gray–CSF borders at the location where the greatest shift in intensity defines the transition to the other tissue class (Dale and Sereno, 1993; Dale et al., 1999; Fischl and Dale, 2000). Each cortical model was registered to a spherical atlas using individual cortical folding patterns to match cortical geometry across participants (Dale et al., 1999).

Images were then processed using FreeSurfer 5.3's longitudinal stream (Reuter et al., 2012). This process includes the creation of an unbiased within-subject template space and image using robust, inverse consistent registration (Reuter et al., 2010). Several processing steps, such as skull stripping, Talairach transforms, and atlas registration, as well as spherical surface maps and parcellations, were then initialized with common information from the within-subject template, significantly increasing reliability and statistical power (Reuter et al., 2012). The QC procedure was coordinated across sites so that all images were visually inspected post-processing by trained operators for accuracy, but no editing was performed.

Surface maps for cortical volume, surface area (white surface), and thickness, as well as symmetrized annual percentage change (APC; i.e., the linear annual rate of change with respect to the average volume/area/thickness measure across all available time points) over all available observations for each measure, were generated and smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of full-width at half-maximum of 15 mm. Additionally, we computed global total cortical volume, total surface area, and weighted mean thickness (with each label contributing to the mean according to its area) for each time point for each subject across all labels in the Desikan–Killiany cortical parcellation (Desikan et al., 2006), and APC for each of the measures. Similar variables were calculated for the frontal (including anterior cingulate), temporal (including insula), parietal (including posterior and retrosplenial cingulate), and occipital lobes, and for each label across both hemispheres.

Statistical analysis.

First, spaghetti plots and longitudinal curve fitting were performed using the Multimodal Imaging Laboratory data portal (Bartsch et al., 2014; Vidal-Pineiro et al., 2016), which uses functions freely available through the statistical environment R (http://www.r-project.org/, RRID:SCR_001905). Cortical volume, surface area, and thickness measures from each time point, adjusted for the effect of sex, were introduced as predicted variables in generalized additive mixed model (GAMM) analyses where the predictor was age, with k parameters specifying the stiffness of the model curves set to five (except for temporal lobe volume where four was used so the models would converge). The main effect of sex was adjusted for through linear mixed-effect models. GAMM can be represented as the following formula: G(y) = X*α + ‖j=1p(xj) + fj(xj) + Zb + ε, where G(·) is a monotonic differentiable link function, α is the vector of regression coefficients for the fixed parameters; X* is the fixed-effects matrix; fj is the smooth function of the covariate xj; Z is the random-effects model matrix; b is the vector of random-effects coefficients; and ε is the residual error vector (Wood, 2006). GAMM fitting was visualized over its correspondent spaghetti plots. Estimated mean values across sex were used as display values. GAMM provides accurate delineations of developmental trajectories, as it avoids some of the inherent weaknesses of global polynomial models, e.g., quadratic and cubic models, where the timing of peaks and the end points of the trajectories may be substantially affected by irrelevant factors, such as the age range sampled (Fjell et al., 2010).

Second, as background analyses before testing for inter-relationships between cortical volume, surface area, and thickness, mean global and lobar APC values for each cortical measure for each sample were calculated, one-sample t tests were used to test whether the APC values were significantly different from zero, and ANOVAs with Tukey's HSD post hoc comparisons were performed to test for sample differences. For each sample, we then performed vertex-wise general linear models (GLMs) as implemented in FreeSurfer 5.3, testing whether APCs for each of the measures were significantly different from zero, with sex, age (mean across time points), and their interaction as covariates.

Third, regional relationships between changes in cortical volume, surface area, and thickness were initially tested for by means of partial correlations between global and lobar APCs in each measure, with sex and mean age as covariates. Then, a series of GLMs were performed in FreeSurfer to test for vertex-wise change–change relationships among the different measures across the cortical surface. APC maps for each measure were entered as per-vertex regressors of interest to the other measures, with sex, mean age, and their interaction as covariates.

All regional (global and lobar) results were Bonferroni-corrected by a factor of five (reflecting the number of regions), corresponding to a corrected α of p < 0.01. For all vertex-wise analyses, the data were tested against an empirical null distribution of maximum cluster size across 10,000 iterations using Z Monte Carlo simulations as implemented in FreeSurfer (Hayasaka and Nichols, 2003; Hagler et al., 2006) synthesized with a cluster-forming threshold of p < 0.05 (two-sided), yielding clusters fully corrected for multiple comparisons across the surfaces. Cluster-wise corrected p < 0.05 was regarded to be significant.

Results

Delineating cortical developmental trajectories

To accurately characterize longitudinal developmental trajectories, global (Fig. 1) and lobar (Fig. 2) cortical volume, surface area, and thickness measures were visualized as spaghetti plots fitted with GAMM. Total cortical volume decreased across the whole age range in all four samples, with slightly accelerated decreases in the adolescent period compared with late childhood and early adulthood. Total cortical surface area showed nearly linear decreases in all four samples, but appeared overall greater for the two European samples (NCD, BT) and had a somewhat flatter slope for one of the US samples (CPB). Mean cortical thickness showed highly overlapping nonlinear trajectories, with accelerated thinning in adolescence.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Developmental trajectories for global cortical measures. Spaghetti plots of mean cortical thickness, total cortical surface area, and total cortical volume, controlling for sex. The colored lines represent the GAMM fitting while the lighter colored areas correspond to the 95% confidence intervals. Pink, CPB; purple, PIT; blue, NCD; green, BT.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Developmental trajectories for lobar cortical measures. Spaghetti plots of lobar cortical thickness, surface area, and volume, controlling for sex. The colored lines represent the GAMM fitting while the lighter colored areas correspond to the 95% confidence intervals. Pink, CPB; Purple, PIT; blue, NCD; green, BT.

The lobar trajectories were overall similar to the global results, although some regional differences were also evident. For example, cortical volume showed a relatively stable trajectory in late childhood in the frontal lobe, and the accelerated thinning in adolescence was most clearly seen in the frontal lobe, while decelerating trajectories with increasing age were seen for thickness in the parietal and occipital lobes.

Mapping longitudinal cortical change

On average for each sample, and on both the global and lobar level, cortical volume, thickness, and surface area all showed negative change rates, i.e., reductions with increasing age (Table 2; see Table 3 for APCs for all measures for all labels in the cortical parcellation). For the global measures and within all four samples, cortical volume showed the largest decrease (APCs from −1.10 to −1.87; sample mean, −1.43), followed by thickness (APCs from −0.83 to −1.38; sample mean, −1.03), and finally surface area (APCs from −0.36 to −0.71; sample mean, −0.55). Although the ranking of APCs in the different global measures was the same in all four samples, there were also significant sample differences in all three measures (Table 2).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Global and lobar change in cortical volume, surface area, and thickness for each sample

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3.

Regional change in cortical volume, surface area and thickness for each sample

For the lobar measures, cortical volume consistently showed the same ranking of APCs within all four samples, with the largest decrease in the parietal lobe, followed by the frontal, the temporal, and finally the occipital lobe. The parietal lobe also showed the largest decrease in both cortical surface area and thickness in all four samples, and the occipital lobe consistently showed the smallest decrease in cortical thickness. Except for frontal lobe volume and thickness and occipital lobe thickness, there were significant sample differences in the lobar APC values in all measures (Table 2).

Vertex-wise surface maps were then created to visualize the statistical significance (controlling for sex and mean age) and rate of APCs in cortical volume (Fig. 3), surface area (Fig. 4), and thickness (Fig. 5) for each of the samples separately. Corrected significant negative changes were seen for all three measures for extensive portions of the cerebral cortex in all four samples. Some exceptions or sample differences were noted. For volume, significant increases or no effects were seen around the central sulcus and in insular, medial temporal, and medial occipital cortices. For surface area compared with the other two measures, more regions did not show significant APCs, especially gyral regions in the three smaller samples. And finally, for thickness, the rate decrease in most regions was larger for the sample with a narrower age range in adolescence (PIT), than for the other three samples. Note that the scale for rate of APCs varies across the different measures.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Longitudinal change in cortical volume. GLMs were used to test the statistical significance of APC in volume across the brain surface in each sample, with sex and mean age included as covariates. The results were corrected for multiple comparisons using cluster-size inference. Left, Uncorrected p values within the corrected significant clusters. Right, Rates of change.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Longitudinal change in cortical surface area. GLMs were used to test the statistical significance of APC in area across the brain surface in each sample, with sex and mean age included as covariates. The results were corrected for multiple comparisons using cluster-size inference. Left, Uncorrected p values within the corrected significant clusters. Right, Rates of change.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Longitudinal change in cortical thickness. GLMs were used to test the statistical significance of APC in thickness across the brain surface in each sample, with sex and mean age included as covariates. The results were corrected for multiple comparisons using cluster-size inference. Left, Uncorrected p values within the corrected significant clusters. Right, Rates of change.

Testing for inter-related changes in different cortical components

Relationships between global and lobar changes in surface area and volume and in thickness and volume were first tested with partial correlations, controlling for sex and mean age (Table 4). All samples showed large positive associations between thickness APCs and volume APCs (r = 0.72–0.95) for both global and all lobar measures. For global measures, the associations between surface area APC and volume APC varied from small to medium (r = 0.16–0.55), while for the lobar measures medium-to-large positive associations (r = 0.51–0.76) were seen in the frontal lobe in three of the samples (PIT, NCD, BT), in the temporal lobe in two samples (NCD, BT), and in the parietal lobe in one sample (PIT).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 4.

Relationships between changes in surface area and volume and between changes in thickness and volume, for each sample

The results from the per-vertex regression models (controlling for sex and mean age) of surface area APCs and volume APCs and of thickness APCs and volume APCs, respectively, confirmed these general patterns (Fig. 6). Highly significant positive associations between thickness change and volume change were observed across nearly the entire cerebral cortex in all four samples. In comparison, the associations between area change and volume change were not as strong or widespread, and in several regions in two of the samples (NCD, BT) even in the opposite direction (i.e., negative).

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Relationships between change in surface area and thickness and change in volume. Vertex-wise p value maps from GLMs testing the relationships between symmetrized APC in different cortical measures, with sex and mean age included as covariates. The results were corrected for multiple comparisons using cluster-size inference. Uncorrected p values within the corrected significant clusters are shown. Red–yellow reflects a positive relationship, where a relatively large decrease in one measure is associated with a relatively large decrease in the other measure. Blue–cyan reflects a negative relationship, in which a relatively large decrease on one measure is associated with a relatively small decrease or increase on the other measure.

Inter-relationships between APCs in cortical surface area and APCs in cortical thickness were first tested on global and lobar measures with partial correlations, controlling for sex and mean age (Table 5). For the global measures, a significant small positive association (r = 0.22) was seen in the largest sample (BT). For the lobar measures, significant small-to-medium positive associations (r = 0.21–0.52) were seen for the frontal lobe in three samples (PIT, NCD, BT) and for the temporal lobe in two samples (NCD, BT), while significant medium negative associations (r = −0.46 to −0.48) were seen for the occipital lobe in two samples (PIT, BT). Inconsistent with the other samples, the CPB sample showed negative, although nonsignificant, associations for the frontal and temporal lobe measures, possibly related to the younger average baseline age of this sample.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 5.

Global and lobar relationships between changes in cortical surface area and thickness for each sample

To investigate these regional differences in more detail, per-vertex regression models (controlling for sex and mean age) of surface area APCs and thickness APCs were performed (Fig. 7). In all four samples, positive associations were observed in lateral prefrontal and temporal cortices, while negative associations were seen around the central sulcus and in paracentral, insular, and both lateral and medial occipital cortices. Generally, the negative associations were more widespread than the positive. Importantly, the vertex-wise results also revealed a complex topographic pattern of positive and negative associations, with positive relations mainly seen in sulcal regions and negative relations seen in gyral and insular regions. The exact location of some of the relations between surface area APCs and thickness APCs did, however, vary across samples, e.g., three samples (CPB, NCD, BT) showed positive associations in the superior temporal sulcus, while one sample (PIT) showed positive associations in the middle and interior temporal cortices. Also, the extent of both the positive and negative associations appeared to be related to sample size, with the spatially most limited effects seen in the CPB sample and the most widespread effects, especially negative associations, seen in the BT sample. In the three smallest samples (CPB, PIT, NCD), most vertices did not show significant associations.

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

Relationships between change in surface area and change in thickness. Vertex-wise p value maps from GLMs testing the relationships between symmetrized APC in different cortical measures, with sex and mean age included as covariates. The results were corrected for multiple comparisons using cluster-size inference. Uncorrected p values within the corrected significant clusters are shown. Red–yellow reflects a positive relationship, where a relatively large decrease in one measure is associated with a relatively large decrease in the other measure. Blue–cyan reflects a negative relationship, in which a relatively large decrease on one measure is associated with a relatively small decrease or increase on the other measure.

Discussion

The current multisample neuroimaging study aimed to examine the development of the human cerebral cortex across adolescence in four independent longitudinal samples. The results were generally consistent across samples and showed the following: (1) that the cerebral cortex undergoes widespread and regionally variable nonlinear decreases in volume and thickness with increasing age, and comparatively smaller steady decreases in surface area; (2) that the dominant contributor to cortical volume reductions during adolescence is cortical thinning; and (3) that there are complex regional and topological patterns in the relationships between longitudinal changes in surface area and thickness. Together, the results increase confidence in conclusions about structural cortical development and provide novel insight into how changes in distinct cortical components are linked.

In the first 2 years of life, cortical volume, surface area, and thickness all increase over time (Gilmore et al., 2012; Lyall et al., 2015). There are almost no data for the following years of early childhood due to head motion-related MRI artifacts, and there are inconsistencies across studies regarding developmental patterns and trajectories of different structural measures from midchildhood to adulthood (Mills and Tamnes, 2014; Walhovd et al., 2016). Early longitudinal studies suggested continued increases in cortical volume until late childhood or early adolescence (Giedd et al., 1999; Lenroot et al., 2007; Raznahan et al., 2011), while later longitudinal studies (Lebel and Beaulieu, 2011; Aubert-Broche et al., 2013; Tamnes et al., 2013; Mills et al., 2014a, 2016; Wierenga et al., 2014), as well as the current results, indicate that cortical volume is at its highest earlier in childhood and decreases in late childhood and throughout adolescence.

Previous longitudinal studies are particularly conflicting with regard to cortical thickness, with some indicating inverted-U trajectories from childhood to adulthood, with estimates of peak thickness in late childhood (Shaw et al., 2007, 2008; Raznahan et al., 2011), while others show widespread monotonic decreases during childhood and adolescence (Sowell et al., 2004; Shaw et al., 2006; van Soelen et al., 2012; Mutlu et al., 2013; Alexander-Bloch et al., 2014; Mills et al., 2014a; Wierenga et al., 2014; Zielinski et al., 2014; Fjell et al., 2015; Zhou et al., 2015; Ducharme et al., 2016; Vijayakumar et al., 2016). Our results support the conclusion of decreasing cortical thickness with increasing age during late childhood and across adolescence. Fewer longitudinal studies have investigated cortical surface area, but with the exception of one recent paper showing increases in adolescence (Vijayakumar et al., 2016), these (Raznahan et al., 2011; Mills et al., 2014a; Wierenga et al., 2014; Ducharme et al., 2015) and the present results together support the conclusion of childhood increases followed by subtle decreases during adolescence.

After applying similar processing and analytic techniques, the results of the present multisample study showed consistent developmental patterns and trajectories for cortical structure across four longitudinal datasets with varying sample and image-acquisition characteristics. We did not observe any global increase or “peak” for cortical volume, surface area, or thickness from ages 7 to 29 in any of the four samples. The same was true for the lobar measures, except for a small early increase in frontal lobe volume in two of the samples. Our results suggest that previous inconsistencies have not primarily resulted from sample or image-acquisition differences. Rather, we speculate that they stem from the combined effects of differences in image processing, including QC procedures, and/or statistical analyses and curve fitting. All datasets in the present study were processed with an extensively used and well validated open-source software suite (Fischl, 2012) and underwent post-processing QC. Also, curve-fitting was performed with models that avoid some of the weaknesses of global polynomial models.

In both adult and developmental samples, head motion has a negative effect on estimates of cortical volume and thickness, even after excluding low-quality scans (Reuter et al., 2015; Alexander-Bloch et al., 2016). As younger participants generally move more during acquisition, motion is often confounded with age or time point (Satterthwaite et al., 2012). The importance of post-processing QC was demonstrated by Ducharme et al. (2016), who showed that exclusion of scans defined as QC failures had a large impact on identified patterns for cortical thickness development, with a shift toward more complex trajectories when scans of lower quality were included. While we attempted to limit the impact of motion by visually inspecting all reconstructed images and only included scans judged to be of adequate quality, future studies might benefit from further efforts to limit motion during data acquisition, for example by further development of on-line motion-correction procedures and quantitative motion assessment within popular software packages (Reuter et al., 2015; Greene et al., 2016; Tisdall et al., 2016). Additionally, it is likely that differences in statistical analyses may have contributed to the inconsistencies, as we recently showed that whether and how one controls for intracranial volume or total brain size influences developmental models of brain volumes (Mills et al., 2016). In relation to this, future studies are needed to investigate the consistency of reported sex differences in brain structure and development (Lenroot et al., 2007; Mutlu et al., 2013; Vijayakumar et al., 2016), using both raw and corrected measures (Marwha et al., 2017).

In addition to providing detailed descriptions of developmental patterns and trajectories of cortical structure, our results showed consistent and very strong positive relationships between cortical thickness change and volume change across nearly the entire cortex, such that relatively large reductions in thickness were associated with relatively large reductions in volume. In comparison, the relationships between surface-area change and volume change were not as strong or widespread, and for most of the occipital lobe either nonsignificant or negative. Thus, although most of the individual variation in adult cortical volume is due to variation in surface area and not thickness (Im et al., 2008), our results show that the greatest contributor to volume decrease from 7 to 29 years is thinning, as previously also shown to be the case across the adult lifespan (23–87 years; Storsve et al., 2014).

Finally, complex regional and topological patterns in the relationships between surface-area change and thickness change were observed. Across samples, both positive and negative associations were found, with positive relationships mainly seen in sulcal regions in prefrontal and temporal cortices, and negative relationships mainly seen in gyral regions in occipital cortices, paracentral cortex, and insula, and around the central sulcus. Our results mainly showed decreases with increasing age for both surface area and thickness. Thus, positive relationships indicate that relatively large reductions in surface area are associated with relatively large reductions in thickness, while negative relationships indicate that relatively large reductions in surface area are associated with relatively small reductions in thickness, and vice versa. The importance of local topology for cortical development was demonstrated in a recent large cross-sectional study finding that age-related decreases in thickness were most pronounced in the sulci (Vandekar et al., 2015), but no previous study has examined the relationships between longitudinal change in different cortical metrics on a vertex-wise level in children and adolescents (but see Storsve et al., 2014, for a study on adults, and Alemán-Gómezet al., 2013, for lobar analyses in adolescents).

The cellular and molecular changes underlying observed developmental changes in the different dimensions of the cerebral cortex and their inter-relationships remain unknown. They likely include multiple interacting processes that vary in their relative importance across regions and age (Mills and Tamnes, 2014). A recent imaging study suggests that increasing intracortical myelination is a significant driver of cortical changes in adolescence (Whitaker et al., 2016). A hypothesis for the relationships between area change and thickness change in development is that white matter growth in the form of increasing myelination and axon caliber (Yakovlev and Lecours, 1967; Benes, 1989; Benes et al., 1994) causes the cerebral cortex to stretch tangentially to the surface, expanding its area and becoming thinner, as well as improving its ability to differentiate incoming signals (Seldon, 2005, 2007). However, this does not fully explain the surface-area decrease seen in many regions in adolescence. A second hypothesis is that synaptic pruning and dendritic arborization (Bourgeois and Rakic, 1993; Huttenlocher and Dabholkar, 1997; Petanjek et al., 2011) results in decreasing gyrification and flattening of the cortex during adolescence (Raznahan et al., 2011; Alemán-Gómezet al., 2013; Klein et al., 2014) due to release of axonal tension (White et al., 2010). It is likely that a combination of these hypotheses might explain the observed complex patterns in the relationships between surface-area change and thickness change.

Conclusion

The present results from four independent longitudinal datasets showed consistent developmental trajectories and patterns of change in cortical volume, surface area, and thickness across adolescence. Regionally variable nonlinear decreases in cortical volume and thickness, and relatively smaller steady decreases in surface area, were observed from ages 7 to 29. Further, analyses of the inter-relationships between changes in these different dimensions of the cortex revealed tight links between volume reductions and thinning, as well as regional and topological patterns in the relationships between surface-area change and thickness change.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by the Research Council of Norway (C.K.T.); the University of Oslo (C.K.T.); The National Institutes of Health (NIH) Intramural Research Program; the NIH Graduate Partnership Program; NIH Grants R01 DA018910 (R.E.D.), R01 HD053893 (E.R.S.), R01 MH087563 (E.R.S.), F32 HD0708084 (M.M.H.), R01 MH107418 (K.L.M.), and K01 MH1087610 (M.M.H.); The Royal Society (S.J.B.), Jacobs Foundation (S.J.B.), The Wellcome Trust (S.J.B.), and the European Research Council starting grant scheme (ERC-2010-StG-263234 to E.A.C.). The authors thank all participating individuals and families in these longitudinal studies.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Christian K. Tamnes, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, P.O. Box 1094 Blindern, 0317 Oslo, Norway. c.k.tamnes{at}psykologi.uio.no

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Alemán-Gómez Y,
    2. Janssen J,
    3. Schnack H,
    4. Balaban E,
    5. Pina-Camacho L,
    6. Alfaro-Almagro F,
    7. Castro-Fornieles J,
    8. Otero S,
    9. Baeza I,
    10. Moreno D,
    11. Bargalló N,
    12. Parellada M,
    13. Arango C,
    14. Desco M
    (2013) The human cerebral cortex flattens during adolescence. J Neurosci 33:15004–15010. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1459-13.2013 pmid:24048830
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    1. Alexander-Bloch AF,
    2. Reiss PT,
    3. Rapoport J,
    4. McAdams H,
    5. Giedd JN,
    6. Bullmore ET,
    7. Gogtay N
    (2014) Abnormal cortical growth in schizophrenia targets normative modules of synchronized development. Biol Psychiatry 76:438–446. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.02.010 pmid:24690112
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Alexander-Bloch A,
    2. Clasen L,
    3. Stockman M,
    4. Ronan L,
    5. Lalonde F,
    6. Giedd J,
    7. Raznahan A
    (2016) Subtle in-scanner motion biases automated measurement of brain anatomy from in vivo MRI. Hum Brain Mapp 37:2385–2397. doi:10.1002/hbm.23180 pmid:27004471
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Amlien IK,
    2. Fjell AM,
    3. Tamnes CK,
    4. Grydeland H,
    5. Krogsrud SK,
    6. Chaplin TA,
    7. Rosa MG,
    8. Walhovd KB
    (2016) Organizing principles of human cortical development-thickness and area from 4 to 30 years: insights from comparative primate neuroanatomy. Cereb Cortex 26:257–267. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhu214 pmid:25246511
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. ↵
    1. Aubert-Broche B,
    2. Fonov VS,
    3. García-Lorenzo D,
    4. Mouiha A,
    5. Guizard N,
    6. Coupé P,
    7. Eskildsen SF,
    8. Collins DL
    (2013) A new method for structural volume analysis of longitudinal brain MRI data and its application in studying the growth trajectories of anatomical brain structures in childhood. Neuroimage 82:393–402. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.065 pmid:23719155
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Bartsch H,
    2. Thompson WK,
    3. Jernigan TL,
    4. Dale AM
    (2014) A web-portal for interactive data exploration, visualization, and hypothesis testing. Front Neuroinform 8:25. doi:10.3389/fninf.2014.00025 pmid:24723882
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Benes FM
    (1989) Myelination of cortical-hippocampal relays during late adolescence. Schizophr Bull 15:585–593. doi:10.1093/schbul/15.4.585 pmid:2623440
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    1. Benes FM,
    2. Turtle M,
    3. Khan Y,
    4. Farol P
    (1994) Myelination of a key relay zone in the hippocampal formation occurs in the human brain during childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. Arch Gen Psychiatry 51:477–484. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950060041004 pmid:8192550
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Bhardwaj RD,
    2. Curtis MA,
    3. Spalding KL,
    4. Buchholz BA,
    5. Fink D,
    6. Björk-Eriksson T,
    7. Nordborg C,
    8. Gage FH,
    9. Druid H,
    10. Eriksson PS,
    11. Frisén J
    (2006) Neocortical neurogenesis in humans is restricted to development. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103:12564–12568. doi:10.1073/pnas.0605177103 pmid:16901981
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    1. Blakemore SJ
    (2012) Imaging brain development: the adolescent brain. Neuroimage 61:397–406. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.080 pmid:22178817
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Bourgeois JP,
    2. Rakic P
    (1993) Changes of synaptic density in the primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey from fetal to adult stage. J Neurosci 13:2801–2820. pmid:8331373
    OpenUrlAbstract
  12. ↵
    1. Braams BR,
    2. van Duijvenvoorde AC,
    3. Peper JS,
    4. Crone EA
    (2015) Longitudinal changes in adolescent risk-taking: a comprehensive study of neural responses to rewards, pubertal development, and risk-taking behavior. J Neurosci 35:7226–7238. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4764-14.2015 pmid:25948271
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  13. ↵
    1. Brown TT,
    2. Kuperman JM,
    3. Chung Y,
    4. Erhart M,
    5. McCabe C,
    6. Hagler DJ Jr.,
    7. Venkatraman VK,
    8. Akshoomoff N,
    9. Amaral DG,
    10. Bloss CS,
    11. Casey BJ,
    12. Chang L,
    13. Ernst TM,
    14. Frazier JA,
    15. Gruen JR,
    16. Kaufmann WE,
    17. Kenet T,
    18. Kennedy DN,
    19. Murray SS,
    20. Sowell ER, et al
    . (2012) Neuroanatomical assessment of biological maturity. Curr Biol 22:1693–1698. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.07.002 pmid:22902750
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Chen CH,
    2. Fiecas M,
    3. Gutiérrez ED,
    4. Panizzon MS,
    5. Eyler LT,
    6. Vuoksimaa E,
    7. Thompson WK,
    8. Fennema-Notestine C,
    9. Hagler DJ Jr.,
    10. Jernigan TL,
    11. Neale MC,
    12. Franz CE,
    13. Lyons MJ,
    14. Fischl B,
    15. Tsuang MT,
    16. Dale AM,
    17. Kremen WS
    (2013) Genetic topography of brain morphology. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110:17089–17094. doi:10.1073/pnas.1308091110 pmid:24082094
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Chenn A,
    2. Walsh CA
    (2002) Regulation of cerebral cortical size by control of cell cycle exit in neural precursors. Science 297:365–569. doi:10.1126/science.1074192 pmid:12130776
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. ↵
    1. Dale AM,
    2. Sereno MI
    (1993) Improved localization of cortical activity by combining EEG and MEG with MRI cortical surface reconstruction: a linear approach. J Cogn Neurosci 5:162–176. doi:10.1162/jocn.1993.5.2.162 pmid:23972151
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Dale AM,
    2. Fischl B,
    3. Sereno MI
    (1999) Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 9:179–194. doi:10.1006/nimg.1998.0395 pmid:9931268
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Desikan RS,
    2. Ségonne F,
    3. Fischl B,
    4. Quinn BT,
    5. Dickerson BC,
    6. Blacker D,
    7. Buckner RL,
    8. Dale AM,
    9. Maguire RP,
    10. Hyman BT,
    11. Albert MS,
    12. Killiany RJ
    (2006) An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31:968–980. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021 pmid:16530430
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Ducharme S,
    2. Albaugh MD,
    3. Nguyen TV,
    4. Hudziak JJ,
    5. Mateos-Pérez JM,
    6. Labbe A,
    7. Evans AC,
    8. Karama S
    , Brain Development Cooperative Group (2015) Trajectories of cortical surface area and cortical volume maturation in normal brain development. Data Brief 5:929–938. doi:10.1016/j.dib.2015.10.044 pmid:26702424
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Ducharme S,
    2. Albaugh MD,
    3. Nguyen TV,
    4. Hudziak JJ,
    5. Mateos-Pérez JM,
    6. Labbe A,
    7. Evans AC,
    8. Karama S
    , Brain Development Cooperative Group (2016) Trajectories of cortical thickness maturation in normal brain development—the importance of quality control procedures. Neuroimage 125:267–279. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.010 pmid:26463175
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Fischl B
    (2012) FreeSurfer. Neuroimage 62:774–781. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021 pmid:22248573
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Fischl B,
    2. Dale AM
    (2000) Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97:11050–11055. doi:10.1073/pnas.200033797 pmid:10984517
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  23. ↵
    1. Fischl B,
    2. Sereno MI,
    3. Dale AM
    (1999) Cortical surface-based analysis. II: inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage 9:195–207. doi:10.1006/nimg.1998.0396 pmid:9931269
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Fischl B,
    2. Liu A,
    3. Dale AM
    (2001) Automated manifold surgery: constructing geometrically accurate and topologically correct models of the human cerebral cortex. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 20:70–80. doi:10.1109/42.906426 pmid:11293693
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Fischl B,
    2. Salat DH,
    3. Busa E,
    4. Albert M,
    5. Dieterich M,
    6. Haselgrove C,
    7. van der Kouwe A,
    8. Killiany R,
    9. Kennedy D,
    10. Klaveness S,
    11. Montillo A,
    12. Makris N,
    13. Rosen B,
    14. Dale AM
    (2002) Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 33:341–355. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00569-X pmid:11832223
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Fjell AM,
    2. Walhovd KB,
    3. Westlye LT,
    4. Østby Y,
    5. Tamnes CK,
    6. Jernigan TL,
    7. Gamst A,
    8. Dale AM
    (2010) When does brain aging accelerate? Dangers of quadratic fits in cross-sectional studies. Neuroimage 50:1376–1383. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.061 pmid:20109562
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Fjell AM,
    2. Grydeland H,
    3. Krogsrud SK,
    4. Amlien I,
    5. Rohani DA,
    6. Ferschmann L,
    7. Storsve AB,
    8. Tamnes CK,
    9. Sala-Llonch R,
    10. Due-Tønnessen P,
    11. Bjørnerud A,
    12. Sølsnes AE,
    13. Håberg AK,
    14. Skranes J,
    15. Bartsch H,
    16. Chen CH,
    17. Thompson WK,
    18. Panizzon MS,
    19. Kremen WS,
    20. Dale AM, et al
    . (2015) Development and aging of cortical thickness corresponds to genetic organization patterns. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 112:15462–15467. doi:10.1073/pnas.1508831112 pmid:26575625
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    1. Geschwind DH,
    2. Rakic P
    (2013) Cortical evolution: judge the brain by its cover. Neuron 80:633–647. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.045 pmid:24183016
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Giedd JN,
    2. Blumenthal J,
    3. Jeffries NO,
    4. Castellanos FX,
    5. Liu H,
    6. Zijdenbos A,
    7. Paus T,
    8. Evans AC,
    9. Rapoport JL
    (1999) Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat Neurosci 2:861–863. doi:10.1038/13158 pmid:10491603
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Giedd JN,
    2. Raznahan A,
    3. Alexander-Bloch A,
    4. Schmitt E,
    5. Gogtay N,
    6. Rapoport JL
    (2015) Child psychiatry branch of the National Institute of Mental Health longitudinal structural magnetic resonance imaging study of human brain development. Neuropsychopharmacology 40:43–49. doi:10.1038/npp.2014.236 pmid:25195638
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Gilmore JH,
    2. Shi F,
    3. Woolson SL,
    4. Knickmeyer RC,
    5. Short SJ,
    6. Lin W,
    7. Zhu H,
    8. Hamer RM,
    9. Styner M,
    10. Shen D
    (2012) Longitudinal development of cortical and subcortical gray matter from birth to 2 years. Cereb Cortex 22:2478–2485. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr327 pmid:22109543
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    1. Greene DJ,
    2. Black KJ,
    3. Schlaggar BL
    (2016) Considerations for MRI study design and implementation in pediatric and clinical populations. Dev Cogn Neurosci 18:101–112. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2015.12.005 pmid:26754461
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Hagler DJ Jr.,
    2. Saygin AP,
    3. Sereno MI
    (2006) Smoothing and cluster thresholding for cortical surface-based group analysis of fMRI data. Neuroimage 33:1093–1103. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.036 pmid:17011792
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Hayasaka S,
    2. Nichols TE
    (2003) Validating cluster size inference: random field and permutation methods. Neuroimage 20:2343–2356. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.08.003 pmid:14683734
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Herting MM,
    2. Gautam P,
    3. Spielberg JM,
    4. Kan E,
    5. Dahl RE,
    6. Sowell ER
    (2014) The role of testosterone and estradiol in brain volume changes across adolescence: a longitudinal structural MRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 35:5633–5645. doi:10.1002/hbm.22575 pmid:24977395
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Huttenlocher PR,
    2. Dabholkar AS
    (1997) Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. J Comp Neurol 387:167–178. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19971020)387:2<167::AID-CNE1>3.0.CO%3B2-Z pmid:9336221
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Im K,
    2. Lee JM,
    3. Lyttelton O,
    4. Kim SH,
    5. Evans AC,
    6. Kim SI
    (2008) Brain size and cortical structure in the adult human brain. Cereb Cortex 18:2181–2191. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm244 pmid:18234686
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  38. ↵
    1. Jernigan TL,
    2. Baaré WF,
    3. Stiles J,
    4. Madsen KS
    (2011) Postnatal brain development: structural imaging of dynamic neurodevelopmental processes. Prog Brain Res 189:77–92. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-53884-0.00019-1 pmid:21489384
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Klein D,
    2. Rotarska-Jagiela A,
    3. Genc E,
    4. Sritharan S,
    5. Mohr H,
    6. Roux F,
    7. Han CE,
    8. Kaiser M,
    9. Singer W,
    10. Uhlhaas PJ
    (2014) Adolescent brain maturation and cortical folding: evidence for reductions in gyrification. PLoS One 9:e84914. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0084914 pmid:24454765
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Kremen WS,
    2. Fennema-Notestine C,
    3. Eyler LT,
    4. Panizzon MS,
    5. Chen CH,
    6. Franz CE,
    7. Lyons MJ,
    8. Thompson WK,
    9. Dale AM
    (2013) Genetics of brain structure: contributions from the Vietnam Era Twin Study of Aging. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 162B:751–761. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.32162 pmid:24132907
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Lebel C,
    2. Beaulieu C
    (2011) Longitudinal development of human brain wiring continues from childhood into adulthood. J Neurosci 31:10937–10947. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5302-10.2011 pmid:21795544
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  42. ↵
    1. Lenroot RK,
    2. Gogtay N,
    3. Greenstein DK,
    4. Wells EM,
    5. Wallace GL,
    6. Clasen LS,
    7. Blumenthal JD,
    8. Lerch J,
    9. Zijdenbos AP,
    10. Evans AC,
    11. Thompson PM,
    12. Giedd JN
    (2007) Sexual dimorphism of brain developmental trajectories during childhood and adolescence. Neuroimage 36:1065–1073. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.053 pmid:17513132
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Lyall AE,
    2. Shi F,
    3. Geng X,
    4. Woolson S,
    5. Li G,
    6. Wang L,
    7. Hamer RM,
    8. Shen D,
    9. Gilmore JH
    (2015) Dynamic development of regional cortical thickness and surface area in early childhood. Cereb Cortex 25:2204–2212. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhu027 pmid:24591525
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  44. ↵
    1. Marwha D,
    2. Halari M,
    3. Eliot L
    (2017) Meta-analysis reveals a lack of sexual dimorphism in human amygdala volume. Neuroimage 147:282–294. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.021 pmid:27956206
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Mills KL,
    2. Tamnes CK
    (2014) Methods and considerations for longitudinal structural brain imaging analysis across development. Dev Cogn Neurosci 9:172–190. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2014.04.004 pmid:24879112
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Mills KL,
    2. Lalonde F,
    3. Clasen LS,
    4. Giedd JN,
    5. Blakemore SJ
    (2014a) Developmental changes in the structure of the social brain in late childhood and adolescence. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 9:123–131. doi:10.1093/scan/nss113 pmid:23051898
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  47. ↵
    1. Mills KL,
    2. Goddings AL,
    3. Clasen LS,
    4. Giedd JN,
    5. Blakemore SJ
    (2014b) The developmental mismatch in structural brain maturation during adolescence. Dev Neurosci 36:147–160. doi:10.1159/000362328 pmid:24993606
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Mills KL,
    2. Goddings AL,
    3. Herting MM,
    4. Meuwese R,
    5. Blakemore SJ,
    6. Crone EA,
    7. Dahl RE,
    8. Güroğlu B,
    9. Raznahan A,
    10. Sowell ER,
    11. Tamnes CK
    (2016) Structural brain development between childhood and adulthood: convergence across four longitudinal samples. Neuroimage 141:273–281. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.07.044 pmid:27453157
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Mutlu AK,
    2. Schneider M,
    3. Debbané M,
    4. Badoud D,
    5. Eliez S,
    6. Schaer M
    (2013) Sex differences in thickness, and folding developments throughout the cortex. Neuroimage 82:200–207. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.076 pmid:23721724
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Petanjek Z,
    2. Judaš M,
    3. Šimic G,
    4. Rasin MR,
    5. Uylings HB,
    6. Rakic P,
    7. Kostovic I
    (2011) Extraordinary neoteny of synaptic spines in the human prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:13281–13286. doi:10.1073/pnas.1105108108 pmid:21788513
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  51. ↵
    1. Rakic P
    (1988) Specification of cerebral cortical areas. Science 241:170–176. doi:10.1126/science.3291116 pmid:3291116
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  52. ↵
    1. Raznahan A,
    2. Shaw P,
    3. Lalonde F,
    4. Stockman M,
    5. Wallace GL,
    6. Greenstein D,
    7. Clasen L,
    8. Gogtay N,
    9. Giedd JN
    (2011) How does your cortex grow? J Neurosci 31:7174–7177. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0054-11.2011 pmid:21562281
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  53. ↵
    1. Reuter M,
    2. Rosas HD,
    3. Fischl B
    (2010) Highly accurate inverse consistent registration: a robust approach. Neuroimage 53:1181–1196. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.020 pmid:20637289
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Reuter M,
    2. Schmansky NJ,
    3. Rosas HD,
    4. Fischl B
    (2012) Within-subject template estimation for unbiased longitudinal image analysis. Neuroimage 61:1402–1418. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.084 pmid:22430496
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Reuter M,
    2. Tisdall MD,
    3. Qureshi A,
    4. Buckner RL,
    5. van der Kouwe AJ,
    6. Fischl B
    (2015) Head motion during MRI acquisition reduces gray matter volume and thickness estimates. Neuroimage 107:107–115. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.006 pmid:25498430
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Satterthwaite TD,
    2. Wolf DH,
    3. Loughead J,
    4. Ruparel K,
    5. Elliott MA,
    6. Hakonarson H,
    7. Gur RC,
    8. Gur RE
    (2012) Impact of in-scanner head motion on multiple measures of functional connectivity: relevance for studies of neurodevelopment in youth. Neuroimage 60:623–632. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.063 pmid:22233733
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. Ségonne F,
    2. Dale AM,
    3. Busa E,
    4. Glessner M,
    5. Salat D,
    6. Hahn HK,
    7. Fischl B
    (2004) A hybrid approach to the skull stripping problem in MRI. Neuroimage 22:1060–1075. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.032 pmid:15219578
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Ségonne F,
    2. Pacheco J,
    3. Fischl B
    (2007) Geometrically accurate topology-correction of cortical surfaces using nonseparating loops. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 26:518–529. doi:10.1109/TMI.2006.887364 pmid:17427739
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Seldon HL
    (2005) Does brain white matter growth expand the cortex like a balloon? Hypothesis and consequences. Laterality 10:81–95. pmid:15841825
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Seldon HL
    (2007) Extended neocortical maturation time encompasses speciation, fatty acid and lateralization theories of the evolution of schizophrenia and creativity. Med Hypotheses 69:1085–1089. doi:10.1016/j.mehy.2007.03.001 pmid:17449192
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Shaw P,
    2. Lerch J,
    3. Greenstein D,
    4. Sharp W,
    5. Clasen L,
    6. Evans A,
    7. Giedd J,
    8. Castellanos FX,
    9. Rapoport J
    (2006) Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and clinical outcome in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 63:540–549. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.63.5.540 pmid:16651511
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    1. Shaw P,
    2. Eckstrand K,
    3. Sharp W,
    4. Blumenthal J,
    5. Lerch JP,
    6. Greenstein D,
    7. Clasen L,
    8. Evans A,
    9. Giedd J,
    10. Rapoport JL
    (2007) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104:19649–19654. doi:10.1073/pnas.0707741104 pmid:18024590
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  63. ↵
    1. Shaw P,
    2. Kabani NJ,
    3. Lerch JP,
    4. Eckstrand K,
    5. Lenroot R,
    6. Gogtay N,
    7. Greenstein D,
    8. Clasen L,
    9. Evans A,
    10. Rapoport JL,
    11. Giedd JN,
    12. Wise SP
    (2008) Neurodevelopmental trajectories of the human cerebral cortex. J Neurosci 28:3586–3594. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5309-07.2008 pmid:18385317
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  64. ↵
    1. Sled JG,
    2. Zijdenbos AP,
    3. Evans AC
    (1998) A nonparametric method for automatic correction of intensity nonuniformity in MRI data. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 17:87–97. doi:10.1109/42.668698 pmid:9617910
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Sowell ER,
    2. Thompson PM,
    3. Leonard CM,
    4. Welcome SE,
    5. Kan E,
    6. Toga AW
    (2004) Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. J Neurosci 24:8223–8231. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1798-04.2004 pmid:15385605
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  66. ↵
    1. Storsve AB,
    2. Fjell AM,
    3. Tamnes CK,
    4. Westlye LT,
    5. Overbye K,
    6. Aasland HW,
    7. Walhovd KB
    (2014) Differential longitudinal changes in cortical thickness, surface area and volume across the adult life span: regions of accelerating and decelerating change. J Neurosci 34:8488–8498. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0391-14.2014 pmid:24948804
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  67. ↵
    1. Sullivan EV,
    2. Pfefferbaum A,
    3. Rohlfing T,
    4. Baker FC,
    5. Padilla ML,
    6. Colrain IM
    (2011) Developmental change in regional brain structure over 7 months in early adolescence: comparison of approaches for longitudinal atlas-based parcellation. Neuroimage 57:214–224. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.003 pmid:21511039
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. Tamnes CK,
    2. Walhovd KB,
    3. Dale AM,
    4. Østby Y,
    5. Grydeland H,
    6. Richardson G,
    7. Westlye LT,
    8. Roddey JC,
    9. Hagler DJ Jr.,
    10. Due-Tønnessen P,
    11. Holland D,
    12. Fjell AM
    , Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (2013) Brain development and aging: overlapping and unique patterns of change. Neuroimage 68:63–74. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.039 pmid:23246860
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    1. Tisdall MD,
    2. Reuter M,
    3. Qureshi A,
    4. Buckner RL,
    5. Fischl B,
    6. van der Kouwe AJ
    (2016) Prospective motion correction with volumetric navigators (vNavs) reduces the bias and variance in brain morphometry induced by subject motion. Neuroimage 127:11–22. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.054 pmid:26654788
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    1. van Soelen IL,
    2. Brouwer RM,
    3. van Baal GC,
    4. Schnack HG,
    5. Peper JS,
    6. Collins DL,
    7. Evans AC,
    8. Kahn RS,
    9. Boomsma DI,
    10. Hulshoff Pol HE
    (2012) Genetic influences on thinning of the cerebral cortex during development. Neuroimage 59:3871–3880. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.044 pmid:22155028
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Vandekar SN,
    2. Shinohara RT,
    3. Raznahan A,
    4. Roalf DR,
    5. Ross M,
    6. DeLeo N,
    7. Ruparel K,
    8. Verma R,
    9. Wolf DH,
    10. Gur RC,
    11. Gur RE,
    12. Satterthwaite TD
    (2015) Topologically dissociable patterns of development of the human cerebral cortex. J Neurosci 35:599–609. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3628-14.2015 pmid:25589754
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  72. ↵
    1. Vidal-Piñeiro D,
    2. Walhovd KB,
    3. Storsve AB,
    4. Grydeland H,
    5. Rohani DA,
    6. Fjell AM
    (2016) Accelerated longitudinal gray/white matter contrast decline in aging in lightly myelinated cortical regions. Hum Brain Mapp 37:3669–3684. doi:10.1002/hbm.23267 pmid:27228371
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    1. Vijayakumar N,
    2. Allen NB,
    3. Youssef G,
    4. Dennison M,
    5. Yücel M,
    6. Simmons JG,
    7. Whittle S
    (2016) Brain development during adolescence: a mixed-longitudinal investigation of cortical thickness, surface area, and volume. Hum Brain Mapp 37:2027–2038. doi:10.1002/hbm.23154 pmid:26946457
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Walhovd KB,
    2. Fjell AM,
    3. Giedd J,
    4. Dale AM,
    5. Brown TT
    (2017) Through thick and thin: a need to reconcile contradictory results on trajectories in human cortical development. Cereb Cortex 1:11. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhv301
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  74. ↵
    1. Whitaker KJ,
    2. Vértes PE,
    3. Romero-Garcia R,
    4. Váša F,
    5. Moutoussis M,
    6. Prabhu G,
    7. Weiskopf N,
    8. Callaghan MF,
    9. Wagstyl K,
    10. Rittman T,
    11. Tait R,
    12. Ooi C,
    13. Suckling J,
    14. Inkster B,
    15. Fonagy P,
    16. Dolan RJ,
    17. Jones PB,
    18. Goodyer IM
    , the NSPN Consortium, Bullmore ET (2016) Adolescence is associated with genomically patterned consolidation of the hubs of the human brain connectome. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 113:9105–9110. doi:10.1073/pnas.1601745113 pmid:27457931
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  75. ↵
    1. White T,
    2. Su S,
    3. Schmidt M,
    4. Kao CY,
    5. Sapiro G
    (2010) The development of gyrification in childhood and adolescence. Brain Cogn 72:36–45. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.009 pmid:19942335
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  76. ↵
    1. Wierenga LM,
    2. Langen M,
    3. Oranje B,
    4. Durston S
    (2014) Unique developmental trajectories of cortical thickness and surface area. Neuroimage 87:120–126. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.010 pmid:24246495
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    1. Wood SN
    (2006) Generalized additive models: an introduction with R. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman and Hall/CRC.
  78. ↵
    1. Yakovlev PA,
    2. Lecours IR
    (1967) The myelogenetic cucles of regional maturation of the brain. In: Regional development of the brain in early life (Minkowski A, ed), pp 3–70. Oxford: Blackwell.
  79. ↵
    1. Zhou D,
    2. Lebel C,
    3. Treit S,
    4. Evans A,
    5. Beaulieu C
    (2015) Accelerated longitudinal cortical thinning in adolescence. Neuroimage 104:138–145. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.005 pmid:25312772
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    1. Zielinski BA,
    2. Prigge MB,
    3. Nielsen JA,
    4. Froehlich AL,
    5. Abildskov TJ,
    6. Anderson JS,
    7. Fletcher PT,
    8. Zygmunt KM,
    9. Travers BG,
    10. Lange N,
    11. Alexander AL,
    12. Bigler ED,
    13. Lainhart JE
    (2014) Longitudinal changes in cortical thickness in autism and typical development. Brain 137:1799–1812. doi:10.1093/brain/awu083 pmid:24755274
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 37 (12)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 37, Issue 12
22 Mar 2017
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Advertising (PDF)
  • Ed Board (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Development of the Cerebral Cortex across Adolescence: A Multisample Study of Inter-Related Longitudinal Changes in Cortical Volume, Surface Area, and Thickness
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Development of the Cerebral Cortex across Adolescence: A Multisample Study of Inter-Related Longitudinal Changes in Cortical Volume, Surface Area, and Thickness
Christian K. Tamnes, Megan M. Herting, Anne-Lise Goddings, Rosa Meuwese, Sarah-Jayne Blakemore, Ronald E. Dahl, Berna Güroğlu, Armin Raznahan, Elizabeth R. Sowell, Eveline A. Crone, Kathryn L. Mills
Journal of Neuroscience 22 March 2017, 37 (12) 3402-3412; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3302-16.2017

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Development of the Cerebral Cortex across Adolescence: A Multisample Study of Inter-Related Longitudinal Changes in Cortical Volume, Surface Area, and Thickness
Christian K. Tamnes, Megan M. Herting, Anne-Lise Goddings, Rosa Meuwese, Sarah-Jayne Blakemore, Ronald E. Dahl, Berna Güroğlu, Armin Raznahan, Elizabeth R. Sowell, Eveline A. Crone, Kathryn L. Mills
Journal of Neuroscience 22 March 2017, 37 (12) 3402-3412; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3302-16.2017
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • brain development
  • gray matter
  • morphometry
  • MRI
  • replication

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Mechanisms of Dominant Electrophysiological Features of Four Subtypes of Layer 1 Interneurons
  • Neural index of reinforcement learning predicts improved stimulus-response retention under high working memory load
  • Activity-dependent Nr4a2 induction modulates synaptic expression of AMPA receptors and plasticity via a Ca2+/CRTC1/CREB pathway
Show more Research Articles

Development/Plasticity/Repair

  • Cbln1 Directs Axon Targeting by Corticospinal Neurons Specifically toward Thoraco-Lumbar Spinal Cord
  • Loss of Motor Cortical Inputs to the Red Nucleus after CNS Disorders in Nonhuman Primates
  • Astrocytes Transplanted during Early Postnatal Development Integrate, Mature, and Survive Long Term in Mouse Cortex
Show more Development/Plasticity/Repair
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.